1
|
Fiske A, Mortimer A, Collins-Jones L, de Klerk CCJM, Gattas SU, Dvergsdal H, Scerif G, Holmboe K. Inhibitory control development from infancy to early childhood: A longitudinal fNIRS study. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2025; 73:101557. [PMID: 40158324 PMCID: PMC11997363 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2025.101557] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2024] [Revised: 02/14/2025] [Accepted: 03/25/2025] [Indexed: 04/02/2025] Open
Abstract
The developmental period from infancy to early childhood is one of substantial change - in advancements in cognitive skills, such as early executive functions, but also in the maturation of the prefrontal and parietal cortices that parallel such advances. The current study aims to investigate the emergence and development of inhibitory control, a core executive function, from infancy to early childhood. We collected longitudinal functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) data from the same sample of participants at 10-months, 16-months, and 3½ years of age whilst they completed the Early Childhood Inhibitory Touchscreen Task. In our previous publications, we reported that 10-month-old infants recruited right lateralised regions of the prefrontal and parietal cortex when inhibition was required. Despite no change in response inhibition performance, 16-month-olds recruited broader and bilateral regions of the prefrontal and parietal cortex. Results of the current study found that 3½-year-olds activated regions of the right inferior parietal cortex and the right inferior frontal gyrus when inhibition was required. Response inhibition performance was significantly improved by early childhood, yet there was commonality in the brain regions recruited at 16-months and 3½ years. This could suggest that these brain regions are fundamental neural indices of inhibitory control, even from toddlerhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Abigail Fiske
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Medical Sciences Division, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom; Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, United Kingdom
| | - Alicia Mortimer
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Medical Sciences Division, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom; Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, United States
| | - Liam Collins-Jones
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - Carina C J M de Klerk
- Centre for Brain Science, Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Colchester, United Kingdom
| | - Sylvia Ulieta Gattas
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Medical Sciences Division, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom; Department of Psychology and Human Development, Institute of Education, Faculty of Education and Society, University College London, London, United Kingdom; School of Psychology, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Surrey, United Kingdom
| | - Henrik Dvergsdal
- Nord University Business School, Department of Entrepreneurship, Innovation and Organisation, Bodø, Norway
| | - Gaia Scerif
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Medical Sciences Division, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Karla Holmboe
- School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Palmer LRJ, Mareschal D, Dumontheil I. Shared neural correlates of interference control and response inhibition in adolescence and young adulthood. Neuropsychologia 2025; 215:109166. [PMID: 40348124 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2025.109166] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2024] [Revised: 03/25/2025] [Accepted: 05/06/2025] [Indexed: 05/14/2025]
Abstract
Inhibitory control (IC) is the ability to inhibit dominant or automatic behaviours, responses or thoughts, to allow for the selection of appropriate goal-directed responses. IC is a core executive function and has been associated with specific brain regions, such as the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) but also with a broader fronto-parietal network similar to the multiple demand network, which is thought to support the elaboration and maintenance of structured mental programs across a range of tasks. Here, functional magnetic resonance imaging data were collected from different IC tasks within the same participants to investigate similarities and differences in brain activation between tasks and age groups. Adolescents (11-15 years-old, n = 34) and adults (18-26, n = 33) completed block-design numerical Stroop and simple and complex Go/No-go (GNG) tasks. Univariate analyses showed large overlapping fronto-parietal activation in the Stroop and complex GNG tasks, with more limited activation in the simple GNG task. When compared to adolescents, adults showed greater increases in activation in the right IFG in the Stroop task and in temporo-parietal and precentral clusters in the complex GNG task. High multivariate similarity was observed across fronto-parietal regions between complex GNG and Stroop tasks, and between simple and complex GNG tasks, but was much lower between Stroop and simple GNG tasks. Adults showed greater similarity between complex GNG and Stroop tasks, suggesting increased reliance on shared neural processes across tasks, rather than increased specialisation of brain networks to specific aspects of IC over development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lucy R J Palmer
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck College, University of London, London, UK.
| | - Denis Mareschal
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck College, University of London, London, UK
| | - Iroise Dumontheil
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck College, University of London, London, UK; Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Nittel C, Hohmann DM, Jansen A, Sommer J, Krauß R, Völk M, Kamp-Becker I, Weber S, Becker K, Stroth S. Test-retest reliability of functional near infrared spectroscopy during tasks of inhibitory control and working memory. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 2025:111993. [PMID: 40280855 DOI: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2025.111993] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2023] [Revised: 03/25/2025] [Accepted: 04/09/2025] [Indexed: 04/29/2025]
Abstract
Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) has become a well-established tool for neuroscience research and been suggested as a potential biomarker during clinical assessment in individuals with mental disorders. Biomarker need to be objective indications of biological processes which can be measured accurately and reproducibly. Despite various applications in clinical research, test-retest reliability of the fNIRS signal has not yet been evaluated sufficiently. To assess reliability of the fNIRS signal during tasks of executive functions, a group of 34 healthy subjects (13 male, 21 female) were tested twice for inhibitory control and working memory. On a group level results show a specific activation pattern throughout the two sessions, reflecting a task-related frontal network associated with the assessed cognitive functions. On the individual level the retest reliability of the activation patterns were considerably lower and differed strongly between participants. In conclusion, the interpretation of fNIRS signal on a single subject level is partially hampered by its low reliability. More studies are needed to optimize the retest reliability of fNIRS and to be applied on a routine basis in developmental research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Clara Nittel
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Philipps University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany.
| | - Daniela Michelle Hohmann
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Philipps University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany; Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), Philipps University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany.
| | - Andreas Jansen
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Philipps University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany; Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), Philipps University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany; Core-Facility Brainimaging, Faculty of Medicine, Philipps University of Marburg, Germany
| | - Jens Sommer
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Philipps University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany; Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), Philipps University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany; Core-Facility Brainimaging, Faculty of Medicine, Philipps University of Marburg, Germany
| | - Ricarda Krauß
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Philipps University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
| | - Max Völk
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Philipps University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
| | - Inge Kamp-Becker
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Philipps University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany; Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), Philipps University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany
| | - Stefanie Weber
- Department of Pediatrics, University Hospital Marburg, Philipps University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
| | - Katja Becker
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Philipps University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany; Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), Philipps University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany
| | - Sanna Stroth
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Philipps University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Rico‐Picó J, Garcia‐de‐Soria Bazan MDC, Conejero Á, Moyano S, Hoyo Á, Ballesteros‐Duperón MDLÁ, Holmboe K, Rueda MR. Oscillatory But Not Aperiodic Frontal Brain Activity Predicts the Development of Executive Control From Infancy to Toddlerhood. Dev Sci 2025; 28:e13613. [PMID: 39923184 PMCID: PMC11807265 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13613] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2024] [Revised: 10/22/2024] [Accepted: 01/07/2025] [Indexed: 02/10/2025]
Abstract
Executive control (EC) emerges in the first year of life, with the ability to inhibit prepotent responses (inhibitory control [IC]) and to flexibly readapt (cognitive flexibility [CF]) steadily improving. Simultaneously, electrophysiological brain activity undergoes profound reconfiguration, which has been linked to individual variability in EC. However, most studies exploring this relationship have used relative/absolute power and tasks that combine different executive processes. In addition, brain activity conflates aperiodic and oscillatory activity, which hinders the interpretation of the relationship between power and cognition. In the current study, we used the Early Childhood Inhibitory Touchscreen Task (ECITT) to examine the development of EC skills from 9 to 16 months in a longitudinal sample, and related performance of the task to resting-state EEG (rs-EEG) power, separating oscillatory and aperiodic activity. Our results showed improvement in IC but not in CF with age. In addition, alpha and theta oscillatory activity were concurrent (9-mo.) and longitudinal predictors of CF in toddlerhood, whereas the aperiodic exponent of the EEG signal did not contribute to EC. These findings demonstrate the relevance of oscillatory brain activity for cognitive development and provide an early brain marker for the early development of EC.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Josué Rico‐Picó
- Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of GranadaGranadaSpain
- Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC)University of GranadaGranadaSpain
| | | | - Ángela Conejero
- Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC)University of GranadaGranadaSpain
- Department of Developmental PsychologyUniversity of GranadaGranadaSpain
| | - Sebastián Moyano
- Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of GranadaGranadaSpain
- Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC)University of GranadaGranadaSpain
| | - Ángela Hoyo
- Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of GranadaGranadaSpain
| | | | - Karla Holmboe
- School of Psychological ScienceUniversity of BristolBristolUK
| | - M. Rosario Rueda
- Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of GranadaGranadaSpain
- Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC)University of GranadaGranadaSpain
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Kelsey C, Kamenetskiy A, Mulligan K, Tiras C, Kent M, Bayet L, Richards J, Enlow MB, Nelson CA. Forming Connections: Functional Brain Connectivity is Associated With Executive Functioning Abilities in Early Childhood. Dev Sci 2025; 28:e13604. [PMID: 39740229 PMCID: PMC11753531 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13604] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2024] [Revised: 12/03/2024] [Accepted: 12/07/2024] [Indexed: 01/02/2025]
Abstract
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies with adults provide evidence that functional brain networks, including the default mode network and frontoparietal network, underlie executive functioning (EF). However, given the challenges of using fMRI with infants and young children, little work has assessed the developmental trajectories of these networks or their associations with EF at key developmental stages. More recently, functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) has emerged as a promising neuroimaging tool which can provide information on cortical functional networks and can be more easily implemented with young children. Children (N = 207; n = 116 male; n = 167 White) had fNIRS data recorded at infancy, 3, 5, and 7 years of age while watching a 2-min nonsocial video. At 3, 5, and 7 years, children completed behavioral assessments and parents completed questionnaires to assess child EF abilities. Results showed that, although early functional brain network connectivity was not associated with later functional brain connectivity, EF was concurrently and longitudinally associated with functional connectivity levels in both networks. Overall, these results inform the understanding of early emerging neural underpinnings of regulatory abilities and point to considerable change in the composition of functional brain networks and a conservation of function across development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caroline Kelsey
- Department of Pediatrics, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
- Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Adelia Kamenetskiy
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Kaitlin Mulligan
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Carly Tiras
- Department of Pediatrics, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Michaela Kent
- Schulich School of Medicine and Dentistry, Western University, London, ON, Canada
| | - Laurie Bayet
- Department of Neuroscience, American University, Washington, DC, United States
| | - John Richards
- Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC, United States
| | - Michelle Bosquet Enlow
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
- Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Charles A. Nelson
- Department of Pediatrics, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
- Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
- Harvard Graduate School of Education, Cambridge, MA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Macrae E, Milosavljevic B, Katus L, Mason L, Amadó MP, Rozhko M, de Haan M, Elwell CE, Moore SE, Lloyd-Fox S. Cognitive control in infancy: Attentional predictors using a tablet-based measure. INFANCY 2024; 29:631-655. [PMID: 38768285 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12599] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/13/2023] [Revised: 11/28/2023] [Accepted: 04/26/2024] [Indexed: 05/22/2024]
Abstract
Cognitive control is a predictor of later-life outcomes and may underpin higher order executive processes. The present study examines the development of early cognitive control during the first 24-month. We evaluated a tablet-based assessment of cognitive control among infants aged 18- and 24-month. We also examined concurrent and longitudinal associations between attentional disengagement, general cognitive skills and cognitive control. Participants (N = 60, 30 female) completed the tablet-task at 18- and 24-month of age. Attentional disengagement and general cognitive development were assessed at 5-, 8-, 12-, 18- and 24-month using an eye-tracking measure and the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL), respectively. The cognitive control task demonstrated good internal consistency, sensitivity to age-related change in performance and stable individual differences. No associations were found between infant cognitive control and MSEL scores longitudinally or concurrently. The eye-tracking task revealed that slower attentional disengagement at 8-month, but faster disengagement at 18-month, predicted higher cognitive control scores at 24-month. This task may represent a useful tool for measuring emergent cognitive control. The multifaceted relationship between attention and infant cognitive control suggests that the rapid development of the attentional system in infancy results in distinct attentional skills, at different ages, being relevant for cognitive control development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emma Macrae
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Bosiljka Milosavljevic
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
- Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK
| | - Laura Katus
- Institute for Lifecourse Development, University of Greenwich, London, UK
| | - Luke Mason
- Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, King's College London, London, UK
| | | | - Maria Rozhko
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Michelle de Haan
- Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health, University College London, London, UK
- Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children NHS Foundation Trust London, London, UK
| | - Clare E Elwell
- Department of Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London, UK
| | - Sophie E Moore
- Department of Women and Children's Health, King's College London, London, UK
- MRC Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Keneba, The Gambia
| | - Sarah Lloyd-Fox
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Bazelmans T, Scerif G, Holmboe K, Gonzalez‐Gomez N, Hendry A. Rates of family history of autism and ADHD varies with recruitment approach and socio-economic status. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2024; 42:117-132. [PMID: 37970752 PMCID: PMC11256865 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12469] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2023] [Revised: 09/23/2023] [Accepted: 11/03/2023] [Indexed: 11/17/2023]
Abstract
Family history (FH) of autism and ADHD is not often considered during the recruitment process of developmental studies, despite high recurrence rates. We looked at the rate of autism or ADHD amongst family members of young children (9 to 46 months) in three UK-based samples (N = 1055) recruited using different methods. The rate of FH-autism or FH-ADHD was 3%-9% for diagnosed cases. The rate was highest in the sample recruited through an online participant pool, which also consisted of the most socio-economically diverse families. Lower parental education and family income were associated with higher rates of FH-ADHD and lower parental education with increased FH-autism. Thus, recruitment strategies have a meaningful impact on neurodiversity and the conclusions and generalizations that can be drawn. Specifically, recruitment using crowdsourcing websites could create a sample that is more representative of the wider population, compared to those recruited through university-related volunteer databases and social media.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tessel Bazelmans
- Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and NeuroscienceKing's College LondonLondonUK
| | - Gaia Scerif
- Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of OxfordOxfordUK
| | - Karla Holmboe
- Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of OxfordOxfordUK
- School of Psychological ScienceUniversity of BristolBristolUK
| | - Nayeli Gonzalez‐Gomez
- Department of Psychology, Health and Professional DevelopmentOxford Brookes UniversityOxfordUK
| | - Alexandra Hendry
- Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of OxfordOxfordUK
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Li Y, Grabell AS, Perlman SB. Irritability Moderates the Association between Cognitive Flexibility Task Performance and Related Prefrontal Cortex Activation in Young Children. Brain Sci 2023; 13:882. [PMID: 37371362 PMCID: PMC10296206 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci13060882] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2023] [Revised: 05/24/2023] [Accepted: 05/29/2023] [Indexed: 06/29/2023] Open
Abstract
The association between cognitive flexibility and related neural functioning has been inconsistent. This is particularly true in young children, where previous studies have found heterogenous results linking behavior and neural function, raising the possibility of unexplored moderators. The current study explored the moderating role of dimensional irritability in the association between cognitive flexibility task performance and prefrontal activation in young children. A total of 106 3- to 7-year-old children were recruited to complete a custom-designed, child-adapted, cognitive flexibility task, and 98 of them were included in the data analysis. The children's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation was monitored using functional near-infrared spectroscopy, and their levels of irritability were reported by parents using the MAP-DB Temper Loss subscale. Results indicated that the mean reaction time of the cognitive flexibility task was negatively correlated with concurrent prefrontal activation. No evidence was found for the association between task accuracy and prefrontal activation. Moreover, irritability moderated the association between the mean reaction time and prefrontal activation. Children high in irritability exhibited a stronger negative association between the mean reaction time and related prefrontal activation than children low in irritability. The moderating model suggested a novel affective-cognitive interaction to investigate the associations between cognitive task performance and their neural underpinnings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yanwei Li
- College of Early Childhood Education, Nanjing Xiaozhuang University, Nanjing 210017, China
| | - Adam S. Grabell
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003, USA
| | - Susan B. Perlman
- Department of Psychiatry, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Moyano S, Rico-Picó J, Conejero Á, Hoyo Á, Ballesteros-Duperón MDLÁ, Rueda MR. Influence of the environment on the early development of attentional control. Infant Behav Dev 2023; 71:101842. [PMID: 37187034 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101842] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/23/2022] [Revised: 04/13/2023] [Accepted: 05/08/2023] [Indexed: 05/17/2023]
Abstract
The control of visual attention is key to learning and has a foundational role in the development of self-regulated behavior. Basic attention control skills emerge early in life and show a protracted development along childhood. Prior research suggests that attentional development is influenced by environmental factors in early and late childhood. Although, much less information is available about the impact of the early environment on emerging endogenous attention skills during infancy. In the current study we aimed to test the impact of parental socioeconomic status (SES) and home environment (chaos) in the emerging control of orienting in a sample of typically-developing infants. A group of 142 (73 female) 6-month-old infants were longitudinally tested at 6, 9 (n = 122; 60 female) and 16-18 (n = 91; 50 female) months of age using the gap-overlap paradigm. Median saccade latency (mdSL) and disengagement failure (DF) were computed as dependent variables for both overlap and gap conditions. Also, composite scores for a Disengagement Cost Index (DCI) and Disengagement Failure Index (DFI) were computed considering mdSL and DF of each condition, respectively. Families reported SES and chaos in the first and last follow-up sessions. Using Linear Mixed Models with Maximum Likelihood estimation (ML) we found a longitudinal decrease in mdSL in the gap but not in the overlap condition, while DF decreased with age independently of the experimental condition. Concerning early environmental factors, an SES index, parental occupation and chaos at 6 months were found to show a negative correlation with DFI at 16-18 months, although in the former case it was only marginally significant. Hierarchical regression models implementing ML showed that both SES and chaos at 6 months significantly predicted a lower DFI at 16-18 months. Results show a longitudinal progression of endogenous orienting between infancy and toddlerhood. With age, an increased endogenous control of orienting is displayed in contexts where visual disengagement is facilitated. Visual orienting involving attention disengagement in contexts of visual competition do not show changes with age. Moreover, these attentional mechanisms of endogenous control seem to be modulated by early experiences of the individual with the environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastián Moyano
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Granada, Granada, Spain; Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain.
| | - Josué Rico-Picó
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Granada, Granada, Spain; Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain
| | - Ángela Conejero
- Department of Developmental & Educational Psychology, University of Granada, Granada, Spain; Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain
| | - Ángela Hoyo
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Granada, Granada, Spain; Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain
| | | | - M Rosario Rueda
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Granada, Granada, Spain; Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Schroeder PA, Artemenko C, Kosie JE, Cockx H, Stute K, Pereira J, Klein F, Mehler DMA. Using preregistration as a tool for transparent fNIRS study design. NEUROPHOTONICS 2023; 10:023515. [PMID: 36908680 PMCID: PMC9993433 DOI: 10.1117/1.nph.10.2.023515] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2022] [Accepted: 01/11/2023] [Indexed: 05/04/2023]
Abstract
Significance The expansion of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) methodology and analysis tools gives rise to various design and analytical decisions that researchers have to make. Several recent efforts have developed guidelines for preprocessing, analyzing, and reporting practices. For the planning stage of fNIRS studies, similar guidance is desirable. Study preregistration helps researchers to transparently document study protocols before conducting the study, including materials, methods, and analyses, and thus, others to verify, understand, and reproduce a study. Preregistration can thus serve as a useful tool for transparent, careful, and comprehensive fNIRS study design. Aim We aim to create a guide on the design and analysis steps involved in fNIRS studies and to provide a preregistration template specified for fNIRS studies. Approach The presented preregistration guide has a strong focus on fNIRS specific requirements, and the associated template provides examples based on continuous-wave (CW) fNIRS studies conducted in humans. These can, however, be extended to other types of fNIRS studies. Results On a step-by-step basis, we walk the fNIRS user through key methodological and analysis-related aspects central to a comprehensive fNIRS study design. These include items specific to the design of CW, task-based fNIRS studies, but also sections that are of general importance, including an in-depth elaboration on sample size planning. Conclusions Our guide introduces these open science tools to the fNIRS community, providing researchers with an overview of key design aspects and specification recommendations for comprehensive study planning. As such it can be used as a template to preregister fNIRS studies or merely as a tool for transparent fNIRS study design.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Philipp A. Schroeder
- University of Tuebingen, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Science, Tuebingen, Germany
| | - Christina Artemenko
- University of Tuebingen, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Science, Tuebingen, Germany
| | - Jessica E. Kosie
- Princeton University, Social and Natural Sciences, Department of Psychology, Princeton, New Jersey, United States
| | - Helena Cockx
- Radboud University, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Biophysics Department, Faculty of Science, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Katharina Stute
- Chemnitz University of Technology, Institute of Human Movement Science and Health, Faculty of Behavioural and Social Sciences, Chemnitz, Germany
| | - João Pereira
- University of Coimbra, Coimbra Institute for Biomedical Imaging and Translational Research, Coimbra, Portugal
| | - Franziska Klein
- University of Oldenburg, Department of Psychology, Neurocognition and functional Neurorehabilitation Group, Oldenburg (Oldb), Germany
- RWTH Aachen University, Medical School, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Aachen, Germany
| | - David M. A. Mehler
- RWTH Aachen University, Medical School, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Aachen, Germany
- University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Medical School, Münster, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Holmboe K. We need to talk about validity - A commentary on "Six solutions for more reliable infant research" from the viewpoint of an early executive functions researcher. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2022; 31:e2352. [PMID: 36583147 PMCID: PMC9786875 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2352] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2022] [Revised: 05/13/2022] [Accepted: 05/17/2022] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
In their methodological article, "Six solutions for more reliable infant research", Byers-Heinlein, Bergmann and Savalei (2021) present compelling arguments for why developmental researchers should report and consider measures of reliability more frequently in their work. They also provide useful guidance on solutions to this "reliability crisis". In this commentary, I highlight a further methodological aspect that I think is key to successful and robust infancy research, that of construct validity. I also discuss recent reliability data from my own research on early executive function development, analyses which were directly inspired by the target article. Highlights Considering measurement reliability and effect sizes is important for robust infant research and for optimising infant tasks to measure group-level effects or individual differences.Construct validity - making sure that we measure what we think we are measuring - is also important.A robust effect at the group-level may not always restrict reliability - it depends on the amount of true variation between infants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karla Holmboe
- School of Psychological Science University of Bristol Bristol UK
| |
Collapse
|