1
|
Rayce SB, Okholm GT, Flensborg-Madsen T. Mobile device screen time is associated with poorer language development among toddlers: results from a large-scale survey. BMC Public Health 2024; 24:1050. [PMID: 38622610 PMCID: PMC11020890 DOI: 10.1186/s12889-024-18447-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/13/2023] [Accepted: 03/26/2024] [Indexed: 04/17/2024] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Despite young children's widespread use of mobile devices, little research exists on this use and its association with children's language development. The aim of this study was to examine the associations between mobile device screen time and language comprehension and expressive language skills. An additional aim was to examine whether three factors related to the domestic learning environment modify the associations. METHODS The study uses data from the Danish large-scale survey TRACES among two- and three-year-old children (n = 31,125). Mobile device screen time was measured as time spent on mobile devices on a normal day. Measurement of language comprehension and expressive language skills was based on subscales from the Five to Fifteen Toddlers questionnaire. Multivariable linear regression was used to examine the association between child mobile device screen time and language development and logistic regression to examine the risk of experiencing significant language difficulties. Joint exposure analyses were used to examine the association between child mobile device screen time and language development difficulties in combination with three other factors related to the domestic learning environment: parental education, reading to the child and child TV/PC screen time. RESULTS High mobile device screen time of one hour or more per day was significantly associated with poorer language development scores and higher odds for both language comprehension difficulties (1-2 h: AOR = 1.30; ≥ 2 h: AOR = 1.42) and expressive language skills difficulties (1-2 h: AOR = 1.19; ≥ 2 h: AOR = 1.46). The results suggest that reading frequently to the child partly buffers the negative effect of high mobile device screen time on language comprehension difficulties but not on expressive language skills difficulties. No modifying effect of parental education and time spent by the child on TV/PC was found. CONCLUSIONS Mobile device screen time of one hour or more per day is associated with poorer language development among toddlers. Reading frequently to the child may have a buffering effect on language comprehension difficulties but not on expressive language skills difficulties.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Signe Boe Rayce
- VIVE- The Danish Center for Social Science Research, Herluf Trolles Gade 11, Copenhagen, 1052, Denmark.
| | - Gunhild Tidemann Okholm
- Center for Clinical Research and Prevention Bispebjerg and Frederiksberg Hospitals The Capital Region, Nordre Fasanvej 57, Frederiksberg, 2000, Denmark
- Unit of Medical Psychology, Department of Public Health, University of Copenhagen, Øster Farimagsgade 5, Copenhagen, 1353, Denmark
| | - Trine Flensborg-Madsen
- Unit of Medical Psychology, Department of Public Health, University of Copenhagen, Øster Farimagsgade 5, Copenhagen, 1353, Denmark
- The Danish National Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Studiestraede 6, Copenhagen, 1455, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Shirtcliff E, Hanson J, Phan J, Ruttle P, Pollak S. Hyper- and hypo-cortisol functioning in post-institutionalized adolescents: The role of severity of neglect and context. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2021; 124:105067. [PMID: 33302238 PMCID: PMC8757590 DOI: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.105067] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/24/2020] [Revised: 09/27/2020] [Accepted: 11/12/2020] [Indexed: 01/30/2023]
Abstract
Understanding the developmental timing of stress exposure may help inform mechanisms underlying how stress "gets under the skin" and influences the stress response system, including the HPA axis and its end-product cortisol. Early adversity may be particularly detrimental; however, it is difficult to disentangle the timing of adversity from its cumulative burden because there is typically high continuity between early and later adversity. Moreover, context and the different stressors inherent in various contexts may interact with stress exposure to influence psychophysiological functioning. To address this issue, we examined adolescents who had been reared in institutions and suffered neglect or social deprivation ranging from approximately six months to several years of life prior to adoption into U.S. homes. We focused on the stress hormone cortisol because it can reflect continued regulatory problems in youth, even years after youth transition to typical homes. We examined cortisol morning levels and diurnal rhythms across multiple contexts (home, school, lab) on 5 separate days in 41 post-institutionalized youth and 78 comparison youth. Employing hierarchical linear modeling, we found that when assessed in the lab, post-institutionalized (PI) youth displayed lower morning cortisol levels and flatter diurnal slopes than the control youth. Yet at home, PI youth displayed higher morning cortisol levels than the control youth. In addition to group effects, we also examined severity of early adversity and found that PI kids who had endured the most severe early adversity displayed lower home cortisol levels than controls. No significant predictors of diurnal cortisol on school days were identified. These data fit with the notion that the HPA axis is impacted by early adversity, even years after adoption, and with emerging theories that postulate that stress physiology calibrates within youth to help them adapt to their context. In the case of severe early adversity, the cost of such adaptation may not be desirable. It also highlights the important role of context when assessing HPA axis activity, particularly in post-institutionalized youth.
Collapse
|
3
|
Schröder M, Lüdtke J, Fux E, Izat Y, Bolten M, Gloger-Tippelt G, Suess GJ, Schmid M. Attachment disorder and attachment theory - Two sides of one medal or two different coins? Compr Psychiatry 2019; 95:152139. [PMID: 31706154 DOI: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2019.152139] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/27/2019] [Revised: 10/10/2019] [Accepted: 10/14/2019] [Indexed: 01/04/2023] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Currently, attachment quality and attachment disorder exist in parallel, but the mutual association is still insufficiently clarified. For policy makers and clinical experts, it can be difficult to differentiate between these constructs, but the distinction is crucial to develop mental-health services and effective treatment concepts. We aimed to investigate the association between attachment representations (AR) and attachment disorders (AD), including Reactive Attachment Disorder (RAD) and Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder (DSED) in children aged between 5 and 9. METHODS A total of 135 children aged between 5 and 9 years (M=7.17 years, SD=1.40, 63% male) and their primary caregivers participated in the study. Children were interviewed with the story stem method to assess AR, and the primary caregiver completed diagnostic interviews and questionnaires on mental disorders, AD, emotional and behavioral problems, and intelligence and development. RESULTS The prevalence of AR in children with AD was 28.6% for the 'secure' form of AR, 17.1% for the 'insecure-avoidant' form, 25.7% for the 'insecure-ambivalent' form, and 28.6% for the 'disorganized' form. Prevalences of the various AR forms did not differ statistically significantly, indicating that AR is conceptionally distinct from AD. Children with disorganized attachment scored significantly lower on language and intelligence skills than children with secure attachment. AD was significantly associated with a higher number of comorbidities, emotional and behavioral problems, and lower language skills. CONCLUSIONS Longitudinal studies using standardized assessment instruments are needed to systematically provide comparable and reliable empirical findings to improve current understanding of AR and AD as well as their etiological models.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martin Schröder
- Psychiatric University Clinics (UPK), Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (UPKKJ), University of Basel, Research Department, Schanzenstrasse 13, 4056, Basel, Switzerland; University of Lüneburg, Faculty of Education, Institute of Social work and Social Education, Universitätsallee 1, 21339, Lüneburg, Germany.
| | - Janine Lüdtke
- Psychiatric University Clinics (UPK), Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (UPKKJ), University of Basel, Research Department, Schanzenstrasse 13, 4056, Basel, Switzerland
| | - Elodie Fux
- Psychiatric University Clinics (UPK), Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (UPKKJ), University of Basel, Research Department, Schanzenstrasse 13, 4056, Basel, Switzerland
| | - Yonca Izat
- Vivantes Clinic Friedrichshain, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Berlin, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, Psychosomatic, Zadekstrasse 53, 12351, Berlin, Germany
| | - Margarete Bolten
- Psychiatric University Clinics (UPK), Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (UPKKJ), University of Basel, Research Department, Schanzenstrasse 13, 4056, Basel, Switzerland
| | | | - Gerhard J Suess
- Hamburg University of Applied Sciences, Faculty Business & Social Sciences, Department Social Work, Alexanderstraße 1, 20099, Hamburg, Germany
| | - Marc Schmid
- Psychiatric University Clinics (UPK), Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (UPKKJ), University of Basel, Research Department, Schanzenstrasse 13, 4056, Basel, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Melling R, Smethurst N. Taking care with attachment disorders and autistic-like traits: the potential significance of cognitive markers. EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY IN PRACTICE 2017. [DOI: 10.1080/02667363.2017.1306489] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Nicola Smethurst
- Educational Psychology Service, Trafford Council, Manchester, UK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Zeanah CH, Chesher T, Boris NW. Practice Parameter for the Assessment and Treatment of Children and Adolescents With Reactive Attachment Disorder and Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2016; 55:990-1003. [PMID: 27806867 DOI: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.08.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2016] [Accepted: 08/14/2016] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
This Practice Parameter is a revision of a previous Parameter addressing reactive attachment disorder that was published in 2005. It reviews the current status of reactive attachment disorder (RAD) and disinhibited social engagement disorder (DESD) with regard to assessment and treatment. Attachment is a central component of social and emotional development in early childhood, and disordered attachment is defined by specific patterns of abnormal social behavior in the context of "insufficient care" or social neglect. Assessment requires direct observation of the child in the context of his or her relationships with primary caregivers. Treatment requires establishing an attachment relationship for the child when none exists and ameliorating disturbed social relatedness with non-caregivers when evident.
Collapse
|