1
|
Stenhouse P, Moseby KE. Patch size and breeding status influence movement patterns in the threatened Malleefowl (
Leipoa ocellata
). AUSTRAL ECOL 2023. [DOI: 10.1111/aec.13311] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/29/2023]
|
2
|
Lin HY, Fagan WF, Jabin PE. Memory-driven movement model for periodic migrations. J Theor Biol 2020; 508:110486. [PMID: 32941915 DOI: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2020.110486] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2020] [Revised: 08/04/2020] [Accepted: 09/03/2020] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
We propose a model for memory-based movement of an individual. The dynamics are modeled by a stochastic differential equation, coupled with an eikonal equation, whose potential depends on the individual's memory and perception. Under a simple periodic environment, we discover that both long and short-term memory with appropriate time scales are essential for producing expected periodic migrations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hsin-Yi Lin
- Center for Scientific Computation and Mathematical Modeling (CSCAMM) and Department of Mathematics, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, United States.
| | - William F Fagan
- Department of Biology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, United States.
| | - Pierre-Emmanuel Jabin
- Center for Scientific Computation and Mathematical Modeling (CSCAMM) and Department of Mathematics, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Teitelbaum CS, Altizer S, Hall RJ. Movement rules determine nomadic species' responses to resource supplementation and degradation. J Anim Ecol 2020; 89:2644-2656. [PMID: 32783225 DOI: 10.1111/1365-2656.13318] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/20/2019] [Accepted: 07/27/2020] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
In environments that vary unpredictably, many animals are nomadic, moving in an irregular pattern that differs from year to year. Exploring the mechanisms of nomadic movement is needed to understand how animals survive in highly variable environments, and to predict behavioural and population responses to environmental change. We developed a network model to identify plausible mechanisms of nomadic animal movement by comparing the performance of multiple movement rules along a continuum from nomadism to residency. Using simulations and analytical results, we explored how different types of habitat modifications (that augment or decrease resource availability) might affect the abundance and movement rates of animals following each of these rules. Movement rules for which departure from patches depended on resource availability and/or competition performed almost equally well and better than residency or uninformed movement under most conditions, even though animals using each rule moved at substantially different rates. Habitat modifications that stabilized resources, either by resource supplementation or degradation, eroded the benefits of informed nomadic movements, particularly for movements based on resource availability alone. These results suggest that simple movement rules can explain nomadic animal movements and determine species' responses to environmental change. In particular, landscape stabilization and supplementation might be useful strategies for promoting populations of resident animals, but would be less beneficial for managing highly mobile species, many of which are threatened by habitat disruption and changes in climate.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Sonia Altizer
- Odum School of Ecology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
| | - Richard J Hall
- Odum School of Ecology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA.,Department of Infectious Diseases, College of Veterinary Medicine, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Kok EMA, Hogan JA, Piersma T. Experimental tests of a seasonally changing visual preference for habitat in a long‐distance migratory shorebird. Ethology 2020. [DOI: 10.1111/eth.13036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/07/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Eva M. A. Kok
- Department of Coastal Systems NIOZ Royal Netherlands Institute for Sea Research and Utrecht University Den Burg The Netherlands
- Conservation Ecology Group Groningen Institute for Evolutionary Life Sciences (GELIFES) University of Groningen Groningen The Netherlands
| | - Jerry A. Hogan
- Department of Psychology University of Toronto Toronto ON Canada
| | - Theunis Piersma
- Department of Coastal Systems NIOZ Royal Netherlands Institute for Sea Research and Utrecht University Den Burg The Netherlands
- Conservation Ecology Group Groningen Institute for Evolutionary Life Sciences (GELIFES) University of Groningen Groningen The Netherlands
- Rudi Drent Chair in Global Flyway Ecology Groningen Institute for Evolutionary Life Sciences (GELIFES) University of Groningen Groningen The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Oliveira KA, Berbert JM. Crossover in spreading behavior due to memory in population dynamics. Math Biosci 2020; 324:108346. [PMID: 32371030 DOI: 10.1016/j.mbs.2020.108346] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/02/2019] [Revised: 02/06/2020] [Accepted: 03/24/2020] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
The reaction-diffusion equation is one of the possible ways for modeling animal movement, where the reactive part stands for the population growth and the diffusive part for random dispersal of the population. However, a reaction-diffusion model may not represent all aspects of the spatial dynamics, because of the existence of distinct mechanisms that can affect the movement, such as spatial memory, which results in a bias for one direction of dispersal. This bias is modeled through an advective term on an advection-reaction-diffusion equation. Thus, considering the effects of memory on the population spread, we propose a model composed of a coupled partial differential equation system with two equations: one for the population dynamics and the other for the memory density distribution. For the population growth, we use either the exponential or logistic growth function. The analytic approach shows that for the exponential and logistic growth, the minimum traveling wave speeds are the same with or without memory dynamics in which the variation of memory is infinitesimal. From the numerical analysis, we explore how our parameters, memory, growth rate, and carrying capacity, affect the population redistribution. The combinations of these parameters result in a redistribution pattern of the population associated with either diffusive or superdiffusive and imply the dispersal is faster than the diffusion. Further, in the parameter-space defined by memory and growth rate, we have shown that memory is a factor that switches the dynamics between two spreading behaviors, one faster than the other.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karen A Oliveira
- Centro de Matemática, Computação e Cognição Universidade Federal do ABC, Santo André, SP, Brazil.
| | - Juliana M Berbert
- Centro de Matemática, Computação e Cognição Universidade Federal do ABC, Santo André, SP, Brazil.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Oudman T, Laland K, Ruxton G, Tombre I, Shimmings P, Prop J. Young Birds Switch but Old Birds Lead: How Barnacle Geese Adjust Migratory Habits to Environmental Change. Front Ecol Evol 2020. [DOI: 10.3389/fevo.2019.00502] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/26/2023] Open
|
7
|
Wells CR, Lethbridge M. Intensive and extensive movements of feral camels in central Australia. RANGELAND JOURNAL 2020. [DOI: 10.1071/rj19054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
A better understanding of the movement of feral dromedary camels (Camelus dromedarius) in Australia would be useful for planning removal operations (harvest or culling), because the pattern and scale of camel movement relates to the period they reside in a given area, and thus the search effort, timing and frequency of removal operations. From our results, we suspect that the dune direction influences how camels move across central Australia; particularly effects like the north–south longitudinal dune systems in the Simpson Desert, which appeared to elongate camel movement in the same direction as the dunes. We called this movement anisotropy. Research suggests camel movement in Australia is not migratory but partially cyclic, with two distinctive movement patterns. Our study investigated this further by using satellite tracking data from 54 camels in central Australia, recorded between 2007 and 2016. The mean tracking period for each animal was 363.9 days (s.e.m.=44.1 days). We used a method labelled multi-scale partitioning to test for changes in movement behaviour and partitioned more localised intensive movements within utilisation areas, from larger-scale movement, called ranging. This involved analysing the proximity of movement trajectories to other nearby trajectories of the same animal over time. We also used Dynamic Brownian Bridges Movement Models, which consider the relationship of consecutive locations to determine the areas of utilisation. The mean utilisation area and duration of a camel (n=658 areas) was found to be 342.6km2 (s.e.m.=33.2km2) over 23.5 days (s.e.m.=1.6 days), and the mean ranging distance (n=611 ranging paths) was a 45.1km (s.e.m.=2.0km) path over 3.1 days (s.e.m.=0.1 days).
Collapse
|
8
|
Merkle JA, Sawyer H, Monteith KL, Dwinnell SPH, Fralick GL, Kauffman MJ. Spatial memory shapes migration and its benefits: evidence from a large herbivore. Ecol Lett 2019; 22:1797-1805. [DOI: 10.1111/ele.13362] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2019] [Revised: 03/30/2019] [Accepted: 07/11/2019] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Jerod A. Merkle
- Department of Zoology and Physiology University of Wyoming Laramie WY USA
| | - Hall Sawyer
- Western Ecosystems Technology, Inc. Laramie WY USA
| | - Kevin L. Monteith
- Department of Zoology and Physiology University of Wyoming Laramie WY USA
- Haub School of Environment and Natural Resources Wyoming Cooperative Fish and Wildlife Research Unit Laramie WY USA
| | | | | | - Matthew J. Kauffman
- US Geological Survey, Wyoming Cooperative Fish and Wildlife Research Unit, Department of Zoology and Physiology University of Wyoming Laramie WY USA
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Teitelbaum CS, Mueller T. Beyond Migration: Causes and Consequences of Nomadic Animal Movements. Trends Ecol Evol 2019; 34:569-581. [PMID: 30885413 DOI: 10.1016/j.tree.2019.02.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 67] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/19/2018] [Revised: 02/08/2019] [Accepted: 02/12/2019] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Recent advances in animal tracking reveal that many species display irregular movements that do not fall into classical categories of movement patterns such as range residency or migration. Here, we develop a unifying framework that distinguishes these nomadic movements based on their patterns, drivers, and mechanisms. Though they occur in diverse taxa and geographic regions, nomadic movements are united by both their underlying environmental drivers, mainly environmental stochasticity, and the resulting irregular, far-ranging movement patterns. The framework further classifies types of nomadic movements, including full, seasonal, phase, irruptive, and partial nomadism. Nomadic movements can have unique effects on populations, communities, and ecosystems, most notably providing intermittent disturbances and novel introductions of propagules.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claire S Teitelbaum
- Odum School of Ecology, University of Georgia, 140 E Green St., Athens, GA 30602, USA. https://twitter.com/@cs_teitelbaum
| | - Thomas Mueller
- Department of Biological Sciences, Goethe-University Frankfurt and Senckenberg Biodiversity and Climate Research Centre (SBiK-F), Senckenberganlage 25, 60325 Frankfurt, Germany. https://twitter.com/@secnkenberg
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Migrating whales depend on memory to exploit reliable resources. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2019; 116:5217-5219. [PMID: 30804183 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1901803116] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
|
11
|
Berbert JM, Lewis MA. Superdiffusivity due to resource depletion in random searches. ECOLOGICAL COMPLEXITY 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ecocom.2017.11.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
12
|
Riotte-Lambert L, Benhamou S, Bonenfant C, Chamaillé-Jammes S. Spatial memory shapes density dependence in population dynamics. Proc Biol Sci 2017; 284:20171411. [PMID: 29167358 PMCID: PMC5719166 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2017.1411] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2017] [Accepted: 10/20/2017] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Most population dynamics studies assume that individuals use space uniformly, and thus mix well spatially. In numerous species, however, individuals do not move randomly, but use spatial memory to visit renewable resource patches repeatedly. To understand the extent to which memory-based foraging movement may affect density-dependent population dynamics through its impact on competition, we developed a spatially explicit, individual-based movement model where reproduction and death are functions of foraging efficiency. We compared the dynamics of populations of with- and without-memory individuals. We showed that memory-based movement leads to a higher population size at equilibrium, to a higher depletion of the environment, to a marked discrepancy between the global (i.e. measured at the population level) and local (i.e. measured at the individual level) intensities of competition, and to a nonlinear density dependence. These results call for a deeper investigation of the impact of individual movement strategies and cognitive abilities on population dynamics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Louise Riotte-Lambert
- Centre d'Ecologie Fonctionnelle et Evolutive, CNRS, Université de Montpellier, 1919 Route de Mende, 34293 Montpellier Cedex 5, France
- Institute of Biodiversity, Animal Health and Comparative Medicine, College of Medical, Veterinary, and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Graham Kerr Building, Glasgow G12 8QQ, UK
| | - Simon Benhamou
- Centre d'Ecologie Fonctionnelle et Evolutive, CNRS, Université de Montpellier, 1919 Route de Mende, 34293 Montpellier Cedex 5, France
| | - Christophe Bonenfant
- Laboratoire de Biométrie et Biologie Évolutive, CNRS, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, - Bat. Grégor Mendel, 43 bd du 11 novembre 1918, 69622 Villeurbanne cedex, France
| | - Simon Chamaillé-Jammes
- Centre d'Ecologie Fonctionnelle et Evolutive, CNRS, Université de Montpellier, 1919 Route de Mende, 34293 Montpellier Cedex 5, France
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Lethbridge MR. Insights into feral goat movement in Australia using dynamic Brownian Bridges for movement analysis. RANGELAND JOURNAL 2016. [DOI: 10.1071/rj15024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Movement analyses were conducted for 50 goats across southern Australia using GPS satellite collars. A radio or satellite-tracked animal used to direct culling operations is generally called a ‘Judas’ animal. Goats used as ‘Judas’ animals in control operations were compared with non-‘Judas’ goats in the states of South Australia and Victoria, respectively. Their movement in two land systems were also compared. Dynamic Brownian Bridges Movement Models were used to calculate home ranges (95% utilisation areas). Changes in movement behaviour were identified to partition sedentary behaviour from long-distance movement events, defined here as ranging. Eleven goats exhibited ranging behaviour and moved from 9 to 33 km between their home ranges. After partitioning, their home ranges varied from 1.97 to 223.8 km2. In this study in the Southern Australian Mallee regions, non-‘Judas’ goats had significantly smaller home ranges than ‘Judas’ goats. However, no significant differences were found in the ranging distances between non-‘Judas’ goats and ‘Judas’ goats. Understanding these two distinct forms of goat movement is important in the planning and budgeting of removal operations. To demonstrate this a simple goat management decision tool is used to illustrate the biases that can result in the expected hours of removal operations when the assumptions about goat movement are ill-defined.
Collapse
|
14
|
Colombo EH, Anteneodo C. Metapopulation dynamics in a complex ecological landscape. PHYSICAL REVIEW. E, STATISTICAL, NONLINEAR, AND SOFT MATTER PHYSICS 2015; 92:022714. [PMID: 26382439 DOI: 10.1103/physreve.92.022714] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2015] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
We propose a general model to study the interplay between spatial dispersal and environment spatiotemporal fluctuations in metapopulation dynamics. An ecological landscape of favorable patches is generated like a Lévy dust, which allows to build a range of patterns, from dispersed to clustered ones. Locally, the dynamics is driven by a canonical model for the time evolution of the population density, consisting of a logistic expression plus multiplicative noises. Spatial coupling is introduced by means of two spreading mechanisms: diffusion and selective dispersal driven by patch suitability. We focus on the long-time population size as a function of habitat configurations, environment fluctuations, and coupling schemes. We obtain the conditions, that the spatial distribution of favorable patches and the coupling mechanisms must fulfill, to grant population survival. The fundamental phenomenon that we observe is the positive feedback between environment fluctuations and spatial spread preventing extinction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E H Colombo
- Department of Physics, PUC-Rio, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| | - C Anteneodo
- Department of Physics, PUC-Rio, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
- Institute of Science and Technology for Complex Systems, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Wolfe BW, Lowe CG. Movement patterns, habitat use and site fidelity of the white croaker (Genyonemus lineatus) in the Palos Verdes Superfund Site, Los Angeles, California. MARINE ENVIRONMENTAL RESEARCH 2015; 109:69-80. [PMID: 26107933 DOI: 10.1016/j.marenvres.2015.06.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/04/2015] [Revised: 06/03/2015] [Accepted: 06/05/2015] [Indexed: 05/25/2023]
Abstract
White croaker (Genyonemus lineatus family: Sciaenidae) are a schooling, benthic foraging fish historically associated with soft sediment and wastewater outfalls in southern California. While they are often used as an indicator species due to their high organochlorine contaminant loads, little is known of their movements in relation to contaminated habitats. A Vemco Positioning System acoustic telemetry array was used to collect fine-scale movement data and characterize the site fidelity, area use, and dispersal of 83 white croaker on the Palos Verdes Shelf Superfund Site, California over 27 months. White croaker generally demonstrated low residency and recurrence to the Palos Verdes Shelf, and were observed to be largely nomadic. However, individual behavior was highly variable. Although the entire monitored shelf was visited by tagged white croaker, habitats in 0-200 m proximity to wastewater outfalls and between 25 and 35 m depth were used most frequently. Approximately half of white croaker migrated into Los Angeles and Long Beach Harbors; areas where they may be targeted by subsistence fishers. A model framework for incorporating fish movement data into contaminant exposure estimates was developed to better understanding organochlorine contaminant exposure for planning future remediation and monitoring.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barrett W Wolfe
- Dept. of Biological Sciences, California State University Long Beach, 1250 Bellflower Blvd., Long Beach, CA 90840, USA.
| | - Christopher G Lowe
- Dept. of Biological Sciences, California State University Long Beach, 1250 Bellflower Blvd., Long Beach, CA 90840, USA
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Vincenot CE, Mazzoleni S, Moriya K, Cartenì F, Giannino F. How spatial resource distribution and memory impact foraging success: A hybrid model and mechanistic index. ECOLOGICAL COMPLEXITY 2015. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ecocom.2015.03.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
|
17
|
McEvoy JF, Roshier DA, Ribot RFH, Bennett ATD. Proximate cues to phases of movement in a highly dispersive waterfowl, Anas superciliosa. MOVEMENT ECOLOGY 2015; 3:21. [PMID: 26331024 PMCID: PMC4556217 DOI: 10.1186/s40462-015-0048-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/20/2015] [Accepted: 06/25/2015] [Indexed: 05/22/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Waterfowl can exploit distant ephemeral wetlands in arid environments and provide valuable insights into the response of birds to rapid environmental change, and behavioural flexibility of avian movements. Currently much of our understanding of behavioural flexibility of avian movement comes from studies of migration in seasonally predictable biomes in the northern hemisphere. We used GPS transmitters to track 20 Pacific black duck (Anas superciliosa) in arid central Australia. We exploited La Niña conditions that brought extensive flooding, so allowing a rare opportunity to investigate how weather and other environmental factors predict initiation of long distance movement toward freshly flooded habitats. We employed behavioural change point analysis to identify three phases of movement: sedentary, exploratory and long distance oriented movement. We then used random forest models to determine the ability of meteorological and remote sensed landscape variables to predict initiation of these phases. RESULTS We found that initiation of exploratory movement phases is influenced by fluctuations in local weather conditions and accumulated rainfall in the landscape. Initiation of long distance movement phases was found to be highly individualistic with minor influence from local weather conditions. CONCLUSIONS Our study reveals how individuals utilise local conditions to respond to changes in resource distribution at broad scales. Our findings suggest that individual movement decisions of dispersive birds are informed by the integration of multiple weather cues operating at different temporal and spatial scales.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John F. McEvoy
- />Centre for Integrative Ecology, Deakin University, Locked Bag 20000, Geelong, VIC 3220 Australia
- />Zoology, School of Environmental and Rural Science, University of New England, Armidale, NSW 2351 Australia
| | - David A. Roshier
- />Australian Wildlife Conservancy, PO Box 6621, Halifax Street, Adelaide, SA 5000 Australia
| | - Raoul F. H. Ribot
- />Centre for Integrative Ecology, Deakin University, Locked Bag 20000, Geelong, VIC 3220 Australia
| | - Andy T. D. Bennett
- />Centre for Integrative Ecology, Deakin University, Locked Bag 20000, Geelong, VIC 3220 Australia
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Giuggioli L, Kenkre VM. Consequences of animal interactions on their dynamics: emergence of home ranges and territoriality. MOVEMENT ECOLOGY 2014; 2:20. [PMID: 25709829 PMCID: PMC4337768 DOI: 10.1186/s40462-014-0020-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2014] [Accepted: 08/08/2014] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
Animal spacing has important implications for population abundance, species demography and the environment. Mechanisms underlying spatial segregation have their roots in the characteristics of the animals, their mutual interaction and their response, collective as well as individual, to environmental variables. This review describes how the combination of these factors shapes the patterns we observe and presents a practical, usable framework for the analysis of movement data in confined spaces. The basis of the framework is the theory of interacting random walks and the mathematical description of out-of-equilibrium systems. Although our focus is on modelling and interpreting animal home ranges and territories in vertebrates, we believe further studies on invertebrates may also help to answer questions and resolve unanswered puzzles that are still inaccessible to experimental investigation in vertebrate species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luca Giuggioli
- />Bristol Centre for Complexity Sciences, Department of Engineering Mathematics and School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, BS8 1UB UK
| | - V M Kenkre
- />Consortium of the Americas for Interdisciplinary Science and Department of Physics and Astronomy, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, 87131 New Mexico USA
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Mettke-Hofmann C. Cognitive ecology: ecological factors, life-styles, and cognition. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2014; 5:345-60. [PMID: 26308568 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1289] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2013] [Revised: 01/30/2014] [Accepted: 02/05/2014] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
UNLABELLED Cognitive ecology integrates cognition, ecology, and neurobiology in one topic and has recently broadened into an exciting diversity of themes covering the entire range of cognition and ecological conditions. The review identifies three major environmental factors interacting with cognition: environmental variation (predictable and unpredictable), environmental complexity and predation. Generally, variable environments favor cognitive abilities such as exploration, learning, innovation, memory and also result in larger brains as compared to stable environments. Likewise, cognition is enhanced in complex versus simple environments, whereas the relationship between predation and cognitive abilities can be positive or negative. However, organisms have often evolved entire life-styles (e.g., residency versus migration, food-caching versus noncaching, generalism versus specialism) to deal with these environmental factors. Considering cognition within this framework provides a much more diverse picture of how cognitive abilities evolved in conjunction with other adaptations to environmental challenges. This integrated approach identifies gaps of knowledge and allows the formulation of hypotheses for future testing. Several recently emerged approaches study cognitive abilities at a new and in part highly integrated level. For example, the effect that environment has on the development of cognitive abilities during ontogeny will improve our understanding about cause and effect and gene-environment interactions. Together with two recently emerged highly integrative approaches that link personality and pace-of-life syndromes with cognitive ecology these new directions will improve insight how cognition is interlinked with other major organizational processes. For further resources related to this article, please visit the WIREs website. CONFLICT OF INTEREST The author has declared no conflicts of interest for this article.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claudia Mettke-Hofmann
- School of Natural Sciences and Psychology, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Abstract
Collective animal behavior studies have led the way in developing models that account for a large number of individuals, but mostly have considered situations in which alignment and attraction play a key role, such as in schooling and flocking. By quantifying how animals react to one another's presence, when interaction is via conspecific avoidance rather than alignment or attraction, we present a mechanistic insight that enables us to link individual behavior and space use patterns. As animals respond to both current and past positions of their neighbors, the assumption that the relative location of individuals is statistically and history independent is not tenable, underscoring the limitations of traditional space use studies. We move beyond that assumption by constructing a framework to analyze spatial segregation of mobile animals when neighbor proximity may elicit a retreat, and by linking conspecific encounter rate to history-dependent avoidance behavior. Our approach rests on the knowledge that animals communicate by modifying the environment in which they live, providing a method to analyze social cohesion as stigmergy, a form of mediated animal-animal interaction. By considering a population of animals that mark the terrain as they move, we predict how the spatiotemporal patterns that emerge depend on the degree of stigmergy of the interaction processes. We find in particular that nonlocal decision rules may generate a nonmonotonic dependence of the animal encounter rate as a function of the tendency to retreat from locations recently visited by other conspecifics, which has fundamental implications for epidemic disease spread and animal sociality.
Collapse
|
21
|
Berbert JM, González RS, Martinez AS. Ergodic crossover in partially self-avoiding stochastic walks. PHYSICAL REVIEW. E, STATISTICAL, NONLINEAR, AND SOFT MATTER PHYSICS 2013; 88:032119. [PMID: 24125225 DOI: 10.1103/physreve.88.032119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2013] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
Consider a one-dimensional environment with N randomly distributed sites. An agent explores this random medium moving deterministically with a spatial memory μ. A crossover from local to global exploration occurs in one dimension at a well-defined memory value μ_{1}=log_{2}N. In its stochastic version, the dynamics is ruled by the memory and by temperature T, which affects the hopping displacement. This dynamics also shows a crossover in one dimension, obtained computationally, between exploration schemes, characterized yet by the trajectory size (N_{p}) (aging effect). In this paper we provide an analytical approach considering the modified stochastic version where the parameter T plays the role of a maximum hopping distance. This modification allows us to obtain a general analytical expression for the crossover, as a function of the parameters μ, T, and N_{p}. Differently from what has been proposed by previous studies, we find that the crossover occurs in any dimension d. These results have been validated by numerical experiments and may be of great value for fixing optimal parameters in search algorithms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juliana M Berbert
- Instituto de Física Teórica (IFT), Universidade Estadual Paulista-UNESP, Caixa Postal 70532-2, Cep 01156-970 São Paulo, SP, Brazil
| | | | | |
Collapse
|