1
|
Li Q, Li M, Wu C. Methods and measures of source monitoring in children: A scoping review. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2024. [PMID: 39315748 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12523] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2024] [Accepted: 09/04/2024] [Indexed: 09/25/2024]
Abstract
Source monitoring (SM) refers to attributing sources of information. There are various methods for measuring SM in children. We searched the PubMed, PsycINFO, Embase, Web of Science and Cochrane Library databases from their inception to February 24, 2023, to summarize the methods and measures of SM in children, identifying 141 studies. The procedure for the SM tasks encompassed encoding, interval and testing. The encoding tasks were classified into ground-based activities (n = 67), computer-based experiments (n = 42) and mixed methods (n = 34). The testing approaches were categorized into old/new recognition and source discrimination, n-alternative forced-choice, yes/no questions and recall/direct questions. Among 10 commonly used indicators, source-correct, source-incorrect, discrimination score and source accuracy were recommended to present the SM function. We also discussed the advantages and disadvantages of different SM methods in children, providing a reference for researchers to design and select SM measurements that meet their research objectives.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Qiuhong Li
- School of Nursing, Peking University, Haidian District, Beijing, China
| | - Mengyuan Li
- School of Nursing, Peking University, Haidian District, Beijing, China
| | - Chao Wu
- School of Nursing, Peking University, Haidian District, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Bögge L, Colás-Blanco I, Ferragu J, Gaston-Bellegarde A, La Corte V, Piolino P. Autonomic and cognitive control in memory: Investigating the psychophysiological link using heart rate variability biofeedback. Psychophysiology 2024; 61:e14588. [PMID: 38769698 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14588] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2023] [Revised: 02/03/2024] [Accepted: 03/30/2024] [Indexed: 05/22/2024]
Abstract
Vagally mediated heart rate variability (vmHRV) at resting state has been associated to cognitive functions dependent on cognitive control, such as memory. However, little is known about the phasic interaction between cognitive and autonomic control. In a pre-registered within-between-subject designed experiment, the potential of vmHRV biofeedback to simultaneously stimulate vmHRV during memory processing and cognitive control over long-term memory was tested, along with investigating psychophysiological association. 71 young healthy adults completed (twice) a false memory task in virtual reality. Immediately before memory encoding and retrieval, participants practiced either vmHRV biofeedback or a control breathing exercise. Cognitive control over long-term memory was assessed as the confidence toward false memories and the capability to discriminate them from true memories. Resting-state vmHRV before each test and phasic vmHRV during memory encoding and retrieval were measured as the root mean square differences (RMSSD) in the heart period. vmHRV biofeedback had neither an immediate effect on measures of cognitive control over long-term memory nor on phasic RMSSD. Moreover, neither resting-state nor phasic vmHRV correlated to the cognitive scores. Consequently, the utility of HRV biofeedback as a psychophysiological stimulation tool and a link between vmHRV and cognitive control over long-term memory could not be verified. Exploratory analyses revealed that baseline shift in parasympathetic activity confounded the psychophysiological association. Future directions are provided that could shed light on the relationship between cognition and vmHRV.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lukas Bögge
- Institute of Psychology, Memory, Brain & Cognition Laboratory, Université Paris Cité, Paris, France
| | - Itsaso Colás-Blanco
- Institute of Psychology, Memory, Brain & Cognition Laboratory, Université Paris Cité, Paris, France
| | - Juliette Ferragu
- Institute of Psychology, Memory, Brain & Cognition Laboratory, Université Paris Cité, Paris, France
| | | | - Valentina La Corte
- Institute of Psychology, Memory, Brain & Cognition Laboratory, Université Paris Cité, Paris, France
- Institut Universitaire de France (IUF), Paris, France
| | - Pascale Piolino
- Institute of Psychology, Memory, Brain & Cognition Laboratory, Université Paris Cité, Paris, France
- Institut Universitaire de France (IUF), Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Bruce M, Savla J, Bell MA. From terrible twos to sassy sixes: The development of vocabulary and executive functioning across early childhood. Dev Sci 2023; 26:e13396. [PMID: 37042169 PMCID: PMC10567994 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13396] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2022] [Revised: 12/05/2022] [Accepted: 03/13/2023] [Indexed: 04/13/2023]
Abstract
Across the early childhood period of development, young children exhibit considerable growth in their executive functioning (EF) and vocabulary abilities. Understanding the developmental trajectory of these seemingly interrelated processes is important as both early vocabulary and EF have been shown to predict critical academic and socio-emotional outcomes later in childhood. Although previous research suggests that EF and vocabulary are correlated in early childhood, much of the existing longitudinal research has focused on unidirectional relations among preschool child samples. The current large-scale study, therefore, sought to examine whether children's vocabulary and EF abilities are bidirectionally related over time across four measurement waves in early childhood (i.e., at ages 2, 3, 4, and 6). At each timepoint, children's vocabulary skills were positively correlated with their concurrent EF abilities. After controlling for child sex and maternal education status, the best-fitting, cross-lagged panel model was a unidirectional model whereby children's early vocabulary scores predicted their later EF performance at each timepoint. Although age 2 EF significantly predicted age 3 vocabulary size, this association was no longer significant after accounting for maternal education status. Our results illustrate that vocabulary size plays an important role in predicting children's later EF performance across various timepoints in early childhood, even after controlling for children's initial EF scores. These findings have important implications for intervention research as fostering early vocabulary acquisition may serve as a possible avenue for improving EF outcomes in young children. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Children's vocabulary size is positively correlated with their concurrent executive functioning skill at ages 2, 3, 4, and 6 Young children's early vocabulary scores predict their later EF performance across measurement waves, even after controlling for initial EF skill There is stability in children's relative vocabulary size and executive functioning performance over time in early childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Madeleine Bruce
- Virginia Tech, Department of Psychology, Blacksburg, Virginia, USA
| | - Jyoti Savla
- Virginia Tech, Department of Human Development & Family Science, Blacksburg, Virginia, USA
| | - Martha Ann Bell
- Virginia Tech, Department of Psychology, Blacksburg, Virginia, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Meza-Cervera T, Kim-Spoon J, Bell MA. Adolescent Depressive Symptoms: The Role of Late Childhood Frontal EEG Asymmetry, Executive Function, and Adolescent Cognitive Reappraisal. Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol 2023; 51:193-207. [PMID: 36251139 PMCID: PMC9574846 DOI: 10.1007/s10802-022-00983-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/05/2022] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
Cognitive reappraisal is adaptive for decreasing symptoms of depression; however, a gap in the research is understanding the childhood processes that contribute to cognitive reappraisal in adolescence. This study examined executive function and frontal electroencephalogram (EEG) asymmetry during late childhood as predictors of adolescent cognitive reappraisal and depressive symptoms. Data were from 123 participants in late childhood (age 10) and adolescence (age 14.5). A moderated mediation model was fit to the data to examine frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator in the relation between late childhood inhibitory control and adolescent cognitive reappraisal as well as adolescent cognitive reappraisal and adolescent depressive symptoms. Results indicated lower inhibitory control was associated with lower cognitive reappraisal when children had right frontal EEG asymmetry. Lower cognitive reappraisal in turn was associated with higher depressive symptoms for children with right frontal EEG asymmetry. Working memory and cognitive flexibility were also examined but were not significant indicators. Results suggest the potential for targeting inhibitory control and cognitive reappraisal to diminish depressive symptoms particularly among adolescents with right frontal EEG asymmetry.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tatiana Meza-Cervera
- Department of Psychology, Virginia Tech, 890 Drillfield Dr., Blacksburg, VA, 24061, USA.
| | - Jungmeen Kim-Spoon
- Department of Psychology, Virginia Tech, 890 Drillfield Dr., Blacksburg, VA, 24061, USA
| | - Martha Ann Bell
- Department of Psychology, Virginia Tech, 890 Drillfield Dr., Blacksburg, VA, 24061, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Broomell AP, Bell MA. Longitudinal development of executive function from infancy to late childhood. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101229] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
6
|
Gordo C, Moreno-Ríos S, Blank H. Auditory hindsight bias in school-age children. J Exp Child Psychol 2022; 217:105346. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105346] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2021] [Revised: 12/01/2021] [Accepted: 12/04/2021] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
7
|
Nicoladis E, Svob C, Smithson L. What is the source of preschool children's memories of events from their own lives? APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.3915] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
8
|
Blankenship TL, Calkins SD, Bell MA. The Role of Executive Functions in Item Recognition and Temporal Order Memory Development. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2022; 23:135-147. [PMID: 35264909 PMCID: PMC8901123 DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2021.1964504] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/03/2023]
Abstract
Item recognition and temporal order memory follow different developmental trajectories during middle childhood, with item recognition performance stabilizing and temporal order memory performance continuing to improve. We investigated the potential unique role of individual executive functions on item recognition and temporal order memory during this critical development period. Our results replicate and expand on previous findings, suggesting that executive functions, specifically inhibitory control and working memory, may be more crucial for successful temporal order memory than for item recognition during middle childhood.
Collapse
|
9
|
Dolgoarshinnaia A, Martin-Luengo B. False Memories in Native and Foreign Languages. Front Psychol 2021; 12:716336. [PMID: 34650480 PMCID: PMC8505984 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.716336] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2021] [Accepted: 08/26/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Human memory is prone to memory errors and distortion. Evidence from studies on cognitive functions in bilinguals indicates that they might be prone to different types of memory errors compared to monolinguals; however, the effect of language in false memories is still understudied. Source monitoring processes required for proper memory functioning, presumably, rely on inhibitory control, which is also heavily utilized by bilinguals. Moreover, it is suggested that thinking in a second language leads to more systematic and deliberate reasoning. All these results lead to expect that bilinguals are more analytical when processing information in their second language overcoming some memory errors depending on the language of information. To test this hypothesis, we run a classical misinformation experiment with an explicit source monitoring task with a sample of Russian–English bilinguals. The language of the misinformation presentation did not affect the degree of the misinformation effect between the Russian and English languages. Source monitoring demonstrated an overall higher accuracy for attributions to the English source over the Russian source. Furthermore, analysis on incorrect source attributions showed that when participants misattributed the sources of false information (English or Russian narrative), they favored the Russian source over the not presented condition. Taken together, these results imply that high proficiency in the second language does not affect misinformation and that information processing and memory monitoring in bilinguals can differ depending on the language of the information, which seems to lead to some memory errors and not others.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aleksandra Dolgoarshinnaia
- Centre for Cognition and Decision Making, Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
| | - Beatriz Martin-Luengo
- Centre for Cognition and Decision Making, Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Forsberg A, Blume CL, Cowan N. The development of metacognitive accuracy in working memory across childhood. Dev Psychol 2021; 57:1297-1317. [PMID: 34591573 PMCID: PMC8496917 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001213] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Growth in working memory capacity, the number of items kept active in mind, is thought to be an important aspect of childhood cognitive development. Here, we focused on participants' awareness of the contents of their working memory, or meta-working memory, which seems important because people can put cognitive abilities to best use only if they are aware of their limitations. In two experiments on the development of meta-working memory in children between 6 and 13 years old and adults, participants were to remember arrays of colored squares and to indicate if a probe item was in the array. On many trials, before the probe recognition test, they reported a metajudgment, how many items they thought they remembered. We compared meta-working memory judgments to actual performance and looked for associations between these measures on individual and trial-by-trial levels. Despite much lower working memory capacity in younger children there was little change in meta-working memory judgments across age groups. Consequently, younger participants were much less realistic in their metajudgments concerning their working memory capability. Higher cognitive capacity was associated with more accurate meta-working memory judgments within an age group. Trial-by-trial tuning of metajudgments was evident only in young adults and then only for small array set sizes. In sum, meta-working memory ability is a sophisticated skill that develops with age and may be an integral aspect of the development of working memory across the school years. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alicia Forsberg
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri
| | | | - Nelson Cowan
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
McLoughlin N, Finiasz Z, Sobel DM, Corriveau KH. Children's developing capacity to calibrate the verbal testimony of others with observed evidence when inferring causal relations. J Exp Child Psychol 2021; 210:105183. [PMID: 34087685 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105183] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2020] [Revised: 04/17/2021] [Accepted: 04/23/2021] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Across two studies (N = 120), we investigated the development of children's ability to calibrate the certainty of verbal testimony with observable data that varied in the degree of predictive causal accuracy. In Study 1, 4- and 5-year-olds heard a certain explanation or an uncertain explanation about deterministic causal relations. The 5-year-olds made more accurate causal inferences when the informant provided a certain and more calibrated explanation. In Study 2, children heard similar explanations about probabilistic relations, making the uncertain informant more calibrated. The 5-year-olds were more likely to infer the correct causal relations when the informant was uncertain, but only when the explanation was attuned to the stochasticity of the individual causal events (or outcomes that sometimes occur). These findings imply that the capacity to integrate, and make efficient inferences, from distinct sources of knowledge emerges during the preschool years.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Niamh McLoughlin
- School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury CT2 7NP, UK; Wheelock College of Education & Human Development, Boston University, Boston, MA 02215, USA.
| | - Zoe Finiasz
- Department of Human Development, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA
| | - David M Sobel
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
| | - Kathleen H Corriveau
- Wheelock College of Education & Human Development, Boston University, Boston, MA 02215, USA
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Roberts KP, Wood KR, Wylie BE. Children's ability to edit their memories when learning about the environment from credible and noncredible websites. COGNITIVE RESEARCH-PRINCIPLES AND IMPLICATIONS 2021; 6:42. [PMID: 34050824 PMCID: PMC8164076 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-021-00305-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2020] [Accepted: 05/07/2021] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
One of the many sources of information easily available to children is the internet and the millions of websites providing accurate, and sometimes inaccurate, information. In the current investigation, we examined children’s ability to use credibility information about websites when learning about environmental sustainability. In two studies, children studied two different websites and were tested on what they had learned a week later using a multiple-choice test containing both website items and new distracters. Children were given either no information about the websites or were told that one of the websites (the noncredible website) contained errors and they should not use any information from that website to answer the test. In both studies, children aged 7- to 9-years reported information from the noncredible website even when instructed not to, whereas the 10- to 12-year-olds used the credibility warning to ‘edit out’ information that they had learned from the noncredible website. In Study 2, there was an indication that the older children spontaneously assessed the credibility of the website if credibility markers were made explicit. A plausible explanation is that, although children remembered information from the websites, they needed explicit instruction to bind the website content with the relevant source (the individual websites). The results have implications for children’s learning in an open-access, digital age where information comes from many sources, credible and noncredible. Education in credibility evaluation may enable children to be critical consumers of information thereby resisting misinformation provided through public sources.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kim P Roberts
- Wilfrid Laurier University, 75 University Ave W, Waterloo, ON, N2L 3C5, Canada.
| | - Katherine R Wood
- Wilfrid Laurier University, 75 University Ave W, Waterloo, ON, N2L 3C5, Canada.,Carleton University, 1125 Colonel By Dr, Ottawa, ON, K1S 5B6, Canada
| | - Breanne E Wylie
- Wilfrid Laurier University, 75 University Ave W, Waterloo, ON, N2L 3C5, Canada.,Brock University, 1812 Sir Isaac Brock Way, St. Catharines, ON, L2S 3A1, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Blankenship TL, Kibbe MM. Examining the limits of Memory-Guided Planning in 3- and 4-year olds. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2020; 52. [PMID: 32863569 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.100820] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Stored memories may be drawn upon when accomplishing goals. In two experiments, we investigated limits on the ability to use episodic memories to support planning in 3- and 4-year-old children. We designed a new memory-guided planning task that required children to both retrieve memories and apply those memories to accomplish multiple, nested goals. We manipulated the difficulty of the task by varying the number of steps required to achieve the goals, and examined the impact of this manipulation on both memory retrieval and planning. We found that, overall, 4-year-olds outperformed 3-year-olds, but as task difficulty increased, all children made more errors. Analysis of these errors suggested that retrieval and planning processes might impose separate limits on memory-guided planning in early childhood, but that these limits may ease across early childhood.
Collapse
|
14
|
Bilingual witnesses are more susceptible to the misinformation effect in their less proficient language. CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY 2020. [DOI: 10.1007/s12144-018-9787-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
15
|
Helm AF, McCormick SA, Deater-Deckard K, Smith CL, Calkins SD, Bell MA. Parenting and Children's Executive Function Stability Across the Transition to School. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2019; 29. [PMID: 32617081 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2171] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Abstract
When children transition to school between the ages of 4 and 6 years, they must learn to control their attention and behavior to be successful. Concurrently, executive function (EF) is an important skill undergoing significant development in childhood. To understand changes occurring during this period, we examined the role of parenting in the development of children's EF from 4 to 6 years old. Participants were mother and child dyads (N = 151). Children completed cognitive tasks to assess overall EF at age 4 and age 6. At both time points, mothers and children completed interaction tasks which were videotaped and coded to assess various parenting dimensions. Results indicated that children with high EF at age 4 were more likely to have high EF at age 6. In addition, results suggested that higher levels of positive parenting across the transition to school promote stability of individual differences in EF.
Collapse
|
16
|
Blankenship TL, Slough MA, Calkins SD, Deater-Deckard K, Kim-Spoon J, Bell MA. Attention and executive functioning in infancy: Links to childhood executive function and reading achievement. Dev Sci 2019; 22:e12824. [PMID: 30828908 PMCID: PMC6722030 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12824] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2018] [Revised: 02/26/2019] [Accepted: 02/27/2019] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
This study provides the first analyses connecting individual differences in infant attention to reading achievement through the development of executive functioning (EF) in infancy and early childhood. Five-month-old infants observed a video, and peak look duration and shift rate were video coded and assessed. At 10 months, as well as 3, 4, and 6 years, children completed age-appropriate EF tasks (A-not-B task, hand game, forward digit span, backwards digit span, and number Stroop). Children also completed a standardized reading assessment and a measure of verbal intelligence (IQ) at age 6. Path analyses on 157 participants showed that infant attention had a direct statistical predictive effect on EF at 10 months, with EF showing a continuous pattern of development from 10 months to 6 years. EF and verbal IQ at 6 years had a direct effect on reading achievement. Furthermore, EF at all time points mediated the relation between 5-month attention and reading achievement. These findings may inform reading interventions by suggesting earlier intervention time points and specific cognitive processes (i.e. 5-month attention).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tashauna L Blankenship
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts
| | | | - Susan D Calkins
- Department of Human Development and Family Studies, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina
| | - Kirby Deater-Deckard
- Department of Psychological and Brain Science, University of Massachusetts at Amherst, Amherst, Massachusetts
| | | | - Martha Ann Bell
- Department of Psychology, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, Virginia
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Francis WS, Strobach EN, Penalver RM, Martínez M, Gurrola BV, Soltero A. Word-context associations in episodic memory are learned at the conceptual level: Word frequency, bilingual proficiency, and bilingual status effects on source memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2018; 45:1852-1871. [PMID: 30570325 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000678] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
[Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported in Vol 45(10) of Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition (see record 2019-53018-001). In the article, a formula error in the scoring spreadsheet for bilingual participants in Experiment 3 systematically inflated their accuracy scores. Therefore, statistical information for analyses involving the bilingual sample, and bilingual portions of Table 7 and Figure 3 have been corrected. All versions of this article have been corrected.] Three source-memory experiments were conducted with Spanish-English bilinguals and monolingual English speakers matched on age, education, nonverbal cognitive ability and socioeconomic status. Bilingual language proficiency and dominance were assessed using standardized objective measures. In Experiment 1, source was manipulated visuo-spatially, in Experiment 2, source was manipulated temporally, and in Experiment 3, source was manipulated by presenting stimuli in different modalities. Bilingual source discrimination was more accurate for low-frequency words than for high-frequency words, but it did not differ for the more fluent and less fluent languages (L1 and L2, respectively). These results contrast with the L2 advantage observed in item recognition (Francis & Gutiérrez, 2012; Francis & Strobach, 2013), adding to evidence that the bases of performance for item and source memory differ. The dissociation of word frequency and language effects indicates that word-context associations are made at the conceptual level rather than the word-form level. Bilinguals exhibited more accurate source discrimination than monolinguals, both under intentional and incidental encoding conditions, indicating that this effect cannot be explained entirely by differences in encoding strategies. We reason that relative to monolinguals, bilinguals more efficiently encode associations between word concepts and contexts or other types of information that do not convey meaning preexperimentally. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
|
18
|
Kim-Spoon J, Deater-Deckard K, Calkins SD, King-Casas B, Bell MA. Commonality between executive functioning and effortful control related to adjustment. JOURNAL OF APPLIED DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2018; 60:47-55. [PMID: 31073257 DOI: 10.1016/j.appdev.2018.10.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
This study examined the association between executive functioning (EF) and effortful control (EC), and tested whether cognitive control as the commonality of EF and EC, predicted competence and internalizing and externalizing symptomatology in children (N = 218, 6-8 years) and adolescents (N = 157, 13-14 years). Confirmatory factor analyses suggested cognitive control-inhibitory control and attentional control-as a significant overlap between EF and EC. Structural equation modeling analyses indicated that the cognitive control latent factor was associated with competence and internalizing and externalizing symptomatology among children and externalizing symptomatology among adolescents. The results provide evidence that inhibitory control and attentional control are the commonality between EF and EC and highlight that they are linked with positive and negative adjustment outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Kirby Deater-Deckard
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst
| | - Susan D Calkins
- Department of Human Development and Family Studies, University of North Carolina at Greensboro
| | - Brooks King-Casas
- Department of Psychology, Virginia Tech and Virginia Tech Carilion Research Institute
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Sleep and mindfulness meditation as they relate to false memory. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2018; 84:1084-1111. [PMID: 30244286 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-018-1098-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2018] [Accepted: 09/17/2018] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
By a systematic analysis of the current literature, we compare two states of sleep and meditation in terms of their role in the formation or suppression of Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) false memory. We aim to suggest that the occurrence of false memory under these two states is a result of reinforcing some abilities and changes in cognitive systems which can ultimately improve some aspects of cognitive functions. In our analogy, we propose that: (1) both sleep and meditation may improve source monitoring ability whose failure is one of the most important mechanisms in producing false memories, and (2) despite improvement in source monitoring ability, adaptive cognitive processes, as mechanisms which are common in sleep and meditation, can still produce false memories. In conclusion, we propose that in spite of their contribution to false memory through adaptive processes, the beneficial role of sleep and meditation in cognition may be more prominent than their harmful role.
Collapse
|
20
|
Tustin K, Hayne H. Recollection improves with age: children's and adults' accounts of their childhood experiences. Memory 2018; 27:92-102. [PMID: 29376766 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1432661] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Since the time of Freud, psychologists have drawn conclusions about children's memory on the basis of retrospective research with adults. Here, we turn the tables by examining what prospective studies with children and adolescents can tell us about the retrospective memory accounts provided by adults. Adults were interviewed about recent events and events from different points during their childhood (Age 5, Age 10) and early adolescence (Age 13). Children (5- and 8- to 9-year-olds) and young adolescents (12- to 13-year-olds) were interviewed about recent events. When matched for age at the time of encoding, adults recalled more about the target events than did 5-year-olds, even though the retention interval for adults was substantially longer. We conclude that retrospective studies with adults may lead researchers to overestimate the content of the early childhood memories that survive. We discuss the theoretical implications of these findings for an understanding of memory development and the practical implications for the interpretation of adults' retrospective accounts in the courtroom.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karen Tustin
- a Psychology Department , University of Otago , Dunedin , New Zealand
| | - Harlene Hayne
- a Psychology Department , University of Otago , Dunedin , New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Alibran É, Angel L, Bouazzaoui B, Gomot M, Isingrini M. Le développement des processus de remémoration et de familiarité : données comportementales et électrophysiologiques. ANNEE PSYCHOLOGIQUE 2018. [DOI: 10.3917/anpsy1.181.0029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022]
|
22
|
Effects of Source Recall Conditions on the Relationships among Source Monitoring, Inhibitory Control, and Working Memory. ADONGHAKOEJI 2017. [DOI: 10.5723/kjcs.2017.38.2.107] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
|
23
|
Parental reminiscing style and children’s suggestibility about an alleged transgression. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.08.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
|
24
|
Ford RM, Griffiths S, Neulinger K, Andrews G, Shum DHK, Gray PH. Impaired prospective memory but intact episodic memory in intellectually average 7- to 9-year-olds born very preterm and/or very low birth weight. Child Neuropsychol 2016; 23:954-979. [PMID: 27539515 DOI: 10.1080/09297049.2016.1216091] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Relatively little is known about episodic memory (EM: memory for personally-experienced events) and prospective memory (PM: memory for intended actions) in children born very preterm (VP) or with very low birth weight (VLBW). This study evaluates EM and PM in mainstream-schooled 7- to 9-year-olds born VP (≤ 32 weeks) and/or VLBW (< 1500 g) and matches full-term children for comparison (n = 35 and n = 37, respectively). Additionally, participants were assessed for verbal and non-verbal ability, executive function (EF), and theory of mind (ToM). The results show that the VP/VLBW children were outperformed by the full-term children on the memory tests overall, with a significant univariate group difference in PM. Moreover, within the VP/VLBW group, the measures of PM, verbal ability and working memory all displayed reliable negative correlations with severity of neonatal illness. PM was found to be independent of EM and cognitive functioning, suggesting that this form of memory might constitute a domain of specific vulnerability for VP/VLBW children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruth M Ford
- a Department of Psychology , Anglia Ruskin University , Cambridge , UK
| | - Sarah Griffiths
- b Menzies Health Institute Queensland and School of Applied Psychology , Griffith University , Queensland , Australia
| | - Kerryn Neulinger
- b Menzies Health Institute Queensland and School of Applied Psychology , Griffith University , Queensland , Australia
| | - Glenda Andrews
- b Menzies Health Institute Queensland and School of Applied Psychology , Griffith University , Queensland , Australia
| | - David H K Shum
- b Menzies Health Institute Queensland and School of Applied Psychology , Griffith University , Queensland , Australia
| | - Peter H Gray
- c Mater Research Institute , University of Queensland and Mater Mothers' Hospital , Queensland , Australia
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Blankenship TL, O'Neill M, Deater-Deckard K, Diana RA, Bell MA. Frontotemporal function]al connectivity and executive functions contribute to episodic memory performance. Int J Psychophysiol 2016; 107:72-82. [PMID: 27388478 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.06.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2015] [Revised: 06/27/2016] [Accepted: 06/28/2016] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
The contributions of hemispheric-specific electrophysiology (electroencephalogram or EEG) and independent executive functions (inhibitory control, working memory, cognitive flexibility) to episodic memory performance were examined using abstract paintings. Right hemisphere frontotemporal functional connectivity during encoding and retrieval, measured via EEG alpha coherence, statistically predicted performance on recency but not recognition judgments for the abstract paintings. Theta coherence, however, did not predict performance. Likewise, cognitive flexibility statistically predicted performance on recency judgments, but not recognition. These findings suggest that recognition and recency operate via separate electrophysiological and executive mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Meagan O'Neill
- Department of Psychology, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, VA, USA
| | - Kirby Deater-Deckard
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, USA
| | - Rachel A Diana
- Department of Psychology, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, VA, USA
| | - Martha Ann Bell
- Department of Psychology, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, VA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Sui J, Zhu Y. Five-year-olds can show the self-reference advantage. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT 2016. [DOI: 10.1177/01650250500172673] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
The current study developed a new paradigm to determine the age at which children begin to show the self-reference advantage in memory. Four-, 5-, and 10-year-olds studied lists of colourful object pictures presented together with self or other face image, and participants were asked to report aloud “who is pointing at the (object).” Then incidental free recall was carried out, followed by source judgments based on the earlier test where participants had to distinguish who pointed to the object. In Experiment 1, only 5-year-old children showed self-reference advantage in the recall, but not in source judgments. By increasing task demand in Experiment 2, 5 and 10-year-olds also showed the self-reference advantage in the recall, but not in source judgments. These results indicated that the new paradigm is appropriate to measure children's self-reference effect in memory, and children as young as 5 years begin to show this effect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jie Sui
- Peking University and Northeast Normal University, People's
Republic of China
| | - Ying Zhu
- Peking University, People's Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Hala S, McKay LA, Brown AMB, San Juan V. Source Monitoring and Executive Function in 2.5- to 3-Year-Olds. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2016. [DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2015.1058261] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
28
|
Blankenship SL, Riggins T. Developmental differences in relations between parent-reported executive function and unitized and non-unitized memory representations during childhood. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1214. [PMID: 26347683 PMCID: PMC4541290 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01214] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2015] [Accepted: 07/30/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous research has documented an association between executive functioning (EF) and memory for bound details. However, it is unknown if this relation varies as a function of the type of bound information (i.e., unitized versus non-unitized) and whether this association changes as a function of age during childhood, when both EF and memory undergo rapid development. The current study sought to address these gaps by examining whether relations between parent-reported EF differed for unitized versus non-unitized memory representations and if these relations differed between children who were 4, 6, or 8 years of age. Results revealed that EF was selectively associated with non-unitized associative memory in 8-year-old children; no significant relations between EF and either memory condition were evident in 4- or 6-year-olds. These results suggest relations between EF and memory may be specific to non-unitized representations and that this association may emerge across childhood as both EF and memory abilities develop.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah L Blankenship
- Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, University of Maryland, College Park MD, USA
| | - Tracy Riggins
- Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, University of Maryland, College Park MD, USA ; Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Phillips CI, Pexman PM. When Do Children Understand "Opposite"? JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2015; 58:1233-1244. [PMID: 25934951 DOI: 10.1044/2015_jslhr-l-14-0222] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/11/2014] [Accepted: 04/25/2015] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The aims of the present research were to determine (a) the age at which children with typical development understand the concept of opposite, (b) whether this is related to other cognitive abilities or experiences, and (c) whether there is early implicit understanding of the concept. METHOD Children (N = 204) between 3 and 5 years of age were randomly assigned to 1 of 3 experimental conditions in a novel opposite task. Children's language and working memory skills were assessed, and parents provided information about children's access to learning materials about opposites. RESULTS In the opposite task, 4- and 5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, demonstrated acquisition of the concept of opposite. Children demonstrated this understanding only when asked for the "opposite" one, suggesting that antonymy was not made salient by stimulus properties alone. Children's accuracy was not significantly related to their language or working memory skills, to their child care experience, or to whether parents reported having books or games about opposites or playing opposite word games with children. Eye gaze analyses provided no evidence for early implicit understanding of the concept of opposite. CONCLUSION Children with typical development have a concept of opposite by 4 years of age.
Collapse
|
30
|
Blankenship TL, Bell MA. Frontotemporal Coherence and Executive Functions Contribute to Episodic Memory During Middle Childhood. Dev Neuropsychol 2015; 40:430-44. [PMID: 27043829 PMCID: PMC4894647 DOI: 10.1080/87565641.2016.1153099] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Contributions of differential behavioral (executive functions) and electrophysiological (frontal-temporal electroencephalogram or EEG coherence) measures to episodic memory performance were examined during middle childhood. Cognitive flexibility and right frontotemporal functional connectivity during encoding (F4/T8), as well as left frontotemporal functional connectivity during retrieval (Fp1/T7), contributed to episodic memory performance in a sample of 9-12-year-olds. These results suggest that executive functions differentially influence episodic memory, as does left and right frontotemporal functional connectivity during different portions of the memory task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Martha Ann Bell
- a Department of Psychology , Virginia Tech , Blacksburg , Virginia
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Rajan V, Bell MA. Developmental changes in fact and source recall: contributions from executive function and brain electrical activity. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2014; 12:1-11. [PMID: 25459873 PMCID: PMC4385478 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.10.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/03/2013] [Revised: 06/27/2014] [Accepted: 10/10/2014] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Fact recall improved between 6 and 8 years, whereas source recall was comparable. Executive function uniquely predicted source recall, controlling for age and language. Task-related increases in theta power were observed for fact and source recall. Executive processes support memory for context. Theta synchronization reflects retrieval-related processes in middle childhood.
Source memory involves recollecting the contextual details surrounding a memory episode. When source information is bound together, it makes a memory episodic in nature. Unfortunately, very little is known about the factors that contribute to its formation in early development. This study examined the development of source memory in middle childhood. Measures of executive function were examined as potential sources of variation in fact and source recall. Continuous electroencephalogram (EEG) measures were collected during baseline and fact and source retrieval in order to examine memory-related changes in EEG power. Six and 8-year-old children were taught 10 novel facts from two different sources and recall for fact and source information was later tested. Older children were better on fact recall, but both ages were comparable on source recall. However, source recall performance was poor at both ages, suggesting that this ability continues to develop beyond middle childhood. Regression analyses revealed that executive function uniquely predicted variance in source recall performance. Task-related increases in theta power were observed at frontal, temporal and parietal electrode sites during fact and source retrieval. This investigation contributes to our understanding of age-related differences in source memory processing in middle childhood.
Collapse
|
32
|
Batta B, Király I, Tompa T. Automatic and controlled monitoring processes in source memory. MAGYAR PSZICHOLÓGIAI SZEMLE 2014; 69:495-514. [DOI: 10.1556/mpszle.69.2014.3.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/27/2023]
Abstract
Vizsgálatunk célja az emlékezeti előhívás során aktiválódó asszociatív és stratégiavezérelt folyamatok (Shingés mtsai, 2010) mechanizmusainak feltárása volt perceptuális forrásmonitorozási paradigma segítségével. E kérdést fejlődési kontextusba helyezve óvodás korú, kisiskolás és felnőtt személyek teljesítményét hasonlítottuk össze egy emlékezeti feladatban. A hétköznapi színűk alapján kongruens, inkongruens és neutrális képeket alakítottunk ki és mutattunk be incidentális tanulási helyzetben, majd azt kértük a vizsgálati személyektől, hogy emlékezzenek arra, milyen színű volt a tesztfázisban újra bemutatott, ezúttal fekete-fehér kép. Hipotézisünk alapján azt vártuk, hogy különbség mutatkozik a csoportok között az inkongruens és neutrális képek helyes színére való emlékezésben, azonban nem feltétlenül találunk különbséget a kongruens színű képek színének felidézésekor, elsősorban a már gyermekkorban aktívnak és érettnek mutatkozó asszociatív folyamatoknak köszönhetően. Eltérést találtunk a kongruens és inkongruens képekre való emlékezésben, illetve az inkongruens képek színében elkövetett hibák mintázatában. Eredményeink megerősítették hipotézisünket, mely szerint a gyermekek előhívási profilját a már érett, felnőttekhez hasonló, univerzális tudásra építő asszociatív folyamatok és az éretlen, gátlási mechanizmusokat is tartalmazó stratégiavezérelt folyamatok jellemzik.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara Batta
- 1 ELTE Pedagógiai és Pszichológiai kar Kognitív Pszichológiai tanszék Budapest Magyarország
| | - Ildikó Király
- 1 ELTE Pedagógiai és Pszichológiai kar Kognitív Pszichológiai tanszék Budapest Magyarország
| | - Tamás Tompa
- 2 Medical University of South Carolina ME Preventív Medicina Tanszék; Department of Neuroscience Charleston South Carolina USA
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Dumontheil I. Development of abstract thinking during childhood and adolescence: the role of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2014; 10:57-76. [PMID: 25173960 PMCID: PMC6987955 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.07.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 112] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2014] [Revised: 07/29/2014] [Accepted: 07/31/2014] [Indexed: 12/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Rostral prefrontal cortex (RPFC) supports self-generated, abstract thought processing. Flexibly attending towards and processing abstract thoughts develop in adolescence. RPFC activation becomes more specific to relational integration during development. Prospective memory development remains to be further studied using neuroimaging. Training of abstract thinking, e.g. reasoning, may have implication for education.
Rostral prefrontal cortex (RPFC) has increased in size and changed in terms of its cellular organisation during primate evolution. In parallel emerged the ability to detach oneself from the immediate environment to process abstract thoughts and solve problems and to understand other individuals’ thoughts and intentions. Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex (RLPFC) is thought to play an important role in supporting the integration of abstract, often self-generated, thoughts. Thoughts can be temporally abstract and relate to long term goals, or past or future events, or relationally abstract and focus on the relationships between representations rather than simple stimulus features. Behavioural studies have provided evidence of a prolonged development of the cognitive functions associated with RLPFC, in particular logical and relational reasoning, but also episodic memory retrieval and prospective memory. Functional and structural neuroimaging studies provide further support for a prolonged development of RLPFC during adolescence, with some evidence of increased specialisation of RLPFC activation for relational integration and aspects of episodic memory retrieval. Topics for future research will be discussed, such as the role of medial RPFC in processing abstract thoughts in the social domain, the possibility of training abstract thinking in the domain of reasoning, and links to education.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iroise Dumontheil
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, UK; Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Earhart B, Roberts KP. The role of executive function in children's source monitoring with varying retrieval strategies. Front Psychol 2014; 5:405. [PMID: 24847302 PMCID: PMC4021134 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00405] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/17/2014] [Accepted: 04/17/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous research on the relationship between executive function and source monitoring in young children has been inconclusive, with studies finding conflicting results about whether working memory and inhibitory control are related to source-monitoring ability. In this study, the role of working memory and inhibitory control in recognition memory and source monitoring with two different retrieval strategies were examined. Children (N = 263) aged 4–8 participated in science activities with two sources. They were later given a recognition and source-monitoring test, and completed measures of working memory and inhibitory control. During the source-monitoring test, half of the participants were asked about sources serially (one after the other) whereas the other half of the children were asked about sources in parallel (considering both sources simultaneously). Results demonstrated that working memory was a predictor of source-monitoring accuracy in both conditions, but inhibitory control was only related to source accuracy in the parallel condition. When age was controlled these relationships were no longer significant, suggesting that a more general cognitive development factor is a stronger predictor of source monitoring than executive function alone. Interestingly, the children aged 4–6 years made more accurate source decisions in the parallel condition than in the serial condition. The older children (aged 7–8) were overall more accurate than the younger children, and their accuracy did not differ as a function of interview condition. Suggestions are provided to guide further research in this area that will clarify the diverse results of previous studies examining whether executive function is a cognitive prerequisite for effective source monitoring.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Becky Earhart
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | - Kim P Roberts
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University Waterloo, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Rajan V, Cuevas K, Bell MA. The Contribution of Executive Function to Source Memory Development in Early Childhood. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2014; 15:304-324. [PMID: 24829540 DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2013.763809] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
Age-related differences in episodic memory judgments assessing recall of fact information and the source of this information were examined. The role of executive function in supporting early episodic memory ability was also explored. Four- and 6-year-old children were taught 10 novel facts from two different sources (experimenter or puppet) and memory for both fact and source information was later tested. Measures of working memory, inhibitory control, and set-shifting were obtained to produce an indicator of children's executive function. Six-year-olds recalled more fact and source information than 4-year-olds. Regression analyses revealed that age, language ability, and executive function accounted for unique variance in children's fact recall and source recall performance. These findings suggest a link between episodic memory and executive function, and we propose that developmental investigations should further explore this association.
Collapse
|
36
|
Miller SE, Chatley N, Marcovitch S, Rogers MM. One of these things is not like the other: distinctiveness and executive function in preschoolers. J Exp Child Psychol 2013; 118:143-51. [PMID: 24210556 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.09.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/19/2013] [Revised: 09/28/2013] [Accepted: 09/29/2013] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
There is scant evidence that children younger than 7 years show a memory advantage for distinct information, a memory phenomenon termed the isolation effect (Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2001, Vol. 27, pp. 1359-1366). We investigated whether 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds' developing organizational processing and executive function contributed to the isolation effect, demonstrated when recall was better for a semantically unique target (e.g., sheep, pig, watermelon, duck) rather than a semantically common target (e.g., apple, banana, watermelon, strawberry). To encourage organizational processing, children were asked to categorize each item presented. Children also completed working memory and cognitive flexibility tasks, and only children who scored high in cognitive flexibility demonstrated the isolation effect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephanie E Miller
- Department of Psychology, University of Mississippi, University, MS 38677, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Goldenberg ER, Sandhofer CM. Who is she? Changes in the person context affect categorization. Front Psychol 2013; 4:745. [PMID: 24133477 PMCID: PMC3796328 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00745] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2013] [Accepted: 09/25/2013] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Changes between the learning and testing contexts affect learning, memory, and generalization. We examined whether a change (between learning and testing) in the person children were interacting with affects generalization. Three-, four-, and five-year-old children were trained on eight novel noun categories by one experimenter. Children were tested for their ability to generalize the label to a new category member by either the same experimenter who trained them or by a novel experimenter. Three-year-old children's performance was not affected by who they were tested by. Four- and five-year-old children's performance was lower when tested by the novel experimenter. The results are discussed in terms of source monitoring and the effect of perceptual context change on category generalization.
Collapse
|
38
|
Guillery-Girard B, Martins S, Deshayes S, Hertz-Pannier L, Chiron C, Jambaqué I, Landeau B, Clochon P, Chételat G, Eustache F. Developmental trajectories of associative memory from childhood to adulthood: a behavioral and neuroimaging study. Front Behav Neurosci 2013; 7:126. [PMID: 24098276 PMCID: PMC3784827 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00126] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2013] [Accepted: 09/05/2013] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Episodic memory refers to the capacity to bind multimodal memories to constitute a unique personal event. Most developmental studies on episodic memory focused on one specific component, i.e., the core factual information. The present study examines the relevance of a novel episodic paradigm to assess its developmental trajectories in a more comprehensive way according to the type of association (item-feature, item-location, and item-sequence associations) with measures of both objective and subjective recollection. We conducted a behavioral study aimed at testing the effects of age in a large sample of 160 children, adolescents, and young adults (6-23 years old). We confronted the behavioral data to the neural correlates in a subgroup of 30 children using voxel-based morphometry. Behavioral data outlined differential developmental trajectories according to the type of association, with a continuous increase of factual associative memory efficiency until 10 years, a linear increase of performance in spatial associative memory that pursues until early adulthood and an abrupt increase in temporal associative memory efficiency between 9 and 10. Regarding recollection, measures showed a more pronounced enhancement from 9 to 10 years. Hence, behavioral data highlight a peculiar period in late childhood (8-10 years old) crucial for the developmental time course of episodic memory. Regarding structural data, we found that the improvement of associative memory efficiency was related to a decrease in gray matter volume in a large cerebral network including the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (and superior and anterior temporal regions), and the hippocampus bilaterally. These data suggest that multimodal integration would probably be related to the maturation of temporal regions and modulated by a fronto-parietal network. Besides, our findings emphasize the relevance of the present paradigm to assess episodic memory especially in the clinical setting.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bérengère Guillery-Girard
- INSERM, U1077 , Caen , France ; Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, UMR-S1077 , Caen , France ; Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, UMR-S1077 , Caen , France ; CHU de Caen, UMR-S1077 , Caen , France
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
39
|
Augusti EM, Melinder A. The effect of neutral and negative colour photographs on children's item directed forgetting. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2013. [DOI: 10.1080/17405629.2012.686741] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
40
|
Zajac R, O’Neill S, Hayne H. Disorder in the courtroom? Child witnesses under cross-examination. DEVELOPMENTAL REVIEW 2012. [DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2012.06.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
41
|
Parker A, Dagnall N. Effects of saccadic bilateral eye movements on memory in children and adults: An exploratory study. Brain Cogn 2012; 78:238-47. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.01.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/20/2010] [Revised: 11/05/2011] [Accepted: 01/06/2012] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
|
42
|
El Haj M, Allain P. What do we know about the relationship between source monitoring deficits and executive dysfunction? Neuropsychol Rehabil 2012; 22:449-72. [PMID: 22324387 DOI: 10.1080/09602011.2012.658267] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
Abstract
In clinical neuropsychology, source monitoring deficits have been classically attributed to executive dysfunction. Nevertheless, in this review we identified only 16 papers that provided statistical data about the relationships between source monitoring and executive processes. Surprisingly, they reported either a total, partial or non-existent relationship between source monitoring and executive tasks. In order to understand and explain these contradictions, we classified the source and executive tasks of the 16 papers according to two well-accepted definitions. Source tasks were classified using the Source Monitoring Framework (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993 ) which specifies reality and external and internal source monitoring. Executive tasks were classified according to the model of Miyake Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, and Howerter (2000) which specifies complex, shifting, updating and inhibition tasks. We found that evaluation of reality and internal source monitoring was limited. Regarding executive functions, there was no assessment of updating and only a limited assessment of shifting and inhibition. Therefore, the relationship between source monitoring and executive functions remains an open question. Our findings point to the need for the simultaneous assessment of source monitoring and executive functions as defined by multidimensional theoretical frameworks. Such investigations would help in understanding the relationship between specific source monitoring deficits and specific executive decline in clinical populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mohamad El Haj
- Laboratoire de Psychologie EA 2646, Université d'Angers, France.
| | | |
Collapse
|
43
|
KANAKOGI Y, MORIGUCHI Y, FU G, LEE K, ITAKURA S. HOW DOES EXECUTIVE FUNCTION CONTRIBUTE TO SOURCE MONITORING IN YOUNG CHILDREN? PSYCHOLOGIA 2012. [DOI: 10.2117/psysoc.2012.194] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
44
|
Ford RM, Lobao SN, Macaulay C, Herdman LM. Empathy, theory of mind, and individual differences in the appropriation bias among 4- and 5-year-olds. J Exp Child Psychol 2011; 110:626-46. [PMID: 21757207 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2011] [Revised: 06/07/2011] [Accepted: 06/07/2011] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Evidence that young children often claim ownership of their partner's contributions to an earlier collaborative activity, the appropriation bias, has been attributed to shared intentionality (Cognitive Development (1998) 13, 91-108). The current investigation explored this notion by examining individual differences in the bias among 4- and 5-year-olds as a function of empathy and theory of mind. On two occasions, children joined an adult and two dolls (with each doll being operated by one of the humans) in a picture matching board game before being asked to remember who placed each picture. Children showed a robust appropriation bias despite excellent recognition memory for the studied pictures (Study 1) and particularly in relation to the human sources (Study 2). Whereas higher levels of self-reported empathy were associated with a greater frequency of appropriation errors and fewer correct attributions for pictures placed by the adult and her doll partner, the opposite pattern emerged for theory of mind. Moreover, the positive relations between theory of mind and source monitoring accuracy remained robust after controlling for general ability and inhibitory skills. We consider the implications of these findings for understanding the processes driving the appropriation bias.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruth M Ford
- School of Psychology and Griffith Health Institute, Griffith University, Brisbane 4122, Australia.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
Otgaar H, Alberts H, Cuppens L. How Cognitive Resources Alter our Perception of the Past: Ego Depletion Enhances the Susceptibility to Suggestion. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2011. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1810] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
|
46
|
Scott RC, Richard GR, Holmes GL, Lenck-Santini PP. Maturational dynamics of hippocampal place cells in immature rats. Hippocampus 2011; 21:347-53. [PMID: 20865725 PMCID: PMC3010481 DOI: 10.1002/hipo.20789] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
The ontogeny of neural substrates underlying episodic memory is not well described. Place cells are a surrogate for episodic memory and are important for spatial navigation in rodents. Although place cells are well described in mature brains, the nature of the maturation processes remains uncertain. We now report on the ontogeny of the place cell system in rats between P22 and P43, a time during which there is rapid improvement in spatial behavior. We found that place cells with adult like firing fields were observed at the earliest ages. However, at this age, adult like place cells were few in number and their place fields were not stable across multiple exposures to the same environment. Independently of confounding factors such as the number of exposures to the environment, the proportion of adult-like place cells, their firing rate, and their stability increased with age and the average spatial signal of all pyramidal cells improved. These findings could account for the poor spatial behavior observed at young ages (P20-P30) and suggests that a small number of adult-like place cells are insufficient to support navigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rod C Scott
- Department of Neurology, Neuroscience Center at Dartmouth, Dartmouth Medical School, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03756, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Cognitive processes supporting episodic memory formation in childhood: The role of source memory, binding, and executive functioning. DEVELOPMENTAL REVIEW 2010. [DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2011.02.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
48
|
Pasquali A, Timmermans B, Cleeremans A. Know thyself: metacognitive networks and measures of consciousness. Cognition 2010; 117:182-90. [PMID: 20825936 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 62] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/15/2009] [Revised: 07/05/2010] [Accepted: 08/11/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Subjective measures of awareness rest on the assumption that conscious knowledge is knowledge that participants know they possess. Post-Decision Wagering (PDW), recently proposed as a new measure of awareness, requires participants to place a high or a low wager on their decisions. Whereas advantageous wagering indicates awareness of the knowledge on which the decisions are based, cases in which participants fail to optimize their wagers suggest performance without awareness. Here, we hypothesize that wagering and other subjective measures of awareness reflect metacognitive capacities subtended by self-developed metarepresentations that inform an agent about its own internal states. To support this idea, we present three simulations in which neural networks learn to wager on their own responses. The simulations illustrate essential properties that are required for such metarepresentations to influence PDW as a measure of awareness. Results demonstrate a good fit to human data. We discuss the implications of this modeling work for our understanding of consciousness and its measures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Antoine Pasquali
- Consciousness, Cognition, and Computation Group, Université Libre de Bruxelles, 1050 Bruxelles, Belgium.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
49
|
Czernochowski D, Mecklinger A, Johansson M. Age-related changes in the control of episodic retrieval: an ERP study of recognition memory in children and adults. Dev Sci 2009; 12:1026-40. [PMID: 19840057 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00841.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
We examined developmental aspects of the ability to monitor the temporal context of an item's previous occurrence while event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded. In a continuous recognition task, children between 10 and 12 years and young adults watched a stream of pictures repeated with a lag of 10-15 intervening items and indicated recurrences. In a second run, these already familiar pictures were repeated as non-targets along with new pictures, while subjects were instructed to indicate only recurrences within the run. Young adults were able to maintain high performance levels in both tasks, whereas children had longer response times and committed a large number of false alarms to non-targets. ERPs in both age groups showed similar parietal old/new effects for target repetitions within runs. In addition, adults' ERPs showed similar old/new effects at frontal electrodes for repetitions and non-targets, presumably reflecting assessments of familiarity, whereas for children repeated relative to first presentations were associated with more negative-going waveforms at anterior frontal recording sites. Together, these results suggest a continuing maturation of the brain networks assessing novelty or familiarity. Recollection as indexed by parietal old/new effects appeared similar between young adults and children, but the development of controlled episodic retrieval, resulting in recollection of non-target information, appears to continue well into adolescence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniela Czernochowski
- Mathematical and Cognitive Psychology, Heinrich-Heine University, Duesseldorf, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
50
|
Abstract
Research and theorizing on executive function (EF) in childhood has been disproportionately focused on preschool age children. This review paper outlines the importance of examining EF throughout childhood, and even across the lifespan. First, examining EF in older children can address the question of whether EF is a unitary construct. The relations among the EF components, particularly as they are recruited for complex tasks, appear to change over the course of development. Second, much of the development of EF, especially working memory, shifting, and planning, occurs after age 5. Third, important applications of EF research concern the role of school-age children's EF in various aspects of school performance, as well as social functioning and emotional control. Future research needs to examine a more complete developmental span, from early childhood through late adulthood, in order to address developmental issues adequately.
Collapse
|