1
|
Sah WH. The use of referring expressions in Mandarin-speaking children with and without developmental language disorder. CLINICAL LINGUISTICS & PHONETICS 2024; 38:40-63. [PMID: 36594504 DOI: 10.1080/02699206.2022.2157751] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/09/2021] [Revised: 09/14/2022] [Accepted: 11/27/2022] [Indexed: 06/17/2023]
Abstract
This study investigated the use of referring expressions in Mandarin-speaking children with and without developmental language disorder (DLD). The data consist of narratives elicited from 18 children with DLD and 18 typically-developing (TD) children matched on chronological age. Participants' referring expressions were analysed in terms of referential form, function, and adequacy. The overall results of form - function mappings showed that the two groups of children were quite similar in their referential choices, indicating that both groups were sensitive to function constraints and listener needs. On the other hand, children with DLD were less successful than TD children in producing adequate subsequent mentions, suggesting that referential adequacy would be useful for identifying areas of difficulty children with DLD would have in referencing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wen Hui Sah
- Department of English, National Chengchi University, Taipei City, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Xue J, Zhuo J, Li P, Liu J, Zhao J. Characterizing macro- and micro-structures of narrative skills for Mandarin-speaking school-age children with specific language impairment. JOURNAL OF COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2022; 96:106199. [PMID: 35227976 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2022.106199] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/06/2020] [Revised: 01/25/2022] [Accepted: 02/22/2022] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Little is known about the narrative skills of Mandarin-speaking children with specific language impairment (SLI). This study was aimed to capture the features of narratives for school-age Mandarin-speaking children with SLI. METHODS Oral narrative samples by 55 Mandarin-speaking children with SLI [higher grades, recruited from Grade 4-5, n = 26, Mage = 11.00 years, SD = 0.56; lower grades, recruited from Grade 1-3, n = 29, Mage = 8.05 years, SD = 0.89] were compared with typically developing (TD) children on macro- and micro-structures of narratives. RESULTS The results revealed that across grades, for macrostructure, children with SLI lagged behind TD children in narrative pattern scores. For microstructure, children with SLI were constrained in the total number of words, the total number of different words, and the mean length of utterance. They also used fewer serial verb constructions, clausal objects, and temporality conjunctions. They were less adequate in the three referential functions of introduction, maintenance, and switch. Furthermore, the results showed that older children demonstrated higher narrative pattern scores, longer MLU, and higher proportions of conjunctions and referential forms of switch. CONCLUSIONS These results suggest that both macro- and micro-structure measures in narratives are sensitive to linguistic difficulties for children with SLI and that some of these measures are sensitive to grade growth. The findings shed light on the assessment and developmental changes of school-age Mandarin-speaking children with SLI and TD children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jin Xue
- University of Science and Technology Beijing, School of Foreign Studies, 30 Xueyuan Road, Haidian District, Beijing 100083, China.
| | - Junjing Zhuo
- University of Science and Technology Beijing, School of Foreign Studies, 30 Xueyuan Road, Haidian District, Beijing 100083, China
| | - Panpan Li
- University of Science and Technology Beijing, School of Foreign Studies, 30 Xueyuan Road, Haidian District, Beijing 100083, China
| | - Juan Liu
- University of Science and Technology Beijing, School of Foreign Studies, 30 Xueyuan Road, Haidian District, Beijing 100083, China
| | - Jianrong Zhao
- University of Science and Technology Beijing, School of Foreign Studies, 30 Xueyuan Road, Haidian District, Beijing 100083, China
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Coelho RM, Drummond C, Mota NB, Erthal P, Bernardes G, Lima G, Molina R, Sudo FK, Tannock R, Mattos P. Network analysis of narrative discourse and attention-deficit hyperactivity symptoms in adults. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0245113. [PMID: 33826632 PMCID: PMC8026017 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/13/2020] [Accepted: 12/23/2020] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous research investigating language in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has demonstrated several deficits in many aspects. However, no previous study employed quantitative methodology providing objective measures that could be compared among different studies with diverse samples. To fill this gap, we used network analysis to investigate how ADHD symptomatology impacts narrative discourse, a complex linguistic task considered to be an ecological measure of language. Fifty-eight adults (34 females and 24 males) with a mean age of 26 years old and a mean of 17 years of educational level were administered the Adult Self-Rating Scale for ADHD symptomatology. They also completed the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, the Beck Depression Inventory and the Urgency, Premeditation, Perseverance, Sensation Seeking Behavior Scale. Intelligence quotient was calculated. Individuals were asked to tell a story based on a wordless picture book. Speech was recorded and transcribed as an input to SpeechGraphs software. Parameters were total number of words (TNW), number of loops of one node (L1), repeated edges (RE), largest strongly connected component (LSC) and average shortest path (ASP). Verbosity was controlled. Statistical analysis was corrected for multiples comparisons and partial correlations were performed for confounding variables. After controlling for anxiety, depression, IQ, and impulsiveness ADHD symptomatology was positively correlated with L1 and negatively correlated with LSC. TNW was positively correlated with ADHD symptoms. In a subdomain analysis, both inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity were negatively correlated with LSC. Only hyperactivity-impulsivity positively correlated with TNW and L1. Results indicated a correlation between ADHD symptoms and lower connectedness in narrative discourse (as indicated by higher L1 and lower LSC), as well as higher total number of words (TNW). Our results suggest that the higher the number of ADHD symptoms, the less connectivity among words, and a higher number of words in narrative discourse.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Cláudia Drummond
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil.,Department of speech and hearing pathology, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | | | - Pilar Erthal
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Gabriel Bernardes
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Gabriel Lima
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Raquel Molina
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Felipe Kenji Sudo
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Rosemary Tannock
- Research Institute, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Canada
| | - Paulo Mattos
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil.,Institute of Psychiatry, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Chen L, Chen W, Zhou Q. The use of evaluative expressions in the narratives of adolescents with a history of SLI. CLINICAL LINGUISTICS & PHONETICS 2019; 33:1086-1101. [PMID: 31017004 DOI: 10.1080/02699206.2019.1606283] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2019] [Revised: 04/04/2019] [Accepted: 04/08/2019] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
Typically, young children diagnosed with specific language impairment (SLI; also called Developmental language disorder, DLD) have been found to show weakness in narrative development, and such weakness has been judged to be unlikely to resolve over time. However, developmental outcomes for adolescents with a prior SLI diagnosis have not been thoroughly studied. In particular, studies on the evaluative aspects of narratives of such persons are almost non-existent. Here we examine the question whether narrative evaluation of adolescents formerly diagnosed with SLI remains problematic into adolescence or comes to resemble more typical narrative performance. We compare the use of evaluative expressions in the narratives of 19 adolescents diagnosed with SLI against 19 typically language developing (TLD) peers. Spoken narratives by the SLI group (Mean age = 14.3; SD = 0.64) and TLD group (Mean age = 14.5; SD = 0.84) using the wordless picture storybook "Frog, where are you?" ( https://childes.talkbank.org/access/Clinical-MOR/Conti/Conti4.html ) were analysed. Each narrative was coded for evaluative clauses, types of evaluative devices (frames of mind, character speech, hedges, negatives, and causal connectives), as well as evaluative perspectives (global vs. local). Although the quantitative analysis did not reveal any significant differences between the two groups, the qualitative analysis showed certain notable strengths in the narrative skills of the focal group of SLI adolescents. Results suggest that adolescents with a history of SLI may approximate TLD narrative skill in evaluating their own narratives, but additional work with more sensitive measures applied at intervals are needed to illuminate developmental pathways of narrative production and of evaluative capacities in adolescents formerly diagnosed with SLI.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Liang Chen
- Communication Sciences and Special Education, University of Georgia , Athens , Georgia , USA
| | - Wei Chen
- Department of Chinese, Defense Language Institute , Monterey , California , USA
| | - Qin Zhou
- School of Foreign Languages, Central South University , Changsha , China
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Fichman S, Altman C. Referential Cohesion in the Narratives of Bilingual and Monolingual Children With Typically Developing Language and With Specific Language Impairment. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2019; 62:123-142. [PMID: 30950755 DOI: 10.1044/2018_jslhr-l-18-0054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
Purpose The study explores referential cohesion in the narratives of bilingual preschool children with typical language development (TLD) and with specific language impairment (SLI). Referential cohesion requires integration of multiple discourse factors and is expected to pose a challenge for children with bilingual SLI due to weak proficiency in both languages. Method Narratives were elicited from 45 bilinguals speaking Russian as the home language (L1) and Hebrew as the societal language (L2; 15 with SLI), 20 Hebrew-speaking monolinguals (10 with SLI), and 20 Russian-speaking monolinguals (10 with SLI) using a story retelling procedure. Bilinguals were tested in both languages. Analyses examined the effect of impairment (SLI vs. TLD) in bilinguals and monolinguals. Language effects were examined in cross-language comparisons of bilinguals (L1 vs. L2) and in differences between monolingual groups (Russian vs. Hebrew speakers) for the use of referential expressions. Results Bilingual children with SLI used a higher proportion of pronouns for character introduction and had fewer pronouns, which have been described as "adequate" ( Colozzo & Whitely, 2014 ) than bilingual children with TLD. No language effect emerged for bilinguals, who performed similarly in their L1 and L2, but a significant cross-linguistic difference emerged in the monolingual data: Russian-speaking children mainly used nouns to introduce and maintain characters, whereas Hebrew-speaking children mainly used pronouns for introduction and maintenance of characters. Conclusion The difficulty of children with SLI in creating a referential connection between a pronoun and a noun phrase is discussed in light of the interaction of local and global processes in narratives, which is shown to be weaker in children with SLI.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sveta Fichman
- Department of English Literature and Linguistics, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
| | - Carmit Altman
- School of Education, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Koutsoftas AD, Petersen V. Written cohesion in children with and without language learning disabilities. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2017; 52:612-625. [PMID: 28035711 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12306] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2015] [Revised: 10/15/2016] [Accepted: 10/30/2016] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Cohesion refers to the linguistic elements of discourse that contribute to its continuity and is an important element to consider as part of written language intervention, especially in children with language learning disabilities (LLD). There is substantial evidence that children with LLD perform more poorly than typically developing (TD) peers on measures of cohesion in spoken language and on written transcription measures; however, there is far less research comparing groups on cohesion as a measure of written language across genres. AIMS The current study addresses this gap through the following two aims. First, to describe and compare cohesion in narrative and expository writing samples of children with and without language learning disabilities. Second, to relate measures of cohesion to written transcription and translation measures, oral language, and writing quality. METHODS & PROCEDURES Fifty intermediate-grade children produced one narrative and one expository writing sample from which measures of written cohesion were obtained. These included the frequency, adequacy and complexity of referential and conjunctive ties. OUTCOMES & RESULTS Expository samples resulted in more complex cohesive ties and children with TD used more complex ties than peers with LLD. Different relationships among cohesion measures and writing were observed for narrative verse expository samples. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS Findings from this study demonstrate cohesion as a discourse-level measure of written transcription and how the use of cohesion can vary by genre and group (LLD, TD). Clinical implications for assessment, intervention, and future research are provided.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anthony D Koutsoftas
- Department of Speech Language Pathology, School of Health and Medical Sciences, Seton Hall University, South Orange, NJ, USA
| | - Victoria Petersen
- Department of Speech Language Pathology, School of Health and Medical Sciences, Seton Hall University, South Orange, NJ, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Mäkinen L, Loukusa S, Laukkanen P, Leinonen E, Kunnari S. Linguistic and pragmatic aspects of narration in Finnish typically developing children and children with specific language impairment. CLINICAL LINGUISTICS & PHONETICS 2014; 28:413-427. [PMID: 24446795 DOI: 10.3109/02699206.2013.875592] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
This study investigates narratives of Finnish children with specific language impairment (SLI) from linguistic and pragmatic perspectives, in order to get a comprehensive overview of these children's narrative abilities. Nineteen children with SLI (mean age 6;1 years) and 19 typically developing age-matched children participated in the study. Their picture-elicited narrations were analysed for linguistic productivity and complexity, grammatical and referential accuracy, event content, the use of mental state expressions and narrative comprehension. Children with SLI showed difficulties in every aspect of narration in comparison to their peers. Only one measure of productivity, the number of communication units, did not reach statistical significance. Not only was linguistic structure fragile but also pragmatic aspects of storytelling (referencing, event content, mental state expressions and inferencing) were demanding for children with SLI. Results suggest that pragmatic aspects of narration should be taken into account more often when assessing narrative abilities of children with SLI.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leena Mäkinen
- Faculty of Humanities, Child Language Research Center, University of Oulu , Oulu , Finland and
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Vandewalle E, Boets B, Boons T, Ghesquière P, Zink I. Oral language and narrative skills in children with specific language impairment with and without literacy delay: a three-year longitudinal study. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2012; 33:1857-1870. [PMID: 22699257 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.05.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2012] [Accepted: 05/01/2012] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
This longitudinal study compared the development of oral language and more specifically narrative skills (storytelling and story retelling) in children with specific language impairment (SLI) with and without literacy delay. Therefore, 18 children with SLI and 18 matched controls with normal literacy were followed from the last year of kindergarten (mean age=5 years 5 months) until the beginning of grade 3 (mean age=8 years 1 month). Oral language tests measuring vocabulary, morphology, sentence and text comprehension and narrative skills were administered yearly. Based on first and third grade reading and spelling achievement, both groups were divided into a group with and a group without literacy problems. Results showed that the children with SLI and literacy delay had persistent oral language problems across all assessed language domains. The children with SLI and normal literacy skills scored also persistently low on vocabulary, morphology and story retelling skills. Only on listening comprehension and storytelling, they evolved towards the level of the control group. In conclusion, oral language skills in children with SLI and normal literacy skills remained in general poor, despite their intact literacy development during the first years of literacy instruction. Only for listening comprehension and storytelling, they improved, probably as a result of more print exposure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ellen Vandewalle
- ExpORL, Department of Neurosciences, Faculty of Medicine, KU Leuven, Belgium.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Schneider P, Hayward D. Who Does What to Whom: Introduction of Referents in Children’s Storytelling From Pictures. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 2010; 41:459-73. [DOI: 10.1044/0161-1461(2010/09-0040)] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose
This article describes the development of a measure, called First Mentions (FM), that can be used to evaluate the referring expressions that children use to introduce characters and objects when telling a story.
Method
Participants were 377 children ages 4 to 9 years (300 with typical development, 77 with language impairment) who told stories while viewing 6 picture sets. Their first mentions of 8 characters and 6 objects were scored as fully adequate, partially adequate, inadequate, or not mentioned. Total FM scores were compared across age and language groups.
Results
There were significant differences for age and language status, as well as a significant Age × Language interaction. Within each age group except age 9, children in the typical development group attained higher scores than children in the group with language impairment.
Conclusion
These results suggest that the FM measure is a useful tool for identifying whether a child has a problem with introducing referents in stories.
Collapse
|