1
|
Jurkiewicz T, Vialatte A, Yeshurun Y, Pisella L. Attentional modulation of peripheral pointing hypometria in healthy participants: An insight into optic ataxia? Neuropsychologia 2025; 208:109084. [PMID: 39890056 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2025.109084] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2024] [Revised: 12/24/2024] [Accepted: 01/28/2025] [Indexed: 02/03/2025]
Abstract
Damage to the superior parietal lobule and intraparietal sulcus (SPL-IPS) causes optic ataxia (OA), characterized by pathological gaze-centered hypometric pointing to targets in the affected peripheral visual field. The SPL-IPS is also involved in covert attention. Here, we investigated the possible link between attention and action. This study investigated the effect of attention on pointing performance in healthy participants and two OA patients. In invalid trials, targets appeared unpredictably across different visual fields and eccentricities. Valid trials involved cued targets at specific locations. The first experiment used a central cue with 75% validity, the second used a peripheral cue with 50% validity. The effect of attention on pointing variability (noise) or time was expected as a confirmation of cueing efficiency. Critically, if OA reflects an attentional deficit, then healthy participants, in the invalid condition (without attention), were expected to produce the gaze-centered hypometric pointing bias characteristic of OA. RESULTS: revealed main effects of validity on pointing biases in all participants with central predictive cueing, but not with peripheral low predictive cueing. This suggests that the typical underestimation of visual eccentricity in OA (visual field effect) at least partially results from impaired endogenous attention orientation toward the affected visual field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tristan Jurkiewicz
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon (CRNL), INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, 69500, Bron, France; Centre d'Exploration de la Rétine Kléber (CERK), 50 cours Franklin Roosevelt, 69006, Lyon, France
| | - Audrey Vialatte
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon (CRNL), INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, 69500, Bron, France
| | - Yaffa Yeshurun
- School of Psychological Science, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
| | - Laure Pisella
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon (CRNL), INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, 69500, Bron, France
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Chaterji S, Belliappa PH, Sathyamurthy A. The superior colliculus directs goal-oriented forelimb movements. Cell Rep 2025; 44:115097. [PMID: 39723891 DOI: 10.1016/j.celrep.2024.115097] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2024] [Revised: 10/23/2024] [Accepted: 12/02/2024] [Indexed: 12/28/2024] Open
Abstract
Skilled forelimb control is essential for daily living, yet our understanding of its neural mechanisms, although extensive, remains incomplete. Here, we present evidence that the superior colliculus (SC), a major midbrain structure, is necessary for accurate forelimb reaching in mice. We found that neurons in the lateral SC are active during goal-directed reaching, and by employing chemogenetic and phase-specific optogenetic silencing of these neurons, we show that the SC causally facilitates reach accuracy. Anatomical studies identified the deep cerebellar nuclei and substantia nigra pars reticulata as sources of inputs to the SC, while functional studies revealed a role for nigrotectal, but not cerebellotectal, neurons in controlling reach endpoints. Silencing the nigrotectal pathway caused paw deviations opposite to those seen with SC silencing, emphasizing the coordinated role of the substantia nigra and SC in regulating optimal reaching. Together, these findings establish the SC as a crucial regulator of skilled forelimb control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shrivas Chaterji
- Centre for Neuroscience, Indian Institute of Science, Bengaluru, Karnataka 560012, India
| | - Punarva H Belliappa
- Centre for Neuroscience, Indian Institute of Science, Bengaluru, Karnataka 560012, India
| | - Anupama Sathyamurthy
- Centre for Neuroscience, Indian Institute of Science, Bengaluru, Karnataka 560012, India.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Knights E, McIntosh RD, Ford C, Buckingham G, Rossit S. Peripheral and bimanual reaching in a stroke survivor with left visual neglect and extinction. Neuropsychologia 2024; 201:108901. [PMID: 38704116 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108901] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2023] [Revised: 04/23/2024] [Accepted: 05/01/2024] [Indexed: 05/06/2024]
Abstract
Whether attentional deficits are accompanied by visuomotor impairments following posterior parietal lesions has been debated for quite some time. This single-case study investigated reaching in a stroke survivor (E.B.) with left visual neglect and visual extinction following right temporo-parietal-frontal strokes. Unlike most neglect patients, E.B. did not present left hemiparesis, homonymous hemianopia nor show evidence of motor neglect or extinction allowing us to examine, for the first time, if lateralised attentional deficits co-occur with deficits in peripheral and bimanual reaching. First, we found a classic optic ataxia field effect: E.B.'s accuracy was impaired when reaching to peripheral targets in her neglected left visual field (regardless of the hand used). Second, we found a larger bimanual cost for movement time in E.B. than controls when both hands reached to incongruent locations. E.B.'s visuomotor profile is similar to the one of patients with optic ataxia showing that attentional deficits are accompanied by visuomotor deficits in the affected field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ethan Knights
- Neuropsychology Laboratory, School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich, NR4 7TJ, United Kingdom
| | - Robert D McIntosh
- Human Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, The University of Edinburgh, EH8 9JZ, United Kingdom
| | - Catherine Ford
- Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychological Therapies, Norwich Medical School, University of East Anglia, Norwich, NR4 7TJ, United Kingdom
| | - Gavin Buckingham
- Department of Sport and Health Sciences, University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom
| | - Stéphanie Rossit
- Neuropsychology Laboratory, School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich, NR4 7TJ, United Kingdom.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Addison RN, Van Gemmert AWA. Bilateral Transfer of a Visuomotor Task in Different Workspace Configurations. J Mot Behav 2023; 56:290-304. [PMID: 38108224 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2023.2293002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2022] [Accepted: 11/19/2023] [Indexed: 12/19/2023]
Abstract
Bilateral transfer occurs when a learned behavior transfers from one (group of) effectors(s) to another. Researchers investigating bilateral transfer of a visuomotor adaptation task between limbs used across workspaces have observed divergent results. This study assessed whether bilateral transfer of a visuomotor adaptation task changes with workspace configuration manipulation. Ninety-six right-handed young adults were assigned to one of three workspace locations, i.e., ipsilateral, contralateral, and central. Within each workspace were two retention groups (RRR/LLL) and two bilateral transfer groups (RLR/LRL). Performance before and after training was collected to determine direct and after-effects. We observed an asymmetric transfer of pathlength (left to right) but no ensuing after-effect. However, the transfer of movement time and normalized jerk was symmetric in the contralateral workspace. These findings showed differences in the pattern of bilateral transfer asymmetry in the different workspace configurations, which was parameter specific.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Reuben N Addison
- School of Kinesiology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
- MGH Institute of Health Professions, Charlestown Navy Yard, Boston, MA, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Rivas Ruvalcaba F, Moreno-Cortez KM, Badial-Ochoa S, Rodriguez-Leyva I. Optic ataxia in a patient with HaNDL syndrome. BMJ Case Rep 2022; 15:e252055. [PMID: 36192033 PMCID: PMC9535137 DOI: 10.1136/bcr-2022-252055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/12/2022] [Indexed: 11/07/2022] Open
Abstract
We present a woman in her 40s who arrived at the emergency room with hypertension and optic ataxia. Her medical history is only relevant for obesity. Her lumbar puncture revealed high intracranial pressure and lymphocytic pleocytosis, and her neuroimaging tests, including angiography and venography, were normal. The patient improved after a cerebrospinal fluid drainage with a lumbar puncture, and her clinical manifestations resolved in parallel to the lymphocytic pleocytosis.The patient was diagnosed with a syndrome of transient headache and neurological deficits with cerebrospinal fluid lymphocytosis and fully recovered 21 days after her discharge.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francisco Rivas Ruvalcaba
- Neurology Department, Hospital Central Dr Ignacio Morones Prieto, San Luis Potosi, Mexico
- Neurology Department of the Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Autonoma de San Luis Potosi, San Luis Potosi, Mexico
| | - Katia Mabiel Moreno-Cortez
- Neurology Department, Hospital Central Dr Ignacio Morones Prieto, San Luis Potosi, Mexico
- Neurology Department of the Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Autonoma de San Luis Potosi, San Luis Potosi, Mexico
| | - Sandra Badial-Ochoa
- Neurology Department, Hospital Central Dr Ignacio Morones Prieto, San Luis Potosi, Mexico
- Neurology Department of the Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Autonoma de San Luis Potosi, San Luis Potosi, Mexico
| | - Ildefonso Rodriguez-Leyva
- Neurology Department, Hospital Central Dr Ignacio Morones Prieto, San Luis Potosi, Mexico
- Neurology Department of the Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Autonoma de San Luis Potosi, San Luis Potosi, Mexico
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Drudik K, Zlatkina V, Petrides M. Morphological patterns and spatial probability maps of the superior parietal sulcus in the human brain. Cereb Cortex 2022; 33:1230-1245. [PMID: 35388402 PMCID: PMC9930623 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhac132] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/06/2021] [Revised: 03/04/2022] [Accepted: 03/05/2022] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
The superior parietal sulcus (SPS) is the defining sulcus within the superior parietal lobule (SPL). The morphological variability of the SPS was examined in individual magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans of the human brain that were registered to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard stereotaxic space. Two primary morphological patterns were consistently identified across hemispheres: (i) the SPS was identified as a single sulcus, separating the anterior from the posterior part of the SPL and (ii) the SPS was found as a complex of multiple sulcal segments. These morphological patterns were subdivided based on whether the SPS or SPS complex remained distinct or merged with surrounding parietal sulci. The morphological variability and spatial extent of the SPS were quantified using volumetric and surface spatial probabilistic mapping. The current investigation established consistent morphological patterns in a common anatomical space, the MNI stereotaxic space, to facilitate structural and functional analyses within the SPL.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristina Drudik
- Corresponding author: Kristina Drudik, Montreal Neurological Institute, 3801 University St., Montreal, QC H3A 2B4, Canada.
| | - Veronika Zlatkina
- Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal Neurological Institute, 3801 University Street, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 2B4,Department of Psychology, McGill University, 2001 McGill College, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 1G1
| | - Michael Petrides
- Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal Neurological Institute, 3801 University Street, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 2B4,Department of Psychology, McGill University, 2001 McGill College, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 1G1
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Mitchell AG, Rossit S, Pal S, Hornberger M, Warman A, Kenning E, Williamson L, Shapland R, McIntosh RD. Peripheral reaching in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Cortex 2022; 149:29-43. [PMID: 35184013 PMCID: PMC9007170 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.01.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/02/2021] [Revised: 12/02/2021] [Accepted: 01/12/2022] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
Abstract
Recent evidence has implicated areas within the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) as among the first to show pathophysiological changes in Alzheimer's disease (AD). Focal brain damage to the PPC can cause optic ataxia, a specific deficit in reaching to peripheral targets. The present study describes a novel investigation of peripheral reaching ability in AD and mild cognitive impairment (MCI), to assess whether this deficit is common among these patient groups. Individuals with a diagnosis of mild-to-moderate AD, or MCI, and healthy older adult controls were required to reach to targets presented in central vision or in peripheral vision using two reaching tasks; one in the lateral plane and another presented in radial depth. Pre-registered case–control comparisons identified 1/10 MCI and 3/17 AD patients with significant peripheral reaching deficits at the individual level, but group-level comparisons did not find significantly higher peripheral reaching error in either AD or MCI by comparison to controls. Exploratory analyses showed significantly increased reach duration in both AD and MCI groups relative to controls, accounted for by an extended Deceleration Time of the reach movement. These findings suggest that peripheral reaching deficits like those observed in optic ataxia are not a common feature of AD. However, we show that cognitive decline is associated with a generalised slowing of movement which may indicate a visuomotor deficit in reach planning or online guidance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandra G Mitchell
- Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK; Center for Functionally Integrative Neuroscience, Aarhus University, Denmark.
| | - Stephanie Rossit
- School of Psychology, Lawrence Stenhouse Building, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK.
| | - Suvankar Pal
- Anne Rowling Regenerative Neurology Clinic, Chancellor's Building, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK.
| | | | - Annie Warman
- School of Psychology, Lawrence Stenhouse Building, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK.
| | - Elise Kenning
- School of Psychology, Lawrence Stenhouse Building, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
| | - Laura Williamson
- School of Psychology, Lawrence Stenhouse Building, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
| | - Rebecca Shapland
- School of Psychology, Lawrence Stenhouse Building, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Abstract
This chapter starts by reviewing the various interpretations of Bálint syndrome over time. We then develop a novel integrative view in which we propose that the various symptoms, historically reported and labeled by various authors, result from a core mislocalization deficit. This idea is in accordance with our previous proposal that the core deficit of Bálint syndrome is attentional (Pisella et al., 2009, 2013, 2017) since covert attention improves spatial resolution in visual periphery (Yeshurun and Carrasco, 1998); a deficit of covert attention would thus increase spatial uncertainty and thereby impair both visual object identification and visuomotor accuracy. In peripheral vision, we perceive the intrinsic characteristics of the perceptual elements surrounding us, but not their precise localization (Rosenholtz et al., 2012a,b), such that without covert attention we cannot organize them to their respective and recognizable objects; this explains why perceptual symptoms (simultanagnosia, neglect) could result from visual mislocalization. The visuomotor symptoms (optic ataxia) can be accounted for by both visual and proprioceptive mislocalizations in an oculocentric reference frame, leading to field and hand effects, respectively. This new pathophysiological account is presented along with a model of posterior parietal cortex organization in which the superior part is devoted to covert attention, while the right inferior part is involved in visual remapping. When the right inferior parietal cortex is damaged, additional representational mislocalizations across saccades worsen the clinical picture of peripheral mislocalizations due to an impairment of covert attention.
Collapse
|
9
|
Pisella L, Havé L, Rossetti Y. Body awareness disorders: dissociations between body-related visual and somatosensory information. Brain 2019; 142:2170-2173. [PMID: 31347680 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awz187] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Laure Pisella
- ImpAct Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Villeurbanne, France
| | - Laurence Havé
- ImpAct Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Villeurbanne, France.,Hôpital d'instruction des armées Desgenettes, Lyon, France
| | - Yves Rossetti
- ImpAct Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Villeurbanne, France.,Plate-forme Mouvement et Handicap, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Bron, France
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Baumard J, Etcharry-Bouyx F, Chauviré V, Boussard D, Lesourd M, Remigereau C, Rossetti Y, Osiurak F, Le Gall D. Effect of object substitution, spontaneous compensation and repetitive training on reaching movements in a patient with optic ataxia. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2019; 30:1786-1813. [PMID: 31030640 DOI: 10.1080/09602011.2019.1607397] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
We report the case of M.B. who demonstrated severe optic ataxia with the right hand following stroke in the left hemisphere. The clinical picture may shed light on both the pathological characteristics of reaching and grasping actions, and potential rehabilitation strategies for optic ataxia. First, M.B. demonstrated a dissociation between severely impaired reaching and relatively spared grasping and tool use skills and knowledge, which confirms that grasping may be more intermingled with non-motoric cognitive mechanisms than reaching. Besides, M.B.'s reaching performance was sensitive to movement repetition. We observed a substitution effect: Reaching time decreased if M.B. repeatedly reached toward the same object but increased when object identity changed. This may imply that not only object localization but also object identity, is integrated into movement programming in reach-to-grasp tasks. Second, studying M.B.'s spontaneous compensation strategies ascertained that the mere repetition of reaching movements had a positive effect, to the point M.B. almost recovered to normal level after an intensive one-day repetitive training session. This case study seems to provide one of the first examples of optic ataxia rehabilitation. Reaching skills can be trained by repetitive training even two years post-stroke and despite the presence of visuo-imitative apraxia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Frédérique Etcharry-Bouyx
- Laboratory of Psychology LPPL (EA 4638), University of Angers, Angers, France.,Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Angers, Angers, France
| | - Valérie Chauviré
- Laboratory of Psychology LPPL (EA 4638), University of Angers, Angers, France.,Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Angers, Angers, France
| | - Delphine Boussard
- Neuropsychological Unit, Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Angers, Angers, France
| | - Mathieu Lesourd
- Laboratory for the Study of Cognitive Mechanisms (EA 3082), University of Lyon, Lyon, France
| | - Chrystelle Remigereau
- Laboratory for the Study of Cognitive Mechanisms (EA 3082), University of Lyon, Lyon, France
| | | | - François Osiurak
- Laboratory for the Study of Cognitive Mechanisms (EA 3082), University of Lyon, Lyon, France.,French Universitary Institute, Paris, France
| | - Didier Le Gall
- Laboratory of Psychology LPPL (EA 4638), University of Angers, Angers, France.,Neuropsychological Unit, Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Angers, Angers, France
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Dosso JA, Chua R, Weeks DJ, Turk DJ, Kingstone A. Attention and awareness: Representation of visuomotor space in split-brain patients. Cortex 2018; 122:253-262. [PMID: 30292346 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.08.029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2018] [Revised: 07/06/2018] [Accepted: 08/29/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Each cerebral hemisphere primarily controls and receives sensory input with regard to the contralateral hand. In the disconnected brain (split-brain), when the hands are uncrossed, direct visual access to each hand is available to the controlling (contralateral) hemisphere. However, when a hand crosses the midline, visual and tactile information regarding the hand are presented to different hemispheres. It is unknown how a contralateral hemisphere codes the position and orientation of a visually inaccessible hand in the disconnected brain. The present work addresses this issue. We ask how each hemisphere represents "its" hand across hand positions that span the midline in the absence of cortical input from the contralateral hemisphere. In other words, when a hand is placed across the midline and is visually inaccessible, is it represented by the controlling hemisphere: (1) in accordance with its new position with respect to the body (e.g., a left hand "becomes" a right effector when it crosses the midline), (2) with left/right position information unaltered (e.g., the left hand is represented as "left" regardless of its location), or (3) stripped of its location information altogether? The relationship between hand position and the spatial codes assigned to potential responses (an index of hand representation) was investigated in two split-brain patients using direct (Experiment 1) and orthogonal (Experiment 2) S-R compatibility paradigms. S-R compatibility effects in split-brain patients were consistent with those displayed by typical individuals. These findings suggest that position-based compatibility effects do not rely on cross-cortical connections. Rather, each hemisphere can accurately represent the full visuomotor space, a process that appears to be subserved by subcortical connections between the hemispheres.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jill A Dosso
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada.
| | - Romeo Chua
- School of Kinesiology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
| | - Daniel J Weeks
- Office of the President, University of Northern British Columbia, Prince George, BC, Canada
| | - David J Turk
- School of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
| | - Alan Kingstone
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Mikula L, Gaveau V, Pisella L, Khan AZ, Blohm G. Learned rather than online relative weighting of visual-proprioceptive sensory cues. J Neurophysiol 2018; 119:1981-1992. [PMID: 29465322 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00338.2017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/09/2023] Open
Abstract
When reaching to an object, information about the target location as well as the initial hand position is required to program the motor plan for the arm. The initial hand position can be determined by proprioceptive information as well as visual information, if available. Bayes-optimal integration posits that we utilize all information available, with greater weighting on the sense that is more reliable, thus generally weighting visual information more than the usually less reliable proprioceptive information. The criterion by which information is weighted has not been explicitly investigated; it has been assumed that the weights are based on task- and effector-dependent sensory reliability requiring an explicit neuronal representation of variability. However, the weights could also be determined implicitly through learned modality-specific integration weights and not on effector-dependent reliability. While the former hypothesis predicts different proprioceptive weights for left and right hands, e.g., due to different reliabilities of dominant vs. nondominant hand proprioception, we would expect the same integration weights if the latter hypothesis was true. We found that the proprioceptive weights for the left and right hands were extremely consistent regardless of differences in sensory variability for the two hands as measured in two separate complementary tasks. Thus we propose that proprioceptive weights during reaching are learned across both hands, with high interindividual range but independent of each hand's specific proprioceptive variability. NEW & NOTEWORTHY How visual and proprioceptive information about the hand are integrated to plan a reaching movement is still debated. The goal of this study was to clarify how the weights assigned to vision and proprioception during multisensory integration are determined. We found evidence that the integration weights are modality specific rather than based on the sensory reliabilities of the effectors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Mikula
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, ImpAct Team, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Lyon 1 University, Bron Cedex, France.,School of Optometry, University of Montreal , Montreal, Quebec , Canada
| | - Valérie Gaveau
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, ImpAct Team, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Lyon 1 University, Bron Cedex, France
| | - Laure Pisella
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, ImpAct Team, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Lyon 1 University, Bron Cedex, France
| | - Aarlenne Z Khan
- School of Optometry, University of Montreal , Montreal, Quebec , Canada
| | - Gunnar Blohm
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University , Kingston, Ontario , Canada
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Jackson SR, Condon LA, Newport RW, Pears S, Husain M, Bajaj N, O'Donoghue M. Optic ataxia and the dorsal visual steam re-visited: Impairment in bimanual haptic matching performed without vision. Cortex 2018; 98:60-72. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.03.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/11/2017] [Revised: 03/14/2017] [Accepted: 03/17/2017] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
14
|
Abstract
This chapter reviews clinical and scientific approaches to optic ataxia. This double historic track allows us to address important issues such as the link between Bálint syndrome and optic ataxia, the alleged double dissociation between optic ataxia and visual agnosia, and the use of optic ataxia to argue for a specific vision-for-action occipitoposterior parietal stream. Clinical cases are described and reveal that perceptual deficits have been long shown to accompany ataxia. Importantly, the term ataxia appears to be misleading as patients exhibit a combination of visual and nonvisual perceptual, attentional, and visuomotor guidance deficits, which are confirmed by experimental approaches. Three major features of optic ataxia are described. The first is a spatial feature whereby the deficits exhibited by patients appear to be specific to peripheral vision, akin to the field effect. Visuomotor field examination allows us to quantify this deficit and reveals that it consists of a highly reliable retinocentric hypometria. The third is a temporal feature whereby these deficits are exacerbated under temporal constraints, i.e., when attending to dynamic stimuli. These two aspects combine in a situation where patients have to quickly respond to a target presented in peripheral vision that is experimentally displaced upon movement onset. In addition to the field effect, a hand effect can be described in conditions where the hand is not visible. Spatial and temporal aspects as well as field and hand effects may rely on several posterior parietal modules that remain to be precisely identified both anatomically and functionally. It is concluded that optic ataxia is not a visuomotor deficit and there is no dissociation between perception and action capacities in optic ataxia, hence a fortiori no double dissociation between optic ataxia and visual agnosia. Future directions for understanding the basic pathophysiology of optic ataxia are proposed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yves Rossetti
- Integrative Multisensory Perception Action Cognition Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Centre, Lyon, France.
| | - Laure Pisella
- Integrative Multisensory Perception Action Cognition Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Centre, Lyon, France
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Martinaud O. Visual agnosia and focal brain injury. Rev Neurol (Paris) 2017; 173:451-460. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neurol.2017.07.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2017] [Revised: 07/11/2017] [Accepted: 07/17/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
16
|
Chen Y, Crawford JD. Cortical Activation during Landmark-Centered vs. Gaze-Centered Memory of Saccade Targets in the Human: An FMRI Study. Front Syst Neurosci 2017; 11:44. [PMID: 28690501 PMCID: PMC5481872 DOI: 10.3389/fnsys.2017.00044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2017] [Accepted: 06/06/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
A remembered saccade target could be encoded in egocentric coordinates such as gaze-centered, or relative to some external allocentric landmark that is independent of the target or gaze (landmark-centered). In comparison to egocentric mechanisms, very little is known about such a landmark-centered representation. Here, we used an event-related fMRI design to identify brain areas supporting these two types of spatial coding (i.e., landmark-centered vs. gaze-centered) for target memory during the Delay phase where only target location, not saccade direction, was specified. The paradigm included three tasks with identical display of visual stimuli but different auditory instructions: Landmark Saccade (remember target location relative to a visual landmark, independent of gaze), Control Saccade (remember original target location relative to gaze fixation, independent of the landmark), and a non-spatial control, Color Report (report target color). During the Delay phase, the Control and Landmark Saccade tasks activated overlapping areas in posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and frontal cortex as compared to the color control, but with higher activation in PPC for target coding in the Control Saccade task and higher activation in temporal and occipital cortex for target coding in Landmark Saccade task. Gaze-centered directional selectivity was observed in superior occipital gyrus and inferior occipital gyrus, whereas landmark-centered directional selectivity was observed in precuneus and midposterior intraparietal sulcus. During the Response phase after saccade direction was specified, the parietofrontal network in the left hemisphere showed higher activation for rightward than leftward saccades. Our results suggest that cortical activation for coding saccade target direction relative to a visual landmark differs from gaze-centered directional selectivity for target memory, from the mechanisms for other types of allocentric tasks, and from the directionally selective mechanisms for saccade planning and execution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ying Chen
- Center for Vision Research, York University, TorontoON, Canada.,Departments of Psychology, Biology, and Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, TorontoON, Canada.,Canadian Action and Perception Network, TorontoON, Canada
| | - J D Crawford
- Center for Vision Research, York University, TorontoON, Canada.,Departments of Psychology, Biology, and Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, TorontoON, Canada.,Canadian Action and Perception Network, TorontoON, Canada.,Vision: Science to Applications Program, York University, TorontoON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Rise and fall of the two visual systems theory. Ann Phys Rehabil Med 2017; 60:130-140. [DOI: 10.1016/j.rehab.2017.02.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 59] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/22/2016] [Accepted: 02/15/2017] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
18
|
Optic ataxia in Bálint-Holmes syndrome. Ann Phys Rehabil Med 2017; 60:148-154. [DOI: 10.1016/j.rehab.2016.01.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/25/2015] [Accepted: 12/05/2015] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
19
|
Chieffi S, Messina G, Messina A, Villano I, Monda V, Ambra FI, Garofalo E, Romano F, Mollica MP, Monda M, Iavarone A. Memory for Spatial Locations in a Patient with Near Space Neglect and Optic Ataxia: Involvement of the Occipitotemporal Stream. Front Neurol 2017; 8:231. [PMID: 28620345 PMCID: PMC5449448 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00231] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2017] [Accepted: 05/12/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous studies suggested that the occipitoparietal stream orients attention toward the near/lower space and is involved in immediate reaching, whereas the occipitotemporal stream orients attention toward the far/upper space and is involved in delayed reaching. In the present study, we investigated the role of the occipitotemporal stream in attention orienting and delayed reaching in a patient (GP) with bilateral damage to the occipitoparietal areas and optic ataxia. GP and healthy controls took part in three experiments. In the experiment 1, the participants bisected lines oriented along radial, vertical, and horizontal axes. GP bisected radial lines farther, and vertical lines more above, than the controls, consistent with an attentional bias toward the far/upper space and near/lower space neglect. The experiment 2 consisted of two tasks: (1) an immediate reaching task, in which GP reached target locations under visual control and (2) a delayed visual reaching task, in which GP and controls were asked to reach remembered target locations visually presented. We measured constant and variable distance and direction errors. In immediate reaching task, GP accurately reached target locations. In delayed reaching task, GP overshot remembered target locations, whereas the controls undershot them. Furthermore, variable errors were greater in GP than in the controls. In the experiment 3, GP and controls performed a delayed proprioceptive reaching task. Constant reaching errors did not differ between GP and the controls. However, variable direction errors were greater in GP than in the controls. We suggest that the occipitoparietal damage, and the relatively intact occipitotemporal region, produced in GP an attentional orienting bias toward the far/upper space (experiment 1). In turns, the attentional bias selectively shifted toward the far space remembered visual (experiment 2), but not proprioceptive (experiment 3), target locations. As a whole, these findings further support the hypothesis of an involvement of the occipitotemporal stream in delayed reaching. Furthermore, the observation that in both delayed reaching tasks the variable errors were greater in GP than in the controls suggested that in optic ataxia is present not only a visuo- but also a proprioceptivo-motor integration deficit.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sergio Chieffi
- Department of Experimental Medicine, University of Campania "Luigi Vanvitelli", Naples, Italy
| | - Giovanni Messina
- Department of Experimental Medicine, University of Campania "Luigi Vanvitelli", Naples, Italy.,Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Foggia, Foggia, Italy
| | - Antonietta Messina
- Department of Experimental Medicine, University of Campania "Luigi Vanvitelli", Naples, Italy
| | - Ines Villano
- Department of Experimental Medicine, University of Campania "Luigi Vanvitelli", Naples, Italy
| | - Vincenzo Monda
- Department of Experimental Medicine, University of Campania "Luigi Vanvitelli", Naples, Italy
| | - Ferdinando Ivano Ambra
- Neurological and Stroke Unit, CTO Hospital, AORN "Ospedali dei Colli" Naples, Naples, Italy
| | - Elisabetta Garofalo
- Neurological and Stroke Unit, CTO Hospital, AORN "Ospedali dei Colli" Naples, Naples, Italy
| | - Felice Romano
- Neurological and Stroke Unit, CTO Hospital, AORN "Ospedali dei Colli" Naples, Naples, Italy
| | - Maria Pina Mollica
- Department of Biology, Università degli Studi di Napoli Federico II, Naples, Italy
| | - Marcellino Monda
- Department of Experimental Medicine, University of Campania "Luigi Vanvitelli", Naples, Italy
| | - Alessandro Iavarone
- Neurological and Stroke Unit, CTO Hospital, AORN "Ospedali dei Colli" Naples, Naples, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Sayegh PF, Gorbet DJ, Hawkins KM, Hoffman KL, Sergio LE. The Contribution of Different Cortical Regions to the Control of Spatially Decoupled Eye-Hand Coordination. J Cogn Neurosci 2017; 29:1194-1211. [PMID: 28253075 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01111] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
Our brain's ability to flexibly control the communication between the eyes and the hand allows for our successful interaction with the objects located within our environment. This flexibility has been observed in the pattern of neural responses within key regions of the frontoparietal reach network. More specifically, our group has shown how single-unit and oscillatory activity within the dorsal premotor cortex (PMd) and the superior parietal lobule (SPL) change contingent on the level of visuomotor compatibility between the eyes and hand. Reaches that involve a coupling between the eyes and hand toward a common spatial target display a pattern of neural responses that differ from reaches that require eye-hand decoupling. Although previous work examined the altered spiking and oscillatory activity that occurs during different types of eye-hand compatibilities, they did not address how each of these measures of neurological activity interacts with one another. Thus, in an effort to fully characterize the relationship between oscillatory and single-unit activity during different types of eye-hand coordination, we measured the spike-field coherence (SFC) within regions of macaque SPL and PMd. We observed stronger SFC within PMdr and superficial regions of SPL (areas 5/PEc) during decoupled reaches, whereas PMdc and regions within SPL surrounding medial intrapareital sulcus had stronger SFC during coupled reaches. These results were supported by meta-analysis on human fMRI data. Our results support the proposal of altered cortical control during complex eye-hand coordination and highlight the necessity to account for the different eye-hand compatibilities in motor control research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Diana J Gorbet
- 1 York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.,2 Canadian Action and Perception Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | | | - Kari L Hoffman
- 1 York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.,2 Canadian Action and Perception Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Lauren E Sergio
- 1 York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.,2 Canadian Action and Perception Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Sheth BR, Young R. Two Visual Pathways in Primates Based on Sampling of Space: Exploitation and Exploration of Visual Information. Front Integr Neurosci 2016; 10:37. [PMID: 27920670 PMCID: PMC5118626 DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2016.00037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/25/2016] [Accepted: 10/25/2016] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Evidence is strong that the visual pathway is segregated into two distinct streams—ventral and dorsal. Two proposals theorize that the pathways are segregated in function: The ventral stream processes information about object identity, whereas the dorsal stream, according to one model, processes information about either object location, and according to another, is responsible in executing movements under visual control. The models are influential; however recent experimental evidence challenges them, e.g., the ventral stream is not solely responsible for object recognition; conversely, its function is not strictly limited to object vision; the dorsal stream is not responsible by itself for spatial vision or visuomotor control; conversely, its function extends beyond vision or visuomotor control. In their place, we suggest a robust dichotomy consisting of a ventral stream selectively sampling high-resolution/focal spaces, and a dorsal stream sampling nearly all of space with reduced foveal bias. The proposal hews closely to the theme of embodied cognition: Function arises as a consequence of an extant sensory underpinning. A continuous, not sharp, segregation based on function emerges, and carries with it an undercurrent of an exploitation-exploration dichotomy. Under this interpretation, cells of the ventral stream, which individually have more punctate receptive fields that generally include the fovea or parafovea, provide detailed information about object shapes and features and lead to the systematic exploitation of said information; cells of the dorsal stream, which individually have large receptive fields, contribute to visuospatial perception, provide information about the presence/absence of salient objects and their locations for novel exploration and subsequent exploitation by the ventral stream or, under certain conditions, the dorsal stream. We leverage the dichotomy to unify neuropsychological cases under a common umbrella, account for the increased prevalence of multisensory integration in the dorsal stream under a Bayesian framework, predict conditions under which object recognition utilizes the ventral or dorsal stream, and explain why cells of the dorsal stream drive sensorimotor control and motion processing and have poorer feature selectivity. Finally, the model speculates on a dynamic interaction between the two streams that underscores a unified, seamless perception. Existing theories are subsumed under our proposal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bhavin R Sheth
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of HoustonHouston, TX, USA; Center for NeuroEngineering and Cognitive Systems, University of HoustonHouston, TX, USA
| | - Ryan Young
- Department of Neuroscience, Brandeis University Waltham, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Vindras P, Blangero A, Ota H, Reilly KT, Rossetti Y, Pisella L. The Pointing Errors in Optic Ataxia Reveal the Role of "Peripheral Magnification" of the PPC. Front Integr Neurosci 2016; 10:27. [PMID: 27507938 PMCID: PMC4960242 DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2016.00027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2016] [Accepted: 07/11/2016] [Indexed: 12/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Interaction with visual objects in the environment requires an accurate correspondence between visual space and its internal representation within the brain. Many clinical conditions involve some impairment in visuo-motor control and the errors created by the lesion of a specific brain region are neither random nor uninformative. Modern approaches to studying the neuropsychology of action require powerful data-driven analyses and error modeling in order to understand the function of the lesioned areas. In the present paper we carried out mixed-effect analyses of the pointing errors of seven optic ataxia patients and seven control subjects. We found that a small parameter set is sufficient to explain the pointing errors produced by unilateral optic ataxia patients. In particular, the extremely stereotypical errors made when pointing toward the contralesional visual field can be fitted by mathematical models similar to those used to model central magnification in cortical or sub-cortical structure(s). Our interpretation is that visual areas that contain this footprint of central magnification guide pointing movements when the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) is damaged and that the functional role of the PPC is to actively compensate for the under-representation of peripheral vision that accompanies central magnification. Optic ataxia misreaching reveals what would be hand movement accuracy and precision if the human motor system did not include elaborated corrective processes for reaching and grasping to non-foveated targets.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Philippe Vindras
- ImpAct Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center CRNL, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292 and University Claude Bernard Lyon I Villeurbanne, France
| | | | - Hisaaki Ota
- Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Health Sciences, Sapporo Medical University Sapporo, Japan
| | - Karen T Reilly
- ImpAct Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center CRNL, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292 and University Claude Bernard Lyon I Villeurbanne, France
| | - Yves Rossetti
- ImpAct Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center CRNL, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292 and University Claude Bernard Lyon I Villeurbanne, France
| | - Laure Pisella
- ImpAct Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center CRNL, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292 and University Claude Bernard Lyon I Villeurbanne, France
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Briscoe R, Schwenkler J. Conscious Vision in Action. Cogn Sci 2015; 39:1435-67. [DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12226] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/10/2012] [Revised: 08/27/2014] [Accepted: 08/29/2014] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
24
|
Ambron E, Lingnau A, Lunardelli A, Pesavento V, Rumiati RI. The effect of goals and vision on movements: A case study of optic ataxia and limb apraxia. Brain Cogn 2015; 95:77-89. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.01.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/28/2014] [Revised: 01/13/2015] [Accepted: 01/14/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
25
|
Abstract
The location of a remembered reach target can be encoded in egocentric and/or allocentric reference frames. Cortical mechanisms for egocentric reach are relatively well described, but the corresponding allocentric representations are essentially unknown. Here, we used an event-related fMRI design to distinguish human brain areas involved in these two types of representation. Our paradigm consisted of three tasks with identical stimulus display but different instructions: egocentric reach (remember absolute target location), allocentric reach (remember target location relative to a visual landmark), and a nonspatial control, color report (report color of target). During the delay phase (when only target location was specified), the egocentric and allocentric tasks elicited widely overlapping regions of cortical activity (relative to the control), but with higher activation in parietofrontal cortex for egocentric task and higher activation in early visual cortex for allocentric tasks. In addition, egocentric directional selectivity (target relative to gaze) was observed in the superior occipital gyrus and the inferior occipital gyrus, whereas allocentric directional selectivity (target relative to a visual landmark) was observed in the inferior temporal gyrus and inferior occipital gyrus. During the response phase (after movement direction had been specified either by reappearance of the visual landmark or a pro-/anti-reach instruction), the parietofrontal network resumed egocentric directional selectivity, showing higher activation for contralateral than ipsilateral reaches. These results show that allocentric and egocentric reach mechanisms use partially overlapping but different cortical substrates and that directional specification is different for target memory versus reach response.
Collapse
|
26
|
Blouin J, Saradjian AH, Lebar N, Guillaume A, Mouchnino L. Opposed optimal strategies of weighting somatosensory inputs for planning reaching movements toward visual and proprioceptive targets. J Neurophysiol 2014; 112:2290-301. [DOI: 10.1152/jn.00857.2013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Behavioral studies have suggested that the brain uses a visual estimate of the hand to plan reaching movements toward visual targets and somatosensory inputs in the case of somatosensory targets. However, neural correlates for distinct coding of the hand according to the sensory modality of the target have not yet been identified. Here we tested the twofold hypothesis that the somatosensory input from the reaching hand is facilitated and inhibited, respectively, when planning movements toward somatosensory (unseen fingers) or visual targets. The weight of the somatosensory inputs was assessed by measuring the amplitude of the somatosensory evoked potential (SEP) resulting from vibration of the reaching finger during movement planning. The target sensory modality had no significant effect on SEP amplitude. However, Spearman's analyses showed significant correlations between the SEPs and reaching errors. When planning movements toward proprioceptive targets without visual feedback of the reaching hand, participants showing the greater SEPs were those who produced the smaller directional errors. Inversely, participants showing the smaller SEPs when planning movements toward visual targets with visual feedback of the reaching hand were those who produced the smaller directional errors. No significant correlation was found between the SEPs and radial or amplitude errors. Our results indicate that the sensory strategy for planning movements is highly flexible among individuals and also for a given sensory context. Most importantly, they provide neural bases for the suggestion that optimization of movement planning requires the target and the reaching hand to both be represented in the same sensory modality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jean Blouin
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, CNRS, Aix-Marseille University, FR 3C 3512, Marseille, France
| | - Anahid H. Saradjian
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, CNRS, Aix-Marseille University, FR 3C 3512, Marseille, France
| | - Nicolas Lebar
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, CNRS, Aix-Marseille University, FR 3C 3512, Marseille, France
| | - Alain Guillaume
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, CNRS, Aix-Marseille University, FR 3C 3512, Marseille, France
| | - Laurence Mouchnino
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, CNRS, Aix-Marseille University, FR 3C 3512, Marseille, France
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Andersen RA, Andersen KN, Hwang EJ, Hauschild M. Optic ataxia: from Balint's syndrome to the parietal reach region. Neuron 2014; 81:967-983. [PMID: 24607223 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 81] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/18/2014] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
Optic ataxia is a high-order deficit in reaching to visual goals that occurs with posterior parietal cortex (PPC) lesions. It is a component of Balint's syndrome that also includes attentional and gaze disorders. Aspects of optic ataxia are misreaching in the contralesional visual field, difficulty preshaping the hand for grasping, and an inability to correct reaches online. Recent research in nonhuman primates (NHPs) suggests that many aspects of Balint's syndrome and optic ataxia are a result of damage to specific functional modules for reaching, saccades, grasp, attention, and state estimation. The deficits from large lesions in humans are probably composite effects from damage to combinations of these functional modules. Interactions between these modules, either within posterior parietal cortex or downstream within frontal cortex, may account for more complex behaviors such as hand-eye coordination and reach-to-grasp.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Richard A Andersen
- Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA.
| | - Kristen N Andersen
- Departments of Neurology and Pediatrics, University of California, Los Angeles Medical Center, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA
| | - Eun Jung Hwang
- Division of Biological Sciences, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
| | - Markus Hauschild
- Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Hall LA, Karl JM, Thomas BL, Whishaw IQ. Reach and Grasp reconfigurations reveal that proprioception assists reaching and hapsis assists grasping in peripheral vision. Exp Brain Res 2014; 232:2807-19. [PMID: 24792500 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-014-3945-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2013] [Accepted: 04/03/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
The dual visuomotor channel theory proposes that prehension consists of a Reach that transports the hand in relation to an object's extrinsic properties (e.g., location) and a Grasp that shapes the hand to an object's intrinsic properties (e.g., size and shape). In central vision, the Reach and the Grasp are integrated but when an object cannot be seen, the movements can decompose with the Reach first used to locate the object and the Grasp postponed until it is assisted by touch. Reaching for an object in a peripheral visual field is an everyday act, and although it is reported that there are changes in Grasp aperture with target eccentricity, it is not known whether the configuration of the Reach and the Grasp also changes. The present study examined this question by asking participants to reach for food items at 0° or 22.5° and 45° from central gaze. Participants made 15 reaches for a larger round donut ball and a smaller blueberry, and hand movements were analyzed using frame-by-frame video inspection and linear kinematics. Perception of targets was degraded as participants could not identify objects in peripheral vision but did recognize their differential size. The Reach to peripheral targets featured a more dorsal trajectory, a more open hand, and less accurate digit placement. The Grasp featured hand adjustments or target manipulations after contact, which were associated with a prolonged Grasp duration. Thus, Grasps to peripheral vision did not consist only of a simple modification of visually guided reaching but included the addition of somatosensory assistance. The kinematic and behavioral changes argue that proprioception assists the Reach and touch assists the Grasp in peripheral vision, supporting the idea that Reach and Grasp movements are used flexibly in relation to sensory guidance depending upon the salience of target properties.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lauren A Hall
- Canadian Centre for Behavioural Neuroscience, Department of Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, AB, T1K 3M4, Canada
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Sayegh PF, Hawkins KM, Neagu B, Crawford JD, Hoffman KL, Sergio LE. Decoupling the actions of the eyes from the hand alters beta and gamma synchrony within SPL. J Neurophysiol 2014; 111:2210-21. [PMID: 24598517 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00793.2013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Eye-hand coordination is crucial for our ability to interact with the world around us. However, much of the visually guided reaches that we perform require a spatial decoupling between gaze direction and hand orientation. These complex decoupled reaching movements are in contrast to more standard eye and hand reaching movements in which the eyes and the hand are coupled. The superior parietal lobule (SPL) receives converging eye and hand signals; however, what is yet to be understood is how the activity within this region is modulated during decoupled eye and hand reaches. To address this, we recorded local field potentials within SPL from two rhesus macaques during coupled vs. decoupled eye and hand movements. Overall we observed a distinct separation in synchrony within the lower 10- to 20-Hz beta range from that in the higher 30- to 40-Hz gamma range. Specifically, within the early planning phase, beta synchrony dominated; however, the onset of this sustained beta oscillation occurred later during eye-hand decoupled vs. coupled reaches. As the task progressed, there was a switch to low-frequency and gamma-dominated responses, specifically for decoupled reaches. More importantly, we observed local field potential activity to be a stronger task (coupled vs. decoupled) and state (planning vs. execution) predictor than that of single units alone. Our results provide further insight into the computations of SPL for visuomotor transformations and highlight the necessity of accounting for the decoupled eye-hand nature of a motor task when interpreting movement control research data.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patricia F Sayegh
- School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Canadian Action and Perception Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and
| | - Kara M Hawkins
- School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Canadian Action and Perception Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and
| | - Bogdan Neagu
- Canadian Action and Perception Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and Division of Neurology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - J Douglas Crawford
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Canadian Action and Perception Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and
| | - Kari L Hoffman
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Canadian Action and Perception Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and
| | - Lauren E Sergio
- School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Canadian Action and Perception Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Bosco A, Breveglieri R, Reser D, Galletti C, Fattori P. Multiple representation of reaching space in the medial posterior parietal area V6A. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2014; 25:1654-67. [PMID: 24421176 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bht420] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022]
Abstract
During foveal reaching, the activity of neurons in the macaque medial posterior parietal area V6A is modulated by both gaze and arm direction. In the present work, we dissociated the position of gaze and reaching targets, and studied the neural activity of single V6A cells while the eyes and reaching targets were arranged in different spatial configurations (peripheral and foveal combinations). Target position influenced neural activity in all stages of the task, from visual presentation of target and movement planning, through reach execution and holding time. The majority of neurons preferred reaches directed toward peripheral targets, rather than foveal. Most neurons discharged in both premovement and action epochs. In most cases, reaching activity was tuned coherently across action planning and execution. When reaches were planned and executed in different eye/target configurations, multiple analyses revealed that few neurons coded reaching actions according to the absolute position of target, or to the position of target relative to the eye. The majority of cells responded to a combination of both these factors. These data suggest that V6A contains multiple representations of spatial information for reaching, consistent with a role of this area in forming cross-reference frame representations to be used by premotor cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Bosco
- Department of Pharmacy and Biotechnology, University of Bologna, 40126 Bologna, Italy
| | - R Breveglieri
- Department of Pharmacy and Biotechnology, University of Bologna, 40126 Bologna, Italy
| | - D Reser
- Department of Physiology, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
| | - C Galletti
- Department of Pharmacy and Biotechnology, University of Bologna, 40126 Bologna, Italy
| | - P Fattori
- Department of Pharmacy and Biotechnology, University of Bologna, 40126 Bologna, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Decoupled visually-guided reaching in optic ataxia: differences in motor control between canonical and non-canonical orientations in space. PLoS One 2013; 8:e86138. [PMID: 24392035 PMCID: PMC3877394 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0086138] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/02/2013] [Accepted: 12/05/2013] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Guiding a limb often involves situations in which the spatial location of the target for gaze and limb movement are not congruent (i.e. have been decoupled). Such decoupled situations involve both the implementation of a cognitive rule (i.e. strategic control) and the online monitoring of the limb position relative to gaze and target (i.e. sensorimotor recalibration). To further understand the neural mechanisms underlying these different types of visuomotor control, we tested patient IG who has bilateral caudal superior parietal lobule (SPL) damage resulting in optic ataxia (OA), and compared her performance with six age-matched controls on a series of center-out reaching tasks. The tasks comprised 1) directing a cursor that had been rotated (180° or 90°) within the same spatial plane as the visual display, or 2) moving the hand along a different spatial plane than the visual display (horizontal or para-sagittal). Importantly, all conditions were performed towards visual targets located along either the horizontal axis (left and right; which can be guided from strategic control) or the diagonal axes (top-left and top-right; which require on-line trajectory elaboration and updating by sensorimotor recalibration). The bilateral OA patient performed much better in decoupled visuomotor control towards the horizontal targets, a canonical situation in which well-categorized allocentric cues could be utilized (i.e. guiding cursor direction perpendicular to computer monitor border). Relative to neurologically intact adults, IG's performance suffered towards diagonal targets, a non-canonical situation in which only less-categorized allocentric cues were available (i.e. guiding cursor direction at an off-axis angle to computer monitor border), and she was therefore required to rely on sensorimotor recalibration of her decoupled limb. We propose that an intact caudal SPL is crucial for any decoupled visuomotor control, particularly when relying on the realignment between vision and proprioception without reliable allocentric cues towards non-canonical orientations in space.
Collapse
|
32
|
Khan AZ, Pisella L, Blohm G. Causal evidence for posterior parietal cortex involvement in visual-to-motor transformations of reach targets. Cortex 2013; 49:2439-48. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.12.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2012] [Revised: 08/30/2012] [Accepted: 12/04/2012] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
33
|
Cavina-Pratesi C, Connolly JD, Milner AD. Optic ataxia as a model to investigate the role of the posterior parietal cortex in visually guided action: evidence from studies of patient M.H. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:336. [PMID: 23882200 PMCID: PMC3712225 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00336] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2013] [Accepted: 06/14/2013] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Optic ataxia is a neuropsychological disorder that affects the ability to interact with objects presented in the visual modality following either unilateral or bilateral lesions of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC). Patients with optic ataxia fail to reach accurately for objects, particularly when they are presented in peripheral vision. The present review will focus on a series of experiments performed on patient M.H. Following a lesion restricted largely to the left PPC, he developed mis-reaching behavior when using his contralesional right arm for movements directed toward the contralesional (right) visual half-field. Given the clear-cut specificity of this patient's deficit, whereby reaching actions are essentially spared when executed toward his ipsilateral space or when using his left arm, M.H. provides a valuable "experiment of nature" for investigating the role of the PPC in performing different visually guided actions. In order to address this, we used kinematic measurement techniques to investigate M.H.'s reaching and grasping behavior in various tasks. Our experiments support the idea that optic ataxia is highly function-specific: it affects a specific sub-category of visually guided actions (reaching but not grasping), regardless of their specific end goal (both reaching toward an object and reaching to avoid an obstacle); and finally, is independent of the limb used to perform the action (whether the arm or the leg). Critically, these results are congruent with recent functional MRI experiments in neurologically intact subjects which suggest that the PPC is organized in a function-specific, rather than effector-specific, manner with different sub-portions of its mantle devoted to guiding actions according to their specific end-goal (reaching, grasping, or looking), rather than according to the effector used to perform them (leg, arm, hand, or eyes).
Collapse
|
34
|
Borchers S, Müller L, Synofzik M, Himmelbach M. Guidelines and quality measures for the diagnosis of optic ataxia. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:324. [PMID: 23847498 PMCID: PMC3698451 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00324] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2013] [Accepted: 06/12/2013] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Since the first description of a systematic mis-reaching by Bálint in 1909, a reasonable number of patients showing a similar phenomenology, later termed optic ataxia (OA), has been described. However, there is surprising inconsistency regarding the behavioral measures that are used to detect OA in experimental and clinical reports, if the respective measures are reported at all. A typical screening method that was presumably used by most researchers and clinicians, reaching for a target object in the peripheral visual space, has never been evaluated. We developed a set of instructions and evaluation criteria for the scoring of a semi-standardized version of this reaching task. We tested 36 healthy participants, a group of 52 acute and chronic stroke patients, and 24 patients suffering from cerebellar ataxia. We found a high interrater reliability and a moderate test-retest reliability comparable to other clinical instruments in the stroke sample. The calculation of cut-off thresholds based on healthy control and cerebellar patient data showed an unexpected high number of false positives in these samples due to individual outliers that made a considerable number of errors in peripheral reaching. This study provides first empirical data from large control and patient groups for a screening procedure that seems to be widely used but rarely explicitly reported and prepares the grounds for its use as a standard tool for the description of patients who are included in single case or group studies addressing optic ataxia similar to the use of neglect, extinction, or apraxia screening tools.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Svenja Borchers
- Division of Neuropsychology, Department of Cognitive Neurology, Centre for Neurology, Hertie-Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Eberhard Karls University Tübingen, Germany
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Fautrelle L, Gueugnon M, Barbieri G, Bonnetblanc F. Inter-hemispheric remapping between arm proprioception and vision of the hand is disrupted by single pulse TMS on the left parietal cortex. Brain Cogn 2013; 82:146-51. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.03.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2012] [Revised: 02/22/2013] [Accepted: 03/19/2013] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
36
|
The role of the caudal superior parietal lobule in updating hand location in peripheral vision: further evidence from optic ataxia. PLoS One 2012; 7:e46619. [PMID: 23071599 PMCID: PMC3465337 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046619] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2012] [Accepted: 09/06/2012] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Patients with optic ataxia (OA), who are missing the caudal portion of their superior parietal lobule (SPL), have difficulty performing visually-guided reaches towards extra-foveal targets. Such gaze and hand decoupling also occurs in commonly performed non-standard visuomotor transformations such as the use of a computer mouse. In this study, we test two unilateral OA patients in conditions of 1) a change in the physical location of the visual stimulus relative to the plane of the limb movement, 2) a cue that signals a required limb movement 180° opposite to the cued visual target location, or 3) both of these situations combined. In these non-standard visuomotor transformations, the OA deficit is not observed as the well-documented field-dependent misreach. Instead, OA patients make additional eye movements to update hand and goal location during motor execution in order to complete these slow movements. Overall, the OA patients struggled when having to guide centrifugal movements in peripheral vision, even when they were instructed from visual stimuli that could be foveated. We propose that an intact caudal SPL is crucial for any visuomotor control that involves updating ongoing hand location in space without foveating it, i.e. from peripheral vision, proprioceptive or predictive information.
Collapse
|
37
|
Syndrome de Balint et fonctions spatiales du lobe pariétal. Rev Neurol (Paris) 2012; 168:741-53. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neurol.2012.08.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/07/2012] [Accepted: 08/17/2012] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
38
|
Vesia M, Crawford JD. Specialization of reach function in human posterior parietal cortex. Exp Brain Res 2012; 221:1-18. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-012-3158-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 108] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2011] [Accepted: 06/21/2012] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
39
|
Crawford JD, Henriques DYP, Medendorp WP. Three-dimensional transformations for goal-directed action. Annu Rev Neurosci 2011; 34:309-31. [PMID: 21456958 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113749] [Citation(s) in RCA: 124] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Much of the central nervous system is involved in visuomotor transformations for goal-directed gaze and reach movements. These transformations are often described in terms of stimulus location, gaze fixation, and reach endpoints, as viewed through the lens of translational geometry. Here, we argue that the intrinsic (primarily rotational) 3-D geometry of the eye-head-reach systems determines the spatial relationship between extrinsic goals and effector commands, and therefore the required transformations. This approach provides a common theoretical framework for understanding both gaze and reach control. Combined with an assessment of the behavioral, neurophysiological, imaging, and neuropsychological literature, this framework leads us to conclude that (a) the internal representation and updating of visual goals are dominated by gaze-centered mechanisms, but (b) these representations must then be transformed as a function of eye and head orientation signals into effector-specific 3-D movement commands.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Douglas Crawford
- York Centre for Vision Research, Canadian Action and Perception Network, and Departments of Psychology, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, M3J 1P3.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
40
|
Borchers S, Hauser TK, Himmelbach M. Bilateral hand representations in human primary proprioceptive areas. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49:3383-91. [PMID: 21864551 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.08.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/05/2011] [Revised: 08/05/2011] [Accepted: 08/09/2011] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Sensory representations in the postcentral gyrus are supposed to be strictly lateralised and to provide spatially unbiased representations of limb positions. However, electrophysiological and behavioural measurements in humans and non-human primates tentatively suggested some degree of bilateral processing even in early somatosensory areas. We report a patient who suffered a small and confined lesion of the hand area in the postcentral gyrus that resulted in a proprioceptive deficit without any concomitant primary motor impairment. We performed a finger position-matching task with target locations being defined proprioceptively. Without visual feedback of either hand, the patient demonstrated a significant leftward shift of perceived locations when reaching with the ipsilesional right hand to her contralesional left hand and an opposite rightward shift when reaching with the left hand to the position of the right hand. Although these directional errors improved when vision of the active hand was allowed, errors were still significantly larger than those of age-matched healthy controls with unconstrained view of the active contralesional hand. Reaching to visual targets without visual online feedback the patient revealed comparable errors with both hands. Reaching to visual targets with full visual feedback, she was as accurate as controls with either hand. In summary, our data demonstrate an effect of the right postcentral lesion on proprioceptive information processing for both hands. The results suggest an integration of contralateral and ipsilateral proprioceptive information already at this early processing stage possibly mediated by callosal connections.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Svenja Borchers
- Division of Neuropsychology, Hertie-Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Eberhard Karls University, Tübingen, Germany
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Programs for action in superior parietal cortex: A triple-pulse TMS investigation. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49:2391-9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2010] [Revised: 04/04/2011] [Accepted: 04/13/2011] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
42
|
Thompson AA, Henriques DY. The coding and updating of visuospatial memory for goal-directed reaching and pointing. Vision Res 2011; 51:819-26. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.01.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/26/2010] [Revised: 12/15/2010] [Accepted: 01/10/2011] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
43
|
Prevosto V, Graf W, Ugolini G. Proprioceptive pathways to posterior parietal areas MIP and LIPv from the dorsal column nuclei and the postcentral somatosensory cortex. Eur J Neurosci 2011; 33:444-60. [PMID: 21226771 DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07541.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) serves as an interface between sensory and motor cortices by integrating multisensory signals with motor-related information. Sensorimotor transformation of somatosensory signals is crucial for the generation and updating of body representations and movement plans. Using retrograde transneuronal transfer of rabies virus in combination with a conventional tracer, we identified direct and polysynaptic somatosensory pathways to two posterior parietal areas, the ventral lateral intraparietal area (LIPv) and the rostral part of the medial intraparietal area (MIP) in macaque monkeys. In addition to direct projections from somatosensory areas 2v and 3a, respectively, we found that LIPv and MIP receive disynaptic inputs from the dorsal column nuclei as directly as these somatosensory areas, via a parallel channel. LIPv is the target of minor neck muscle-related projections from the cuneate (Cu) and the external cuneate nuclei (ECu), and direct projections from area 2v, that likely carry kinesthetic/vestibular/optokinetic-related signals. In contrast, MIP receives major arm and shoulder proprioceptive inputs disynaptically from the rostral Cu and ECu, and trisynaptically (via area 3a) from caudal portions of these nuclei. These findings have important implications for the understanding of the influence of proprioceptive information on movement control operations of the PPC and the formation of body representations. They also contribute to explain the specific deficits of proprioceptive guidance of movement associated to optic ataxia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vincent Prevosto
- Laboratoire de Neurobiologie Cellulaire et Moléculaire (NBCM), FRE3295 CNRS, 91198 Gif sur Yvette, France
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
44
|
|
45
|
Correlated deficits of perception and action in optic ataxia. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49:131-7. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.11.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/11/2010] [Revised: 10/27/2010] [Accepted: 11/12/2010] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
46
|
Optic ataxia and Bálint's syndrome: neuropsychological and neurophysiological prospects. HANDBOOK OF CLINICAL NEUROLOGY 2010. [PMID: 18631703 DOI: 10.1016/s0072-9752(07)88020-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register]
|
47
|
Specificity of human parietal saccade and reach regions during transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Neurosci 2010; 30:13053-65. [PMID: 20881123 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1644-10.2010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 111] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Single-unit recordings in macaque monkeys have identified effector-specific regions in posterior parietal cortex (PPC), but functional neuroimaging in the human has yielded controversial results. Here we used on-line repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to determine saccade and reach specificity in human PPC. A short train of three TMS pulses (separated by an interval of 100 ms) was delivered to superior parieto-occipital cortex (SPOC), a region over the midposterior intraparietal sulcus (mIPS), and a site close to caudal IPS situated over the angular gyrus (AG) during a brief memory interval while subjects planned either a saccade or reach with the left or right hand. Behavioral measures then were compared to controls without rTMS. Stimulation of mIPS and AG produced similar patterns: increased end-point variability for reaches and decreased saccade accuracy for contralateral targets. In contrast, stimulation of SPOC deviated reach end points toward visual fixation and had no effect on saccades. Contralateral-limb specificity was highest for AG and lowest for SPOC. Visual feedback of the hand negated rTMS-induced disruptions of the reach plan for mIPS and AG, but not SPOC. These results suggest that human SPOC is specialized for encoding retinally peripheral reach goals, whereas more anterior-lateral regions (mIPS and AG) along the IPS possess overlapping maps for saccade and reach planning and are more closely involved in motor details (i.e., planning the reach vector for a specific hand). This work provides the first causal evidence for functional specificity of these parietal regions in healthy humans.
Collapse
|
48
|
Jones SAH, Henriques DYP. Memory for proprioceptive and multisensory targets is partially coded relative to gaze. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:3782-92. [PMID: 20934442 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/10/2010] [Revised: 09/21/2010] [Accepted: 10/01/2010] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
We examined the effect of gaze direction relative to target location on reach endpoint errors made to proprioceptive and multisensory targets. We also explored if and how visual and proprioceptive information about target location are integrated to guide reaches. Participants reached to their unseen left hand in one of three target locations (left of body midline, body midline, or right or body midline), while it remained at a target site (online), or after it was removed from this location (remembered), and also after the target hand had been briefly lit before reaching (multisensory target). The target hand was guided to a target location using a robot-generated path. Reaches were made with the right hand in complete darkness, while gaze was varied in one of four eccentric directions. Horizontal reach errors systematically varied relative to gaze for all target modalities; not only for visually remembered and online proprioceptive targets as has been found in previous studies, but for the first time, also for remembered proprioceptive targets and proprioceptive targets that were briefly visible. These results suggest that the brain represents the locations of online and remembered proprioceptive reach targets, as well as visual-proprioceptive reach targets relative to gaze, along with other motor-related representations. Our results, however, do not suggest that visual and proprioceptive information are optimally integrated when coding the location of multisensory reach targets in this paradigm.
Collapse
|
49
|
Prevosto V, Graf W, Ugolini G. Cerebellar inputs to intraparietal cortex areas LIP and MIP: functional frameworks for adaptive control of eye movements, reaching, and arm/eye/head movement coordination. Cereb Cortex 2010; 20:214-28. [PMID: 19465740 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhp091] [Citation(s) in RCA: 124] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Using retrograde transneuronal transfer of rabies virus in combination with a conventional tracer (cholera toxin B), we studied simultaneously direct (thalamocortical) and polysynaptic inputs to the ventral lateral intraparietal area (LIPv) and the medial intraparietal area (MIP) in nonhuman primates. We found that these areas receive major disynaptic inputs from specific portions of the cerebellar nuclei, the ventral dentate (D), and ventrolateral interpositus posterior (IP). Area LIPv receives inputs from oculomotor domains of the caudal D and IP. Area MIP is the target of projections from the ventral D (mainly middle third), and gaze- and arm-related domains of IP involved in reaching and arm/eye/head coordination. We also showed that cerebellar cortical "output channels" to MIP predominantly stem from posterior cerebellar areas (paramedian lobe/Crus II posterior, dorsal paraflocculus) that have the required connectivity for adaptive control of visual and proprioceptive guidance of reaching, arm/eye/head coordination, and prism adaptation. These findings provide important insight about the interplay between the posterior parietal cortex and the cerebellum regarding visuospatial adaptation mechanisms and visual and proprioceptive guidance of movement. They also have potential implications for clinical approaches to optic ataxia and neglect rehabilitation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vincent Prevosto
- Laboratoire de Neurobiologie Cellulaire et Moléculaire, UPR9040 CNRS, 1 av de la Terrasse, Gif sur Yvette, France
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
50
|
Beurze SM, Toni I, Pisella L, Medendorp WP. Reference frames for reach planning in human parietofrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 2010; 104:1736-45. [PMID: 20660416 DOI: 10.1152/jn.01044.2009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 65] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
To plan a reaching movement, the brain must integrate information about the spatial goal of the reach with positional information about the selected hand. Recent monkey neurophysiological evidence suggests that a mixture of reference frames is involved in this process. Here, using 3T functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we tested the role of gaze-centered and body-centered reference frames in reach planning in the human brain. Fourteen human subjects planned and executed arm movements to memorized visual targets, while hand starting position and gaze direction were monitored and varied on a trial-by-trial basis. We further introduced a variable delay between target presentation and movement onset to dissociate cerebral preparatory activity from stimulus- and movement-related responses. By varying the position of the target and hand relative to the gaze line, we distinguished cerebral responses that increased for those movements requiring the integration of peripheral target and hand positions in a gaze-centered frame. Posterior parietal and dorsal premotor areas showed such gaze-centered integration effects. In regions closer to the primary motor cortex, body-centered hand position effects were found. These results suggest that, in humans, spatially contiguous neuronal populations operate in different frames of reference, supporting sensorimotor transformations according to gaze-centered or body-centered coordinates. The former appears suited for calculating a difference vector between target and hand location, whereas the latter may be related to the implementation of a joint-based motor command.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S M Beurze
- Radboud University Nijmegen, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, P.O. Box 9104, NL-6500 HE, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|