1
|
Ross CT, Pisor AC. Perceived inequality and variability in the expression of parochial altruism. EVOLUTIONARY HUMAN SCIENCES 2025; 7:e3. [PMID: 39935444 PMCID: PMC11810521 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2024.43] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/06/2024] [Revised: 08/23/2024] [Accepted: 08/28/2024] [Indexed: 02/13/2025] Open
Abstract
It is commonly argued that humans have generalised predispositions for within-group favouritism and between-group animus (i.e. that humans are parochially altruistic), leading to higher levels of internal conflict in societies with greater diversity. Other research, however, has questioned both the ubiquity of parochial altruism and the role of diversity per se in causing social discord. Here, we use ethnographic, social network and experimental economic game data to explore this topic in two multi-ethnic Colombian communities. We examine the extent to which Afrocolombian and Emberá residents express parochial altruism, finding appreciable variability between communities, and across individuals within communities. When present, parochial altruism appears to be driven by divergent perceptions of group-based economic need, not group identity per se. Our results suggest that diversity may be less likely to cause social discord than past work has suggested, as long as group-based inequalities in wealth, well-being and representation - that can destabilise positive inter-group relationships - are minimised.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cody T. Ross
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and Culture, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Anne C. Pisor
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and Culture, Leipzig, Germany
- Department of Anthropology and Social Science Research Institute, The Pennyslvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
- Department of Anthropology, Washington State University, Pullman, WA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Hoffman M, Moya C. Group myths can create shared understanding even if they don't act as superstimuli. Behav Brain Sci 2025; 47:e181. [PMID: 39743812 DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x24000785] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2025]
Abstract
Sijilmassi et al. argue that myths around shared ancestry and history exploit an evolved psychology of interdependence. In contrast, we argue that psychological exploitation is not required. Rather, such myths may be one method, among many, to create a shared understanding of group boundaries, which can be "self-enforcing." We summarize the game-theoretic basis for this account and some supportive evidence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Moshe Hoffman
- Department of Economics, Boston College, Harvard University, Boston, MA, USA ; https://sites.google.com/site/hoffmanmoshe/
| | - Cristina Moya
- Department of Anthropology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA ; https://sites.google.com/site/cristinasolermoya/
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Mannheim B. Mother Tongue, Father Tongue, Place Tongue: Twenty-First-Century Language Transmission and Language Survival in the Andes and Western Amazonia. JOURNAL OF ANTHROPOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2022. [DOI: 10.1086/721974] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Bruce Mannheim
- Department of Anthropology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Integrating cultural evolution and behavioral genetics. Behav Brain Sci 2022; 45:e182. [PMID: 36098400 DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x22000036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Abstract
The 29 commentaries amplified our key arguments; offered extensions, implications, and applications of the framework; and pushed back and clarified. To help forge the path forward for cultural evolutionary behavioral genetics, we (1) focus on conceptual disagreements and misconceptions about the concepts of heritability and culture; (2) further discuss points raised about the intertwined relationship between culture and genes; and (3) address extensions to the proposed framework, particularly as it relates to cultural clusters, development, and power. These commentaries, and the deep engagement they represent, reinforce the importance of integrating cultural evolution and behavioral genetics.
Collapse
|
5
|
van der Does T, Galesic M, Dunivin ZO, Smaldino PE. Strategic identity signaling in heterogeneous networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2022; 119:e2117898119. [PMID: 35239438 PMCID: PMC8915961 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2117898119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/29/2021] [Accepted: 01/19/2022] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
SignificanceMuch of online conversation today consists of signaling one's political identity. Although many signals are obvious to everyone, others are covert, recognizable to one's ingroup while obscured from the outgroup. This type of covert identity signaling is critical for collaborations in a diverse society, but measuring covert signals has been difficult, slowing down theoretical development. We develop a method to detect covert and overt signals in tweets posted before the 2020 US presidential election and use a behavioral experiment to test predictions of a mathematical theory of covert signaling. Our results show that covert political signaling is more common when the perceived audience is politically diverse and open doors to a better understanding of communication in politically polarized societies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Mirta Galesic
- Santa Fe Institute, Santa Fe, NM 87501
- Complexity Science Hub Vienna, 1080 Vienna, Austria
- Vermont Complex Systems Center, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT 05405
| | - Zackary Okun Dunivin
- Center for Complex Networks and Systems Research, Luddy School of Informatics, Computer Science, and Engineering, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47408
- Department of Sociology, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405
| | - Paul E. Smaldino
- Department of Cognitive and Information Sciences, University of California, Merced, CA 95343
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Pisor AC, Ross CT. Distinguishing Intergroup and Long-Distance Relationships. HUMAN NATURE (HAWTHORNE, N.Y.) 2022; 33:280-303. [PMID: 36181615 PMCID: PMC9741575 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-022-09431-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/07/2022] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Abstract
Intergroup and long-distance relationships are both central features of human social life, but because intergroup relationships are emphasized in the literature, long-distance relationships are often overlooked. Here, we make the case that intergroup and long-distance relationships should be studied as distinct, albeit related, features of human sociality. First, we review the functions of both kinds of relationship: while both can be conduits for difficult-to-access resources, intergroup relationships can reduce intergroup conflict whereas long-distance relationships are especially effective at buffering widespread resource shortfalls. Second, to illustrate the importance of distinguishing the two relationship types, we present a case study from rural Bolivia. Combining ethnography and two different experimental techniques, we find that the importance of intergroup relationships-and the salience of group membership itself-varies across populations and across methods. Although ethnography revealed that participants often rely on long-distance relationships for resource access, we were unable to capture participant preferences for these relationships with a forced-choice technique. Taken together, our review and empirical data highlight that (1) intergroup and long-distance relationships can have different functions and can be more or less important in different contexts and (2) validating experimental field data with ethnography is crucial for work on human sociality. We close by outlining future directions for research on long-distance relationships in humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anne C. Pisor
- grid.30064.310000 0001 2157 6568Department of Anthropology, Washington State University, Pullman, WA USA ,grid.419518.00000 0001 2159 1813Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Cody T. Ross
- grid.419518.00000 0001 2159 1813Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Padilla-Iglesias C, Kramer KL. The Role of Language in Structuring Social Networks Following Market Integration in a Yucatec Maya Population. Front Psychol 2021; 12:656963. [PMID: 34975603 PMCID: PMC8716436 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.656963] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/22/2021] [Accepted: 11/09/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Language is the human universal mode of communication, and is dynamic and constantly in flux accommodating user needs as individuals interface with a changing world. However, we know surprisingly little about how language responds to market integration, a pressing force affecting indigenous communities worldwide today. While models of culture change often emphasize the replacement of one language, trait, or phenomenon with another following socioeconomic transitions, we present a more nuanced framework. We use demographic, economic, linguistic, and social network data from a rural Maya community that spans a 27-year period and the transition to market integration. By adopting this multivariate approach for the acquisition and use of languages, we find that while the number of bilingual speakers has significantly increased over time, bilingualism appears stable rather than transitionary. We provide evidence that when indigenous and majority languages provide complementary social and economic payoffs, both can be maintained. Our results predict the circumstances under which indigenous language use may be sustained or at risk. More broadly, the results point to the evolutionary dynamics that shaped the current distribution of the world's linguistic diversity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cecilia Padilla-Iglesias
- Department of Anthropology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
- Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies, Department of Archaeology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - Karen L. Kramer
- Anthropology Department, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Tucker B, Ringen EJ, Tombo J, Hajasoa P, Gérard S, Lahiniriko R, Garçon AH. Ethnic Markers without Ethnic Conflict : Why do Interdependent Masikoro, Mikea, and Vezo of Madagascar Signal their Ethnic Differences? HUMAN NATURE-AN INTERDISCIPLINARY BIOSOCIAL PERSPECTIVE 2021; 32:529-556. [PMID: 34546550 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09412-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/20/2021] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
People often signal their membership in groups through their clothes, hairstyle, posture, and dialect. Most existing evolutionary models argue that markers label group members so individuals can preferentially interact with those in their group. Here we ask why people mark ethnic differences when interethnic interaction is routine, necessary, and peaceful. We asked research participants from three ethnic groups in southwestern Madagascar to sort photos of unfamiliar people by ethnicity, and by with whom they would prefer or not prefer to cooperate, in a wage labor vignette. Results indicate that southwestern Malagasy reliably send and detect ethnic signals; they signal less in the marketplace, a primary site of interethnic coordination and cooperation; and they do not prefer co-ethnics as cooperation partners in novel circumstances. Results from a cultural knowledge survey and calculations of cultural FST suggest that these ethnic groups have relatively little cultural differentiation. We concur with Moya and Boyd (Human Nature 26:1-27, 2015) that ethnicity is unlikely to be a singular social phenomenon. The current functions of ethnic divisions and marking may be different from those at the moment of ethnogenesis. Group identities may persist without group conflict or differentiation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bram Tucker
- Department of Anthropology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA.
| | - Erik J Ringen
- Department of Anthropology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Jaovola Tombo
- Department of Geography, Université de Toliara, Toliara, Madagascar
| | - Patricia Hajasoa
- Department of Geography, Université de Toliara, Toliara, Madagascar
| | - Soanahary Gérard
- Department of Geography, Université de Toliara, Toliara, Madagascar
| | - Rolland Lahiniriko
- Department of Malagasy Language, Civilization, and Letters, Université de Toliara, Toliara, Madagascar
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Opportunities for Interaction. HUMAN NATURE-AN INTERDISCIPLINARY BIOSOCIAL PERSPECTIVE 2021; 32:208-238. [DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09393-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/26/2021] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
|
10
|
Pisor AC, Jones JH. Do people manage climate risk through long-distance relationships? Am J Hum Biol 2020; 33:e23525. [PMID: 33103823 DOI: 10.1002/ajhb.23525] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2020] [Revised: 10/08/2020] [Accepted: 10/09/2020] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Long-distance social relationships have been a feature of human evolutionary history; evidence from the paleoanthropological, archeological, and ethnographic records suggest that one function of these relationships is to manage the risk of resource shortfalls due to climate variability. We should expect long-distance relationships to be especially important when shortfalls are chronic or temporally positively autocorrelated, as these are more likely to exhaust local adaptations for managing risk. Further, individuals who experience shortfalls not as rare shocks, but as patterned events, should be more likely to pay the costs of maintaining long-distance relationships. We test these hypotheses in the context of two communities of Bolivian horticulturalists, where climate variability-especially precipitation variability-is relevant to production and access to long-distance connections is improving. METHODS Data on individuals' migration histories, social relationships, and other relevant variables were collected in 2017 (n = 119). Precipitation data were obtained from the US National Center for Atmospheric Research, allowing us to estimate participants' exposure to drought and excess precipitation. RESULTS Exposure duration, proximity in time, and frequency did not predict having a greater number of long-distance relationships. Males, extraverted individuals, and those who had traveled more did have more long-distance relationships, however. CONCLUSION Another function of long-distance relationships is to access resources that can never be obtained locally; ethnographic data suggest this is their primary function in rural Bolivia. We conclude by refining our predictions about the conditions under which long-distance relationships are likely to help individuals manage the risks posed by climate variability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anne C Pisor
- Department of Anthropology, Washington State University, Pullman, Washington, USA.,Department of Human Behavior, Ecology, and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - James Holland Jones
- Department of Earth System Science, Stanford University, Stanford, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Padilla-Iglesias C, Foley RA, Shneidman LA. Language as a marker of ethnic identity among the Yucatec Maya. EVOLUTIONARY HUMAN SCIENCES 2020; 2:e38. [PMID: 37588346 PMCID: PMC10427450 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2020.39] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Most human variation is structured around symbolically marked cultural ('ethnic') groups that require common codes of communication. Consequently, many have hypothesised that using others' linguistic competences as markers of their descent is part of an evolved human psychology. However, there is also evidence that the use of language as ethnic markers is not universally applied, but context specific. We explore the tension between these views by studying responses to bilingualism among 121 adults living in Mayan communities undergoing rapid socioeconomic changes involving increased contact with Spanish-speaking towns. We show that, although competences in Mayan were strongly tied to perceiving others as having a Mayan ethnic identity, ethnolinguistic category membership was not seen as stable through life, vertically transmitted, nor regarded as incompatible with competences in Spanish. Moreover, we find variation in how people reasoned about ethnolinguistic identities depending on their own linguistic repertoires. Our work suggests that, while there may be an evolved predisposition to use language as a signal of group identity, our developmental plasticity allows us to respond adaptively to social information around us, leading to psychological and behavioural variation within and across populations. How people reason about others based on their linguistic profiles will affect the payoffs of acquiring different languages and ultimately the long-term sustainability of linguistic diversity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cecilia Padilla-Iglesias
- Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies, Department of Archaeology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
- Department of Anthropology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Robert A. Foley
- Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies, Department of Archaeology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Pathways to cognitive design. Behav Processes 2019; 161:73-86. [DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2018.05.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2017] [Revised: 05/01/2018] [Accepted: 05/22/2018] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
13
|
Bunce JA, McElreath R. Sustainability of minority culture when inter-ethnic interaction is profitable. Nat Hum Behav 2018. [DOI: 10.1038/s41562-018-0306-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
14
|
|
15
|
Robinson EJH, Barker JL. Inter-group cooperation in humans and other animals. Biol Lett 2017; 13:20160793. [PMID: 28250206 PMCID: PMC5377026 DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2016.0793] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2016] [Accepted: 02/02/2017] [Indexed: 01/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Social interactions are often characterized by cooperation within groups and conflict or competition between groups. In certain circumstances, however, cooperation can arise between social groups. Here, we examine the circumstances under which inter-group cooperation is expected to emerge and present examples with particular focus on groups in two well-studied but dissimilar taxa: humans and ants. Drivers for the evolution of inter-group cooperation include overarching threats from predators, competitors or adverse conditions, and group-level resource asymmetries. Resources can differ between groups in both quantity and type. Where the difference is in type, inequalities can lead to specialization and division of labour between groups, a phenomenon characteristic of human societies, but rarely seen in other animals. The ability to identify members of one's own group is essential for social coherence; we consider the proximate roles of identity effects in shaping inter-group cooperation and allowing membership of multiple groups. Finally, we identify numerous valuable avenues for future research that will improve our understanding of the processes shaping inter-group cooperation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elva J H Robinson
- Department of Biology and York Centre for Complex Systems Analysis, University of York, York, UK
| | - Jessica L Barker
- Aarhus Institute of Advanced Studies, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Pisor AC, Gurven M. Risk buffering and resource access shape valuation of out-group strangers. Sci Rep 2016; 6:30435. [PMID: 27470126 PMCID: PMC4965756 DOI: 10.1038/srep30435] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2016] [Accepted: 07/05/2016] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Unlike other primates, humans exhibit extensive inter-group tolerance and frequently build relationships with out-group members. Despite its common occurrence, little is known about the conditions leading to out-group relationship building in humans. What are the social and ecological factors promoting valuation of out-group members as potential social partners? Do they differ from those promoting valuation of in-group members? We propose that opportunities for non-local resource access and resource buffering, crucial in the human foraging niche, will increase valuation of out-group strangers. Using survey and experimental data collected among three Bolivian horticultural populations, we find that individuals with fewer non-locally available resources and more information about out-groups demonstrate more generosity toward out-group strangers, but not in-group strangers. The effects are specific to subjective resource access, not objective measures of access, and out-group exposure, not stereotypes. Further, depending on the measure, existing network connections affect both out-group and in-group giving, suggesting that new partnerships from both in-groups and out-groups may bolster one's networks. Our results illustrate how evolved human psychology is sensitive to the costs and benefits of both out-group and in-group relationships, but underscore that the social and ecological factors favoring new relationships with in-group versus out-group strangers may differ.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anne C Pisor
- Department of Anthropology, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106-3210, USA.,Department of Human Behavior, Evolution, and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Michael Gurven
- Department of Anthropology, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106-3210, USA
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Moya C, Boyd R. The Evolution and Development of Inferential Reasoning about Ethnic Markers. CURRENT ANTHROPOLOGY 2016. [DOI: 10.1086/685939] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
|
18
|
Culture–gene coevolutionary psychology: cultural learning, language, and ethnic psychology. Curr Opin Psychol 2016; 8:112-118. [DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/22/2015] [Revised: 09/17/2015] [Accepted: 10/03/2015] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
|
19
|
Moya C, Boyd R, Henrich J. Reasoning About Cultural and Genetic Transmission: Developmental and Cross-Cultural Evidence From Peru, Fiji, and the United States on How People Make Inferences About Trait Transmission. Top Cogn Sci 2015; 7:595-610. [PMID: 26417672 PMCID: PMC4661786 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12163] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2013] [Revised: 08/11/2014] [Accepted: 09/26/2014] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Using samples from three diverse populations, we test evolutionary hypotheses regarding how people reason about the inheritance of various traits. First, we provide a framework for differentiat-ing the outputs of mechanisms that evolved for reasoning about variation within and between (a) biological taxa and (b) culturally evolved ethnic categories from (c) a broader set of beliefs and categories that are the outputs of structured learning mechanisms. Second, we describe the results of a modified "switched-at-birth" vignette study that we administered among children and adults in Puno (Peru), Yasawa (Fiji), and adults in the United States. This protocol permits us to study perceptions of prenatal and social transmission pathways for various traits and to differentiate the latter into vertical (i.e., parental) versus horizontal (i.e., peer) cultural influence. These lines of evidence suggest that people use all three mechanisms to reason about the distribution of traits in the population. Participants at all three sites develop expectations that morphological traits are under prenatal influence, and that belief traits are more culturally influenced. On the other hand, each population holds culturally specific beliefs about the degree of social influence on non-morphological traits and about the degree of vertical transmission-with only participants in the United States expecting parents to have much social influence over their children. We reinterpret people's differentiation of trait transmission pathways in light of humans' evolutionary history as a cultural species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cristina Moya
- Department of Population Health, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Keppel St., London, WC1E 7HT, UK
| | - Robert Boyd
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, PO Box 87402, Tempe, AZ 85287
| | - Joseph Henrich
- Departments of Psychology and Economics, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, V6T 1Z1, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Pietraszewski D, Curry OS, Petersen MB, Cosmides L, Tooby J. Constituents of political cognition: Race, party politics, and the alliance detection system. Cognition 2015; 140:24-39. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/04/2014] [Revised: 03/10/2015] [Accepted: 03/12/2015] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|