1
|
Yıldırım Gökay N, Pula D, Karamert R, Gündüz B, Orhan E, Kabiş B, Gölaç H, Tutar V, Tutar H, Uğur MB. Sequential or simultaneous bilateral cochlear implantation: attention, memory, and language skills in children. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 2025; 282:1783-1790. [PMID: 39580553 DOI: 10.1007/s00405-024-09061-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2024] [Accepted: 10/28/2024] [Indexed: 11/25/2024]
Abstract
PURPOSE The effect of the inter-implant interval time on language and communication skills has been a subject of research for years in children with bilateral cochlear implants. This study aimed to investigate language and some attention and memory skills in children with bilateral sequential and simultaneous cochlear implants. METHOD The study included a total of 40 children aged between 6 and 8 years old, grouping bilateral simultaneous cochlear implanted children, sequential implanted children with 0-2 years interval, sequential implanted children with 2-4 years interval, and normal hearing peers. The language skills of the children were assessed using the Test of Language Development: Primary 4th edition, their selective attention and coping skills with disruptive effect were evaluated using the Stroop test, sustained attention and visual-motor synchronization skills were assessed using the Cancellation Test, and short-term memory skills were evaluated using the Visual Auditory Digit Span Test. The findings were analyzed using nonparametric statistical methods, with a significance level set at 0.05. RESULTS Children with bilateral simultaneous cochlear implants demonstrated better performance in language skills and the attention-related subtests of the Stroop test (p < 0.05). On the other hand, children with simultaneous and sequential cochlear implants achieved similar scores in the Cancellation and the Visual Auditory Digit Span Test (p > 0.05). CONCLUSIONS Bilateral simultaneous cochlear implantation in children is beneficial for language, selective attention, and coping with the Stroop effect. However, these children may still lag behind their normal-hearing peers in terms of language, attention, and memory skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nuriye Yıldırım Gökay
- Faculty of Health Sciences, Department of Audiology, Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey.
| | - Drilon Pula
- Faculty of Medicine, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Recep Karamert
- Faculty of Health Sciences, Department of Speech and Language Therapy, Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Bülent Gündüz
- Faculty of Health Sciences, Department of Audiology, Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Emre Orhan
- Faculty of Health Sciences, Department of Audiology, Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Burak Kabiş
- Faculty of Health Sciences, Department of Audiology, Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Hakan Gölaç
- Faculty of Health Sciences, Department of Speech and Language Therapy, Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Volkan Tutar
- Faculty of Health Sciences, Department of Audiology, Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Hakan Tutar
- Faculty of Medicine, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Mehmet Birol Uğur
- Faculty of Medicine, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Bal M, Kara Aydemir AG, Tepetaş Cengiz GŞ, Altındağ A. Examining the relationship between language development, executive function, and screen time: A systematic review. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0314540. [PMID: 39724067 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0314540] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/04/2024] [Accepted: 11/13/2024] [Indexed: 12/28/2024] Open
Abstract
This systematic review study examines the relationship between language development, executive function, and screen time in early childhood. The early childhood period is a crucial phase for the development of the brain, during which fundamental language and executive function skills undergo rapid evolution. This review synthesizes findings from 14 peer-reviewed studies that focused on language development, executive function, and screen time together to provide a comprehensive understanding of their relationship. The findings of current study were categorized under four themes: screen content and adherence to guidelines, parent-child interaction and the family context, passive and active screen time, and attention issues. The findings suggest that interactive and educational screen content may positively influence language development and executive functions when aligned with recommended screen time guidelines. In contrast, excessive passive screen time, such as watching television, has been associated with negative impacts on cognitive and social skills, particularly affecting attention, memory, and emotional regulation. The prevalence of attention problems is found to be higher in individuals who engage in high levels of screen time. This highlights the necessity for balanced consumption of screen media. The review emphasizes the pivotal role of parent-child interaction, where high-quality engagement and verbal scaffolding during screen time can mitigate adverse effects. Furthermore, socioeconomic and cultural factors also play a significant role. Higher socioeconomic status (SES) families are better able to manage screen time and leverage educational content to support development. These findings have the potential to inform the actions of parents, educators, and policymakers. Adherence to recommended screen time guidelines can mitigate the potential negative impact on executive functions and language skills. Furthermore, the importance of limiting passive screen time and ensuring a balance between screen use and real-world interactions and play opportunities is also highlighted.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mazhar Bal
- Department of Turkish and Social Sciences Education, Faculty of Education, Akdeniz University, Antalya, Turkey
| | | | - Gülüzar Şule Tepetaş Cengiz
- Child Development Department of Bolu Abant İzzet Baysal University, Mehmet Tanrıkulu Vocational School of Health Services, Bolu, Turkey
| | - Ahmet Altındağ
- Faculty of Education, Department of Educational Sciences, Bolu Abant İzzet Baysal University, Bolu, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Karadöller DZ, Sümer B, Ünal E, Özyürek A. Sign advantage: Both children and adults' spatial expressions in sign are more informative than those in speech and gestures combined. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2024; 51:876-902. [PMID: 36510476 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000922000642] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/17/2023]
Abstract
Expressing Left-Right relations is challenging for speaking-children. Yet, this challenge was absent for signing-children, possibly due to iconicity in the visual-spatial modality of expression. We investigate whether there is also a modality advantage when speaking-children's co-speech gestures are considered. Eight-year-old child and adult hearing monolingual Turkish speakers and deaf signers of Turkish-Sign-Language described pictures of objects in various spatial relations. Descriptions were coded for informativeness in speech, sign, and speech-gesture combinations for encoding Left-Right relations. The use of co-speech gestures increased the informativeness of speakers' spatial expressions compared to speech-only. This pattern was more prominent for children than adults. However, signing-adults and children were more informative than child and adult speakers even when co-speech gestures were considered. Thus, both speaking- and signing-children benefit from iconic expressions in visual modality. Finally, in each modality, children were less informative than adults, pointing to the challenge of this spatial domain in development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dilay Z Karadöller
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Netherlands
- Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University, Netherlands
| | - Beyza Sümer
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Netherlands
- Amsterdam Center for Language and Communication, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Ercenur Ünal
- Department of Psychology, Ozyegin University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Aslı Özyürek
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Netherlands
- Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University, Netherlands
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Yang Z, Zhang J, Wang M, Wang X, Liu H, Zhang F, Fan H. Prenatal endocrine-disrupting chemicals exposure and impact on offspring neurodevelopment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurotoxicology 2024; 103:335-357. [PMID: 39013523 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuro.2024.07.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/27/2024] [Revised: 06/11/2024] [Accepted: 07/10/2024] [Indexed: 07/18/2024]
Abstract
PURPOSE Considering that endocrine disruptors have certain effects on fetal growth, we conducted a systematic review of epidemiological literature to elucidate the correlation between exposure to endocrine-disrupting chemicals during pregnancy and the neurodevelopment of offspring. METHOD We systematically explored PubMed, Web of Science, and CINAHL databases from inception to April 4, 2023. References from pertinent studies were reviewed, and data regarding the link between maternal prenatal EDC exposure and offspring neurological development were compiled. A domain-based approach was used to evaluate studies of neurodevelopmental effects in children ≤3 years old by two reviewers, including cognition, motor, behavior, language, and non-verbal ability. RESULTS A comprehensive search yielded 45,373 articles, from which 48 articles, involving 26,005 mother-child pairs, met the criteria and were subsequently included in our analysis. The results revealed that EDC exposure during pregnancy had a significant impact on offspring neurobehavior development, especially in cognition, motor, and language. Our findings indicated adverse associations between prenatal exposure to metals and offspring cognition (before 12 months: β coefficient: -0.28; 95 % CI, -0.50 to -0.06; 1-3 years old: β coefficient: -0.55; 95 % CI: -1.08 to -0.02). Furthermore, metals (β coefficient: -0.71; 95 % CI: -1.23 to -0.19) and phthalates (β coefficient: -0.69; 95 % CI: -1.05 to -0.33) exposure exhibited detrimental effects on motor development from1-3 years old, while poly-fluoroalkyl substances were linked to the disruption of offspring language development (β coefficient: -1.01; 95 % CI: -1.90 to -0.11) within this timeframe. Additionally, exposure to EDCs during pregnancy had a negative impact on cognition development among girls from 12 to 36 months of age (β coefficient: -0.53; 95 % CI: -1.01 to -0.06). CONCLUSION Prenatal exposure to EDCs, especially metals, phthalates and, poly-fluoroalkyl substances, was associated with disrupting the development of offspring neurobehavior in the short and long term. Additionally, cognitive development showed gender differences due to prenatal endocrine-disrupting chemicals exposure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ziyi Yang
- School of Nursing and Rehabilitation, Nantong University, Nantong, Jiangsu, China
| | - Jie Zhang
- Medical School of Nantong University, Nantong, Jiangsu 226007, China
| | - Mingbo Wang
- School of Nursing and Rehabilitation, Nantong University, Nantong, Jiangsu, China
| | - Xin Wang
- School of Nursing and Rehabilitation, Nantong University, Nantong, Jiangsu, China
| | - Huahua Liu
- Nantong Maternity and Child Health Care Hospital Affiliated to Nantong University, Nantong, Jiangsu 226018, China
| | - Feng Zhang
- School of Nursing and Rehabilitation, Nantong University, Nantong, Jiangsu, China.
| | - Hong Fan
- School of Nursing and Rehabilitation, Nantong University, Nantong, Jiangsu, China.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Katus L, Crespo-Llado MM, Milosavljevic B, Saidykhan M, Njie O, Fadera T, McCann S, Acolatse L, Perapoch Amadó M, Rozhko M, Moore SE, Elwell CE, Lloyd-Fox S. It takes a village: Caregiver diversity and language contingency in the UK and rural Gambia. Infant Behav Dev 2024; 74:101913. [PMID: 38056188 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101913] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/09/2023] [Revised: 09/28/2023] [Accepted: 11/26/2023] [Indexed: 12/08/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION There is substantial diversity within and between contexts globally in caregiving practices and family composition, which may have implications for the early interaction's infants engage in. We draw on data from the Brain Imaging for Global Health (BRIGHT, www.globalfnirs.org/the-bright-project) project, which longitudinally examined infants in the UK and in rural Gambia, West Africa. In The Gambia, households are commonly characterized by multigenerational, frequently polygamous family structures, which, in part, is reflected in the diversity of caregivers a child spends time with. In this paper, we aim to 1) evaluate and validate the Language Environment Analysis (LENA) for use in the Mandinka speaking families in The Gambia, 2) examine the nature (i.e., prevalence of turn taking) and amount (i.e., adult and child vocalizations) of conversation that infants are exposed to from 12 to 24 months of age and 3) investigate the link between caregiver diversity and child language outcomes, examining the mediating role of contingent turn taking. METHOD We obtained naturalistic seven-hour-long LENA recordings at 12, 18 and 24 months of age from a cohort of N = 204 infants from Mandinka speaking households in The Gambia and N = 61 infants in the UK. We examined developmental changes and site differences in LENA counts of adult word counts (AWC), contingent turn taking (CTT) and child vocalizations (CVC). In the larger and more heterogenous Gambian sample, we also investigated caregiver predictors of turn taking frequency. We hereby examined the number of caregivers present over the recording day and the consistency of caregivers across two subsequent days per age point. We controlled for children's cognitive development via the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL). RESULTS Our LENA validation showed high internal consistency between the human coders and automated LENA outputs (Cronbach's alpha's all >.8). All LENA counts were higher in the UK compared to the Gambian cohort. In The Gambia, controlling for overall neurodevelopment via the MSEL, CTT at 12 and 18 months predicted CVC at 18 and 24 months. Caregiver consistency was associated with CTT counts at 18 and 24 months. The number of caregivers and CTT counts showed an inverted u-shape relationship at 18 and 24 months, with an intermediate number of caregivers being associated with the highest CTT frequencies. Mediation analyses showed a partial mediation by number of caregivers and CTT and 24-month CVC. DISCUSSION The LENA provided reliable estimates for the Mandinka language in the home recording context. We showed that turn taking is associated with subsequent child vocalizations and explored contextual caregiving factors contributing to turn taking in the Gambian cohort.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Katus
- Institute for Lifecourse Development, School of Human Sciences, University of Greenwich, UK; Centre for Family Research, University of Cambridge, UK.
| | | | - Bosiljka Milosavljevic
- Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, Queen Mary University of London, UK
| | - Mariama Saidykhan
- Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, UK
| | - Omar Njie
- Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, UK
| | - Tijan Fadera
- Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, UK
| | - Samantha McCann
- Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, UK; Department of Women and Children's Health, Kings College London, UK
| | - Lena Acolatse
- Nutrition Innovation Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), School of Biomedical Sciences, Ulster University, UK
| | | | - Maria Rozhko
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, UK
| | - Sophie E Moore
- Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, UK; Department of Women and Children's Health, Kings College London, UK
| | - Clare E Elwell
- Department of Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Barrett KJ, Moding KJ, Flesher A, Johnson SL. "The Kale Is Green" and Other Things Caregivers Say when Feeding Infants and Toddlers Are Associated with Child Rate of Acceptance. J Nutr 2023; 153:1297-1304. [PMID: 36803576 DOI: 10.1016/j.tjnut.2023.01.034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/01/2022] [Revised: 01/18/2023] [Accepted: 01/30/2023] [Indexed: 02/05/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Responsive feeding is important for helping children to develop healthy eating behaviors. Verbal feeding interactions between caregivers and children may reflect caregiver's responsiveness and contribute to children's developing lexical networks related to food and eating. OBJECTIVES This project aimed to: 1) characterize what caregivers say to infants and toddlers during a single feeding session and 2) test the associations between caregiver's verbal prompts and food acceptance by children. METHODS Filmed interactions of caregivers and their infants (N = 46 infants aged 6-11 mo) and toddlers (N = 60 toddlers aged 12-24 mo) were coded and analyzed to explore the following: 1) what caregivers said during a single feeding session and 2) whether caregiver's verbalizations were associated with child food acceptance. Caregiver verbal prompts were coded during each food offer and summed across the feeding session; prompts were categorized as supportive, engaging, and unsupportive. Outcomes included accepted tastes, rejected tastes, and rate of acceptance. Mann-Whitney's U tests and Spearman's correlations tested bivariate associations. Multilevel ordered logistic regression tested associations between verbal prompt categories and the rate of acceptance across offers. RESULTS Verbal prompts were largely supportive (41%) and engaging (46%), and caregivers of toddlers used significantly more verbal prompts than caregivers of infants (mean ± SD: 34.5 ± 16.9 compared with 25.2 ± 11.6; P = 0.006). Among toddlers, more engaging and unsupportive prompts were associated with a lower rate of acceptance (ρ = -0.30, P = 0.02; ρ = -0.37, P = 0.004). For all children, multilevel analyses revealed that more unsupportive verbal prompts were associated with a lower rate of acceptance (b = -1.52; SE = 0.62; P = 0.01) and individual caregiver use of more engaging and unsupportive prompts than usual was associated with a lower rate of acceptance (b = -0.33; SE = 0.08; P < 0.001: b = -0.58; SE = 0.11; P < 0.001). CONCLUSIONS These findings suggest that caregivers may strive for a supportive and engaging emotional setting during feeding, although verbalization category may change as children exhibit more rejection. Furthermore, what caregivers say may change as children develop more advanced language capabilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine J Barrett
- University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, Department of Pediatrics, Section of Nutrition, Aurora, CO, United States.
| | - Kameron J Moding
- Purdue University, Department of Human Development and Family Studies, West Lafayette, IN, United States
| | - Abigail Flesher
- University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, Department of Pediatrics, Section of Nutrition, Aurora, CO, United States
| | - Susan L Johnson
- University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, Department of Pediatrics, Section of Nutrition, Aurora, CO, United States
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Shapiro ALB, Lawless MC, Flesher A, Lattanzi K, Charlifue-Smith R, Johnson SL. Acceptance of a Novel Food is Related to Caregiver Perceptions of Infant and Toddler Food-related Receptive Language. JOURNAL OF NUTRITION EDUCATION AND BEHAVIOR 2022; 54:684-690. [PMID: 35643749 PMCID: PMC9682559 DOI: 10.1016/j.jneb.2022.04.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/11/2022] [Revised: 03/17/2022] [Accepted: 04/06/2022] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Language development, both what is understood (receptive language) and spoken (expressive language), is considered critical to a child's ability to understand and interact with their environment. However, little research has investigated the role children's early language skills might play in their food acceptance. The objective of this study was to explore the relationships between young children's food-related receptive language (FRL) and food-related expressive language (FEL) and acceptance of novel food. METHODS Caregivers (n = 54) reported their perceptions of children's (aged 7-24 months) FRL and FEL using the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory. Novel food acceptance was observed (grams consumed) during a laboratory visit. Multivariable linear regression tested associations between FRL, FEL, and novel food acceptance, by child age (infants [aged from 7 to < 12 months], toddlers [aged 12-24 months]), and at a significance level of P < 0.1 for hypothesis-generating research. RESULTS Children's FRL and food acceptance differed by age (F = 8.08, P = 0.01). Among toddlers, greater FRL was associated with greater novel food acceptance (0.22 g [95% confidence interval, -0.04 to 0.49]), P = 0.09). In infants, greater FRL was associated with lower novel food acceptance (-0.80 g [95% confidence interval, -1.53 to -0.07], P = 0.03). No association between FEL and novel food acceptance was noted in either group. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS Toddlers' understanding of food-related vocabulary may facilitate food acceptance; however, young infants may not yet have sufficient FRL to facilitate novel food acceptance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Allison L B Shapiro
- Department of Pediatrics, University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, Aurora, CO
| | - Megan C Lawless
- Department of Pediatrics, University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, Aurora, CO
| | - Abigail Flesher
- Department of Pediatrics, University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, Aurora, CO
| | - Kendal Lattanzi
- JFK Partners, University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, Aurora, CO
| | | | - Susan L Johnson
- Department of Pediatrics, University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, Aurora, CO.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Melzi G, Mesalles V, Caspe M, Prishker N. Spatial language during a household task with bilingual Latine families. JOURNAL OF APPLIED DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.appdev.2022.101409] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
9
|
Mazibuko XI, Chimbari M. The effect of schistosomiasis and soil-transmitted helminths on expressive language skills among African preschool children. BMC Infect Dis 2022; 22:264. [PMID: 35303827 PMCID: PMC8931967 DOI: 10.1186/s12879-022-07260-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/17/2020] [Accepted: 03/09/2022] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Schistosomiasis and soil transmitted helminths (STH) have been associated with compromised child development. We determined the effect of schistosomiasis and STH on expressive language skills among isiZulu speaking preschool children focusing on the variables: age, gender, school and stunting. Methods We quantitatively compared the performance of a cohort of infected and non-infected children using a 2 phased approach. In phase 1 infected children were treated with praziquantel and matched with non-infected children and both groups were tested for expressive language performance. In phase 2 both groups of children were re-tested for expressive language skills using a similar but modified test. The participants were 106 preschool children between the age of 4 and 6 years, 11 months. The Developmental Language Test was adapted as a linguistically and culturally appropriate tool for assessing isiZulu expressive language skills. Results The overall performance of the children in phases 1 and 2 were statistically similar. There was significant Pearson’s correlation of expressive language skills to age (0.002, P < 0.01), schistosomiasis i.e. vocabulary 1 (0.024, P < 0.05) and narrative skills (0.001, P < 0.01) and soil-transmitted helminths i.e. vocabulary 1 (0.006, P < 0.05), colours (0.029, P < 0.05) and narrative skills (0.001, P < 0.01) in phase 2 with small to high Cohen’s d effect size for various language subtests. Conclusion We concluded that even mild schistosomiasis and STH may compromise the performance of preschool children on expressive language. However poor ability in following instructions may have contributed to general poor performance across the two groups tested. Diet, school effect and stunting did not influence the performance of the children on expressive language. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s12879-022-07260-2.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xolisile I Mazibuko
- School of Nursing and Public Health, College of Health Sciences, University of KwaZulu-Natal UKZN, George Campbell Building, Howard College Campus, 4001, Durban, South Africa.
| | - Moses Chimbari
- School of Nursing and Public Health, College of Health Sciences, University of KwaZulu-Natal UKZN, George Campbell Building, Howard College Campus, 4001, Durban, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Russak S, Zaretsky E. Cognitive and Linguistic Skills Associated With Cross-Linguistic Transfer in the Production of Oral Narratives in English as a Foreign Language by Arabic- and Hebrew-Speaking Children: Finding Common Denominators. Front Psychol 2021; 12:664152. [PMID: 34434135 PMCID: PMC8381353 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.664152] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/04/2021] [Accepted: 07/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Many studies have examined literacy and related skills among learners of English as a foreign language (EFL), but little attention has been given to the role of oral language within a cross-linguistic framework despite the fact that English is the most widely spoken additional language today. Oral narratives rely on lexical, morphosyntactic, and conceptual knowledge. An in-depth examination of this modality can shed light on specific associations between cognitive and linguistic L1 and EFL skills and suggest possible mediating variables that assist multilingual speakers in producing complete oral narratives in EFL. The present study examined L1 and EFL contributors to EFL oral narratives produced by native Arabic (n = 85) and Hebrew (n = 86) speaking sixth graders seeking to identify cross-linguistic influences. We assessed general cognitive skills, phonological memory (PM), lexical, morphosyntactic knowledge, and reading comprehension in L1 (Hebrew speakers), Modern Standard Arabic (MSA, L2), L3 Hebrew (for Arabic speakers) and EFL. The “Cookie Theft” task assessed EFL elicited narratives using modified narrative analysis scales to account for microstructure (lexical and morphosyntactic complexity) and macrostructure (understanding story elements), generating a Total Narrative score. Our results yielded different patterns of underlying psycholinguistic profiles, and cross and within language associations for each group. Strong interactions between L1, L2/L3, and EFL morphological awareness and reading comprehension suggested cross-linguistic transfer. Regression analysis identified the most influential skills supporting EFL narratives for each linguistic group: English reading comprehension (ERC) was essential for Hebrew speakers and English morphological awareness (EMA) for Arabic ones. These results suggested different allocations of cognitive and linguistic resources in EFL narratives. The results also allowed to identify a common mediating skill for both groups. Findings are discussed within the theoretical framework of the Interdependence Hypothesis, the Linguistic Proximity Model, as well as accounts of direct and indirect transfer, which illuminate the impact of typological distance, general language proficiency and components of linguistic knowledge on cross-linguistic transfer in EFL oral language production.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susie Russak
- English Teacher Training Track, Faculty of Education, Beit Berl College, Kfar Saba, Israel
| | - Elena Zaretsky
- Department of Psychology, Clark University, Worcester, MA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Micheletti S, Galli J, Scaglioni V, Renzetti S, Scarano E, Foresti V, Fazzi E. Promoting Language Skills in Children With Neuromotor and Intellectual Disorders: Telepractice at the Time of SARS-CoV-2. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2021; 30:1866-1879. [PMID: 34232698 DOI: 10.1044/2021_ajslp-20-00222] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
Purpose The aim of this pilot study is to gather preliminary results on the effectiveness of intensive, parent-oriented, telepractice-based intervention to improve language skills in preschool children with neuromotor and intellectual disorders. Method Nine preschool children (M = 63 months, SD = 8.7 months) underwent a telepractice program 4 times a week designed to promote speech, lexical, and syntactic skills. Families were remotely connected from home with the therapists, who controlled the rehabilitation procedures from the hospital. The number of stable phonemes, of understood and repeated words, and of understood and repeated sentences were evaluated as outcome measures 3 months (prebaseline) and 1 week (baseline) before the intervention, immediately after the intervention (T1) and at a 3-month follow-up (T2). Results An increase in the number of stable phonemes was detected after the treatment, even if it was not statistically significant. After the intervention program, there was a significant increase in the number of understood words (ratio T1 vs. baseline: 1.33; 95% CI [1.03, 1.71]) and repeated words (ratio T1 vs. baseline: 1.39; 95% CI [1.00, 1.92]), as well as of understood sentences (ratio T1 vs. baseline: 1.80; 95% CI [1.24, 2.35]) and repeated sentences (ratio T1 vs. baseline: 4.23; 95% CI [1.96, 9.12]). No significant differences were found when comparing all the outcome measures at prebaseline and at baseline. Conclusion An intensive, parent-oriented, telepractice-based intervention has the potential to increase scores of lexical and syntactic tasks in children with neuromotor and intellectual disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Serena Micheletti
- Child Neurology and Psychiatry Unit, ASST Spedali Civili of Brescia, Italy
| | - Jessica Galli
- Child Neurology and Psychiatry Unit, ASST Spedali Civili of Brescia, Italy
- Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Italy
| | - Vera Scaglioni
- Child Neurology and Psychiatry Unit, ASST Spedali Civili of Brescia, Italy
| | - Stefano Renzetti
- Unit of Biostatistics and Bioinformatics, Department of Molecular and Translational Medicine, University of Brescia, Italy
| | - Elisa Scarano
- Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Italy
| | - Valentina Foresti
- Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Italy
| | - Elisa Fazzi
- Child Neurology and Psychiatry Unit, ASST Spedali Civili of Brescia, Italy
- Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Baxter P, Droop M, van den Hurk M, Bekkering H, Dijkstra T, Leoné F. Contrasting Similar Words Facilitates Second Language Vocabulary Learning in Children by Sharpening Lexical Representations. Front Psychol 2021; 12:688160. [PMID: 34295290 PMCID: PMC8290082 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.688160] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2021] [Accepted: 06/08/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
This study considers one of the cognitive mechanisms underlying the development of second language (L2) vocabulary in children: The differentiation and sharpening of lexical representations. We propose that sharpening is triggered by an implicit comparison of similar representations, a process we call contrasting. We investigate whether integrating contrasting in a learning method in which children contrast orthographically and semantically similar L2 words facilitates learning of those words by sharpening their new lexical representations. In our study, 48 Dutch-speaking children learned unfamiliar orthographically and semantically similar English words in a multiple-choice learning task. One half of the group learned the similar words by contrasting them, while the other half did not contrast them. Their word knowledge was measured immediately after learning as well as 1 week later. Contrasting was found to facilitate learning by leading to more precise lexical representations. However, only highly skilled readers benefitted from contrasting. Our findings offer novel insights into the development of L2 lexical representations from fuzzy to more precise, and have potential implications for education.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peta Baxter
- Donders Centre for Cognition, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Mienke Droop
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | | | - Harold Bekkering
- Donders Centre for Cognition, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Ton Dijkstra
- Donders Centre for Cognition, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Frank Leoné
- Donders Centre for Cognition, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Marinopoulou M, Billstedt E, Lin P, Hallerbäck M, Bornehag C. Number of words at age 2.5 years is associated with intellectual functioning at age 7 years in the SELMA study. Acta Paediatr 2021; 110:2134-2141. [PMID: 33686710 DOI: 10.1111/apa.15835] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2020] [Revised: 02/19/2021] [Accepted: 03/05/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
AIM We examined the association between the number of words used at age 2.5 years and deficits in intellectual functioning at age 7 years, in 549 children, and whether such association is confirmed by parental concern about the child's development. METHODS Parental reports of how many words their children used at age 2.5 years were analysed for the association to intellectual functioning (assessed with Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Fourth Edition,WISC-IV) at age 7 years using linear regression, adjusting for sex, maternal education level, parental IQ and smoking during pregnancy. Parental concern at age 7 years was examined with the Early Symptomatic Syndromes Eliciting Neurodevelopmental Clinical Examinations-Questionnaire (ESSENCE-Q). RESULTS Adjusted linear regression showed that use of 50 words or fewer at age 2.5 years, relative to use of more than 50 words, was associated with lower scores of Full-scale IQ (B = 7.27, p = 0.001), verbal comprehension (B = 8.53, p < 0.001), working memory (B = 9.04, p < 0.001) and perceptual reasoning (B = 4.21, p = 0.045), in the WISC-IV, at age 7 years. Parental concern was more common in the group that used 50 words or fewer (Mann-Whitney U test, p = 0.011). CONCLUSION This easily accessible measure of number of words seems to be a valuable marker for intellectual functioning later in life.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Marinopoulou
- Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology Sahlgrenska Academy University of Gothenburg Gothenburg Sweden
- Child and Adolescent Habilitation Region Värmland Karlstad Sweden
| | - Eva Billstedt
- Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology Sahlgrenska Academy University of Gothenburg Gothenburg Sweden
- Child Neuropsychiatric Clinic Sahlgrenska University Hospital Gothenburg Sweden
| | - Ping‐I Lin
- School of Psychiatry University of New South Wales Sydney NSW Australia
- South Western Sydney Local Health District Warwick Farm NSW Australia
| | - Maria Hallerbäck
- School of Medical Sciences Örebro University Örebro Sweden
- Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Region Värmland Karlstad Sweden
| | - Carl‐Gustaf Bornehag
- Karlstad University Karlstad Sweden
- Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai New York NY USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Ivanova AA, Mineroff Z, Zimmerer V, Kanwisher N, Varley R, Fedorenko E. The Language Network Is Recruited but Not Required for Nonverbal Event Semantics. NEUROBIOLOGY OF LANGUAGE (CAMBRIDGE, MASS.) 2021; 2:176-201. [PMID: 37216147 PMCID: PMC10158592 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/04/2020] [Accepted: 01/07/2021] [Indexed: 05/24/2023]
Abstract
The ability to combine individual concepts of objects, properties, and actions into complex representations of the world is often associated with language. Yet combinatorial event-level representations can also be constructed from nonverbal input, such as visual scenes. Here, we test whether the language network in the human brain is involved in and necessary for semantic processing of events presented nonverbally. In Experiment 1, we scanned participants with fMRI while they performed a semantic plausibility judgment task versus a difficult perceptual control task on sentences and line drawings that describe/depict simple agent-patient interactions. We found that the language network responded robustly during the semantic task performed on both sentences and pictures (although its response to sentences was stronger). Thus, language regions in healthy adults are engaged during a semantic task performed on pictorial depictions of events. But is this engagement necessary? In Experiment 2, we tested two individuals with global aphasia, who have sustained massive damage to perisylvian language areas and display severe language difficulties, against a group of age-matched control participants. Individuals with aphasia were severely impaired on the task of matching sentences to pictures. However, they performed close to controls in assessing the plausibility of pictorial depictions of agent-patient interactions. Overall, our results indicate that the left frontotemporal language network is recruited but not necessary for semantic processing of nonverbally presented events.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna A. Ivanova
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
- McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Zachary Mineroff
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Vitor Zimmerer
- Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, London, UK
| | - Nancy Kanwisher
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
- McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Rosemary Varley
- Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, London, UK
| | - Evelina Fedorenko
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
- McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Vaillant E, Geytenbeek JJM, Jansma EP, Oostrom KJ, Vermeulen RJ, Buizer AI. Factors associated with spoken language comprehension in children with cerebral palsy: a systematic review. Dev Med Child Neurol 2020; 62:1363-1373. [PMID: 32852786 PMCID: PMC7692918 DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.14651] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/13/2020] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
AIM To identify factors that are relevant for spoken language comprehension in children with cerebral palsy (CP), following the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health - Children and Youth (ICF-CY) framework. METHOD A systematic literature search was conducted using the electronic literature databases PubMed, Embase, PsycInfo, and Cochrane Library, from January 1967 to December 2019. Included studies involved children with CP, results regarding spoken language comprehension, and analysis of at least one associated factor. Factors were classified within ICF-CY domains. RESULTS Twenty-one studies met inclusion criteria. Factors in the ICF-CY domains of body functions and structure were most frequently reported. White brain matter abnormalities, motor type, functional mobility, and intellectual functioning appear to be relevant factors in spoken language comprehension in CP. Factors in the domain of activities and participation, as well as contextual factors, have rarely been studied in the context of spoken language comprehension in CP. INTERPRETATION Most factors known to be important for spoken language comprehension in typically developing children and/or known to be susceptible to change by interventions are understudied in CP.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emma Vaillant
- Department of Rehabilitation MedicineAmsterdam Movement SciencesAmsterdam UMCVrije Universiteit AmsterdamAmsterdamthe Netherlands
| | - Johanna J M Geytenbeek
- Department of Rehabilitation MedicineAmsterdam Movement SciencesAmsterdam UMCVrije Universiteit AmsterdamAmsterdamthe Netherlands
| | - Elise P Jansma
- Department of Epidemiology and BiostatisticsEMGO+ Institute for Health and Care Research and Medical LibraryAmsterdam UMCVrije Universiteit AmsterdamAmsterdamthe Netherlands
| | - Kim J Oostrom
- Psychosocial DepartmentAmsterdam Reproduction and DevelopmentEmma Children’s HospitalAmsterdam UMCUniversity of Amsterdam and Vrije Universiteit AmsterdamAmsterdamthe Netherlands
| | | | - Annemieke I Buizer
- Department of Rehabilitation MedicineAmsterdam Movement SciencesAmsterdam UMCVrije Universiteit AmsterdamAmsterdamthe Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
SÜmer B, ÖzyÜrek A. No effects of modality in development of locative expressions of space in signing and speaking children. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2020; 47:1101-1131. [PMID: 32178753 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000919000928] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
Linguistic expressions of locative spatial relations in sign languages are mostly visually motivated representations of space involving mapping of entities and spatial relations between them onto the hands and the signing space. These are also morphologically complex forms. It is debated whether modality-specific aspects of spatial expressions modulate spatial language development differently in signing compared to speaking children. In a picture description task, we compared the use of locative expressions for containment, support, and occlusion relations by deaf children acquiring Turkish Sign Language and hearing children acquiring Turkish (age 3;5-9;11). Unlike previous reports suggesting a boosting effect of iconicity, and/or a hindering effect of morphological complexity of the locative forms in sign languages, our results show similar developmental patterns for signing and speaking children's acquisition of these forms. Our results suggest the primacy of cognitive development guiding the acquisition of locative expressions by speaking and signing children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Beyza SÜmer
- Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Department of Linguistics, University of Amsterdam
| | - Aslı ÖzyÜrek
- Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Ashkenazi O, Gillis S, Ravid D. Input-output relations in Hebrew verb acquisition at the morpho-lexical interface. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2020; 47:509-532. [PMID: 31554527 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000919000540] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
This study examined early Hebrew verb acquisition, highlighting CDS-CS relations across inflectional and derivational verb learning. It was carried out on a corpus of longitudinal dense dyadic interactions of two Hebrew-speaking toddlers aged 1;8-2;2 and their parents. Findings revealed correlated patterns within and between CDS and CS corpora in terms of verbs, structural root categories, and their components (roots, binyan conjugations, and derivational verb families), and clear relations between lexical-derivational development and inflectional growth in input-output relations, measured by MSP. It also showed that both corpora had few, yet highly semantically coherent, derivational families. Lexical learning in Hebrew was shown to be morphologically oriented, with both inflectional and derivational learning supporting and being supported by the development of the verb lexicon. These findings support findings in the general literature regarding the close relationship between parental input and child speech, and the affinity between lexical and grammatical growth.
Collapse
|
18
|
Linguistic Diversity, Multilingualism, and Cognitive Skills: A Study of Disadvantaged Children in India. LANGUAGES 2020. [DOI: 10.3390/languages5010010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Multilingualism and linguistic diversity are the norm in India. Although studies have shown a relation between bilingualism and cognitive gains, linguistic diversity has so far been ignored as a potential factor affecting cognitive skills. This study aims to fill this gap by examining how cognitive skills—as measured by the n-back and Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices tasks—are affected by multilingualism and/or sociolinguistic diversity in a large cohort of socioeconomically disadvantaged primary school children in two urban sites of India: Delhi and Hyderabad. We present a questionnaire estimating sociolinguistic diversity and show that this measure assesses a distinct construct, as compared to a child’s multilingualism. Children were classified as growing up monolingually or bilingually, depending on whether they grew up with one or more languages in the home. Regarding cognitive performance, bilinguals were found to outperform monolinguals on the n-back task, as well as on the Raven’s task. In addition, a socially and linguistically diverse environment seems to enhance cognitive performance for children who are not multilingual themselves. Finally, several contextual factors such as city were found to influence cognitive performance. Overall, this shows that cognitive tasks are subject to contextual effects and that bilingualism and linguistic diversity can enhance cognitive performance of children in disadvantaged contexts.
Collapse
|
19
|
Pomiechowska B, Gliga T. Lexical Acquisition Through Category Matching: 12-Month-Old Infants Associate Words to Visual Categories. Psychol Sci 2018; 30:288-299. [DOI: 10.1177/0956797618817506] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Although it is widely recognized that human infants build a sizeable conceptual repertoire before mastering language, it remains a matter of debate whether and to what extent early conceptual and category knowledge contributes to language development. We addressed this question by investigating whether 12-month-olds used preverbal categories to discover the meanings of new words. We showed that one group of infants ( n = 18) readily extended novel labels to previously unseen exemplars of preverbal visual categories after only a single labeling episode, but two other groups struggled to do so when taught labels for unfamiliar categories (those who had been previously exposed, n = 18, or not exposed, n = 18, to category tokens). These results suggest that infants expect labels to denote categories of objects and are equipped with learning mechanisms responsible for matching prelinguistic knowledge structures with linguistic inputs. This ability is consistent with the idea that our conceptual machinery provides building blocks for vocabulary and language acquisition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara Pomiechowska
- Department of Cognitive Science, Cognitive Development Center, Central European University
| | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Han MJ, Kim SJ. Effects of Antiepileptic Drugs on Language Abilities in Benign Epilepsy of Childhood with Centrotemporal Spikes. J Clin Neurol 2018; 14:523-529. [PMID: 30198231 PMCID: PMC6172516 DOI: 10.3988/jcn.2018.14.4.523] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/08/2018] [Revised: 06/18/2018] [Accepted: 06/19/2018] [Indexed: 01/28/2023] Open
Abstract
Background and Purpose This study is to assess the responsiveness of electroencephalography (EEG) abnormalities and their effects on language ability after initiating different types of antiepileptic therapy in children with newly diagnosed benign epilepsy of childhood with centrotemporal spikes (BECTS). Methods The records of patients newly diagnosed with BECTS (n=120; 69 males) were reviewed retrospectively. The patients were randomly treated with lamotrigine, oxcarbazepine, or topiramate monotherapy, and underwent at least two EEG and standardized language tests. Effects were compared using Pearson's chi-square tests and paired t-tests. Results The recurrence rates for seizures in the lamotrigine, topiramate, and oxcarbazepine groups were 19.4%, 21.7%, and 11.4%, respectively, while complete or partial recovery (as indicated by EEG) occurred in 32%, 39%, and 16% of the patients. Patients in the lamotrigine group showed significant improvements in all parameters assessed by the Test of Language Problem Solving Abilities, except for ‘determining cause.’ Patients in the oxcarbazepine group also showed improvements, except for ‘making inferences’ (p<0.05). Most linguistic index scores were worse in the topiramate group except for Mean Length of Utterance in Words. Patients in the lamotrigine and oxcarbazepine groups showed significant improvements in the receptive language test (p<0.05). EEG improvements were not related to language ability. Conclusions The improvements in language and problem-solving performance in children with BECTS were greater for lamotrigine and oxcarbazepine than for topiramate. However, EEG remission did not imply that language function would be improved after the treatments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Min Jeong Han
- Department of Pediatrics, Chonbuk National University Medical School, Jeonju, Korea
| | - Sun Jun Kim
- Department of Pediatrics, Chonbuk National University Medical School, Jeonju, Korea.,Research Institute of Clinical Medicine of Chonbuk National University, Chonbuk National University Medical School, Jeonju, Korea.
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Moser A, Olsen S, Rusnak SN, Barr R, Gerhardstein P. How self-generated labelling shapes transfer of learning during early childhood: The role of individual differences. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2018; 37:68-83. [PMID: 29981173 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12254] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2017] [Revised: 05/31/2018] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Multiple factors influence imitation during toddlerhood, including task complexity, social contingency, and individual differences. We conducted a secondary data analysis of individual differences in self-generated labelling using data collected from a complex puzzle imitation task with 355 2- to 3-year-olds. This analysis indicated that toddlers' ability to label the completed puzzle (fish or boat) was associated with better imitation performance. Labelling occurs during social interactions; therefore, our second analysis tested how labelling differed as a function of the level of social scaffolding in each condition. This analysis revealed that self-generated labelling was lower when the social demonstrator was removed and the task was presented on a touchscreen. This study is one of the first to examine self-generated labelling during a complex imitation task in toddlers and increases our understanding of the complexity of memory processing needed for imitation learning. Statement of contribution What is already known on this subject? Toddlers exhibit a transfer of learning deficit from 2D media, including books, TV, and tablets. Self-generated labelling enhances children's learning, through attentional and cognitive mechanisms. Children are sensitive to reduced social cues in screen media contributing to the transfer deficit. What does this study add? Self-generated labelling is associated with better goal imitation performance. Self-generated labelling occurs more frequently under social conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alecia Moser
- Department of Psychology, Binghamton University, New York, USA
| | - Sarah Olsen
- Department of Psychology, Binghamton University, New York, USA
| | - Sylvia N Rusnak
- Department of Psychology, Georgetown University, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
| | - Rachel Barr
- Department of Psychology, Georgetown University, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Vavatzanidis NK, Mürbe D, Friederici AD, Hahne A. Establishing a mental lexicon with cochlear implants: an ERP study with young children. Sci Rep 2018; 8:910. [PMID: 29343736 PMCID: PMC5772553 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-18852-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2017] [Accepted: 12/18/2017] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
In the present study we explore the implications of acquiring language when relying mainly or exclusively on input from a cochlear implant (CI), a device providing auditory input to otherwise deaf individuals. We focus on the time course of semantic learning in children within the second year of implant use; a period that equals the auditory age of normal hearing children during which vocabulary emerges and extends dramatically. 32 young bilaterally implanted children saw pictures paired with either matching or non-matching auditory words. Their electroencephalographic responses were recorded after 12, 18 and 24 months of implant use, revealing a large dichotomy: Some children failed to show semantic processing throughout their second year of CI use, which fell in line with their poor language outcomes. The majority of children, though, demonstrated semantic processing in form of the so-called N400 effect already after 12 months of implant use, even when their language experience relied exclusively on the implant. This is slightly earlier than observed for normal hearing children of the same auditory age, suggesting that more mature cognitive faculties at the beginning of language acquisition lead to faster semantic learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Niki K Vavatzanidis
- Max Planck Institute for Human and Cognitive Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany. .,Saxonian Cochlear Implant Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany.
| | - Dirk Mürbe
- Saxonian Cochlear Implant Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
| | - Angela D Friederici
- Max Planck Institute for Human and Cognitive Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Anja Hahne
- Saxonian Cochlear Implant Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Strickland B, Chemla E. Cross-linguistic regularities and learner biases reflect "core" mechanics. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0184132. [PMID: 29324761 PMCID: PMC5764231 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0184132] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2015] [Accepted: 08/18/2017] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent research in infant cognition and adult vision suggests that the mechanical object relationships may be more salient and naturally attention grabbing than similar but non-mechanical relationships. Here we examine two novel sources of evidence from language related to this hypothesis. In Experiments 1 and 2, we show that adults preferentially infer that the meaning of a novel preposition refers to a mechanical as opposed to a non-mechanical relationship. Experiments 3 and 4 examine cross-linguistic adpositions obtained on a large scale from machines or from experts, respectively. While these methods differ in the ease of data collection relative to the reliability of the data, their results converge: we find that across a range of diverse and historically unrelated languages, adpositions (such as prepositions) referring to the mechanical relationships of containment (e.g “in”) and support (e.g. “on”) are systematically shorter than closely matched but not mechanical words such as “behind,” “beside,” “above,” “over,” “out,” and “off.” These results first suggest that languages regularly contain traces of core knowledge representations and that cross-linguistic regularities can therefore be a useful and easily accessible form of information that bears on the foundations of non-linguistic thought.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brent Strickland
- Département d'Etudes Cognitives, Ecole Normale Supérieure, PSL Research University, Paris, France
- Institut Jean Nicod (ENS, EHESS, CNRS), Paris, France
- * E-mail:
| | - Emmanuel Chemla
- Département d'Etudes Cognitives, Ecole Normale Supérieure, PSL Research University, Paris, France
- Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique (ENS, EHESS, CNRS), Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Stolzenberg SN, Lyon TD. 'Where were your clothes?' Eliciting descriptions of clothing placement from children alleging sexual abuse in criminal trials and forensic interviews. LEGAL AND CRIMINOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2017; 22:197-212. [PMID: 28890662 PMCID: PMC5588022 DOI: 10.1111/lcrp.12094] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/25/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The present study examined how children alleging sexual abuse are asked about clothing placement during abusive episodes, both in criminal trials and forensic interviews. The placement of clothing is of great importance, because it facilitates distinguishing abusive touch from non-abusive touch, as well as the severity of abuse when the touching is in fact sexual. If clothing has not been removed, then sexual abuse appears less likely and certain types of sexual contact are physically impossible (or at least highly improbable). METHODS We examined how trial attorneys (n = 142) and forensic interviewers in investigative interviews (n = 155) questioned 5- 12-year-olds about the location of clothing during alleged sexual abuse. To do so, we identified all question-answer pairs that included references to clothing placement, and coded for the clothing item mentioned, whether the interviewer elicited information about clothing placement or the child spontaneously provided such information, question-type, and response-type. RESULTS Discussions about clothing placement were commonplace in both settings, particularly in court. Fewer than one in five question-answer pairs about clothing placement were spontaneous mentions by children; the questioner elicited most discussions. When interviewers asked wh- questions rather than yes/no and forced-choice questions, children provided more elaboration, more detailed clothing information, and were over six times more likely to describe clothing placement in a fashion that could not be captured by a single preposition (e.g., neither on nor off). CONCLUSIONS The findings suggest that descriptions of clothing placement are subject to serious misinterpretation when closed-ended questions are asked.
Collapse
|
25
|
Göksun T, Hirsh-Pasek K, Michnick Golinkoff R. Trading Spaces: Carving up Events for Learning Language. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2017; 5:33-42. [DOI: 10.1177/1745691609356783] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Relational terms (e.g., verbs and prepositions) are the cornerstone of language development, bringing together two distinct fields: linguistic theory and infants’ event processing. To acquire relational terms such as run, walk, in, and on, infants must first perceive and conceptualize components of dynamic events such as containment—support, path—manner, source—goal, and figure—ground. Infants must then uncover how the particular language they are learning encodes these constructs. This review addresses the interaction of language learning with infants’ conceptualization of these nonlinguistic spatial event components. We present the thesis that infants start with language-general nonlinguistic constructs that are gradually refined and tuned to the requirements of their native language. In effect, infants are trading spaces, maintaining their sensitivity to some relational distinctions while dampening other distinctions, depending on how their native language expresses these constructs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tilbe Göksun
- Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA,
| | | | - Roberta Michnick Golinkoff
- School of Education and Departments of Psychology and Linguistics and Cognitive Science, University of Delaware, Newark
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Ferreira-Vasques AT, Abramides DVM, Lamônica DAC. Consideração da idade mental na avaliação do vocabulário expressivo de crianças com Síndrome de Down. REVISTA CEFAC 2017. [DOI: 10.1590/1982-0216201719216516] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/06/2023] Open
Abstract
RESUMO Objetivo: verificar vocabulário expressivo de crianças com Síndrome de Down, comparar ao desempenho de crianças com desenvolvimento típico de mesmo gênero em dois pareamentos distintos: considerando idade cronológica e mental, separadamente, e determinar a influência da idade considerada na avaliação desta população. Métodos: cumpriram-se aspectos éticos. Participaram 14 com Síndrome de Down, idade cronológica entre 38 a 63 meses, 14 com desenvolvimento típico pareado por gênero e idade mental e 14 com desenvolvimento típico pareado por gênero e idade cronológica. Após entrevista, aplicou-se Teste de Linguagem Infantil ABFW-Vocabulário Parte B. Para pareamento da idade mental utilizou-se o teste Stanford Binet. Aplicação do teste paramétrico ANOVA, teste Kruskal-Walis e Teste Tukey, quando necessário. Resultados: verificou-se desempenho inferior das crianças com Síndrome de Down comparadas ao grupo pareado por idade cronológica para nomeação correta e não nomeação das figuras. Não houve diferença significante entre o grupo com Síndrome de Down e com desenvolvimento típico pareado por idade mental. Conclusão: as crianças com Síndrome de Down apresentaram vocabulário expressivo aquém do esperado para a idade cronológica, porém próximo ao esperado para a idade mental, permitindo inferir que a idade considerada na avaliação da linguagem desta população interfere na análise dos resultados encontrados.
Collapse
|
27
|
Tzuriel D, Isman EB, Klung T, Haywood HC. Effects of Teaching Classification on Classification, Verbal Conceptualization, and Analogical Reasoning in Children With Developmental Language Delays. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE EDUCATION AND PSYCHOLOGY 2017. [DOI: 10.1891/1945-8959.16.1.107] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Children, 4–6 years of age, in special education kindergartens were randomly assigned to a classification training (n = 45) and a comparison (n = 49) group. Children in the training group were taught the Classification unit of Bright Start, whereas those in the comparison group received a regular content-oriented curriculum. Both groups were given pre- and posttests of classification, semantic categories, and conceptual and perceptual analogies. Children who received the classification training improved more on all tests than did those in the comparison group. Significant positive correlations were found between verbal conceptualization and classification, conceptual analogies, and perceptual analogies. Teaching classification appears to have effects that generalize to other domains of language and higher order thinking that are significant in the cognitive development of young children with developmental language delays. The findings support the interplay between thinking and language and positive cognitive developmental effects of training in classification.
Collapse
|
28
|
Hartshorne JK, O'Donnell TJ, Sudo Y, Uruwashi M, Lee M, Snedeker J. Psych verbs, the linking problem, and the acquisition of language. Cognition 2016; 157:268-288. [PMID: 27693942 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2014] [Revised: 08/06/2016] [Accepted: 08/16/2016] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
In acquiring language, children must learn to appropriately place the different participants of an event (e.g., causal agent, affected entity) into the correct syntactic positions (e.g., subject, object) so that listeners will know who did what to whom. While many of these mappings can be characterized by broad generalizations, both within and across languages (e.g., semantic agents tend to be mapped onto syntactic subjects), not all verbs fit neatly into these generalizations. One particularly striking example is verbs of psychological state: The experiencer of the state can appear as either the subject (Agnes fears/hates/loves Bartholomew) or the direct object (Agnes frightens/angers/delights Bartholomew). The present studies explore whether this apparent variability in subject/object mapping may actually result from differences in these verbs' underlying meanings. Specifically, we suggest that verbs like fear describe a habitual attitude towards some entity whereas verbs like frighten describe an externally caused emotional episode. We find that this distinction systematically characterizes verbs in English, Mandarin, and Korean. This pattern is generalized to novel verbs by adults in English, Japanese, and Russian, and even by English-speaking children who are just beginning to acquire psych verbs. This results support a broad role for systematic mappings between semantics and syntax in language acquisition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joshua K Hartshorne
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, United States; Department of Psychology, Harvard University, United States.
| | - Timothy J O'Donnell
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, United States; Department of Psychology, Harvard University, United States
| | - Yasutada Sudo
- Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, United States
| | - Miki Uruwashi
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, United States
| | - Miseon Lee
- Department of English Language and Literature, Hanyang University, Republic of Korea
| | - Jesse Snedeker
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, United States
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
van der Graaf J, Segers E, Verhoeven L. Scientific reasoning in kindergarten: Cognitive factors in experimentation and evidence evaluation. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.06.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
30
|
Urbain C, De Tiège X, Op De Beeck M, Bourguignon M, Wens V, Verheulpen D, Van Bogaert P, Peigneux P. Sleep in children triggers rapid reorganization of memory-related brain processes. Neuroimage 2016; 134:213-222. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/12/2016] [Accepted: 03/21/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022] Open
|
31
|
Rowe L, Jacobson R, Saylor MM. Differences in how monolingual and bilingual children learn second labels for familiar objects. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2015; 42:1219-1236. [PMID: 25630837 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000914000774] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
Monolingual children sometimes resist learning second labels for familiar objects. One explanation is that they are guided by word learning constraints that lead to the assumption that objects have only one name. It is less clear whether bilingual children observe this constraint. In the current study, we test the hypothesis that bilingual children might be more willing to accept second labels for objects and ask how they are affected by different amounts of information relevant to the second label. Although monolingual and bilingual children benefited from increased levels of information, only bilingual children chose the referent at above chance levels when they were offered increased levels of information. They were also more likely than monolingual children to accept second labels. Differences emerged even when English language vocabulary size was controlled for in the analyses.
Collapse
|
32
|
Geytenbeek JJM, Heim MJM, Knol DL, Vermeulen RJ, Oostrom KJ. Spoken language comprehension of phrases, simple and compound-active sentences in non-speaking children with severe cerebral palsy. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2015; 50:499-515. [PMID: 25703269 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12151] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2014] [Accepted: 11/03/2014] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Children with severe cerebral palsy (CP) (i.e. 'non-speaking children with severely limited mobility') are restricted in many domains that are important to the acquisition of language. AIMS To investigate comprehension of spoken language on sentence type level in non-speaking children with severe CP. METHODS & PROCEDURES From an original sample of 87 non-speaking children with severe CP, 68 passed the pre-test (i.e. they matched at least five spoken words to the corresponding objects) of a specifically developed computer-based instrument for low motor language testing (C-BiLLT), admitting them to the actual C-BiLLT computer test. As a result, the present study included 68 children with severe CP (35 boys, 33 girls; mean age 6;11 years, SD 3;0 years; age range 1;9-11;11 years) who were investigated with the C-BiLLT for comprehension of different sentence types: phrases, simple active sentences (with one or two arguments) and compound sentences. The C-BiLLT provides norm data of typically developing (TD) children (1;6-6;6 years). Binomial logistic regression analyses were used to compare the percentage correct of each sentence type in children with severe CP with that in TD children (subdivided into age groups) and to compare percentage correct within the CP subtypes. OUTCOMES & RESULTS Sentence comprehension in non-speaking children with severe CP followed the developmental trajectory of TD children, but at a much slower rate; nevertheless, they were still developing up to at least age 12 years. Delays in sentence type comprehension increased with sentence complexity and showed a large variability between individual children and between subtypes of CP. Comprehension of simple and syntactically more complex sentences were significantly better in children with dyskinetic CP than in children with spastic CP. Of the children with dyskinetic CP, 10-13% showed comprehension of simple and compound sentences within the percentage correct of TD children, as opposed to none of the children with spastic CP. CONCLUSION & IMPLICATIONS In non-speaking children with severe CP sentence comprehension is delayed rather than deviant. Results indicate the importance of following comprehension skills across all age groups, even beyond age 12 years. Moreover, the subtype of CP should be considered when establishing an educational programme for sentence comprehension, and augmentative and alternative communication support. In addition, educational programmes for children with severe CP should take into account the linguistic hierarchy of sentence comprehension when focusing on the input and understanding of spoken language comprehension.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joke J M Geytenbeek
- Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
- The EMGO Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
- Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | | | - Dirk L Knol
- University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - R Jeroen Vermeulen
- Department of Paediatric Neurology, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
- Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Kim J Oostrom
- Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
- Department of Pediatric Psychology, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Geytenbeek JJM, Vermeulen RJ, Becher JG, Oostrom KJ. Comprehension of spoken language in non-speaking children with severe cerebral palsy: an explorative study on associations with motor type and disabilities. Dev Med Child Neurol 2015; 57:294-300. [PMID: 25349105 DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.12619] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/22/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
AIM To assess spoken language comprehension in non-speaking children with severe cerebral palsy (CP) and to explore possible associations with motor type and disability. METHOD Eighty-seven non-speaking children (44 males, 43 females, mean age 6y 8mo, SD 2y 1mo) with spastic (54%) or dyskinetic (46%) CP (Gross Motor Function Classification System [GMFCS] levels IV [39%] and V [61%]) underwent spoken language comprehension assessment with the computer-based instrument for low motor language testing (C-BiLLT), a new and validated diagnostic instrument. A multiple linear regression model was used to investigate which variables explained the variation in C-BiLLT scores. Associations between spoken language comprehension abilities (expressed in z-score or age-equivalent score) and motor type of CP, GMFCS and Manual Ability Classification System (MACS) levels, gestational age, and epilepsy were analysed with Fisher's exact test. A p-value <0.05 was considered statistically significant. RESULTS Chronological age, motor type, and GMFCS classification explained 33% (R=0.577, R(2) =0.33) of the variance in spoken language comprehension. Of the children aged younger than 6 years 6 months, 52.4% of the children with dyskinetic CP attained comprehension scores within the average range (z-score ≥-1.6) as opposed to none of the children with spastic CP. Of the children aged older than 6 years 6 months, 32% of the children with dyskinetic CP reached the highest achievable age-equivalent score compared to 4% of the children with spastic CP. No significant difference in disability was found between CP-related variables (MACS levels, gestational age, epilepsy), with the exception of GMFCS which showed a significant difference in children aged younger than 6 years 6 months (p=0.043). INTERPRETATION Despite communication disabilities in children with severe CP, particularly in dyskinetic CP, spoken language comprehension may show no or only moderate delay. These findings emphasize the importance of introducing alternative and/or augmentative communication devices from early childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joke J M Geytenbeek
- Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; The EMGO Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Theakston AL, Ibbotson P, Freudenthal D, Lieven EVM, Tomasello M. Productivity of Noun Slots in Verb Frames. Cogn Sci 2015; 39:1369-95. [DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12216] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/23/2011] [Revised: 05/27/2014] [Accepted: 06/02/2014] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Paul Ibbotson
- Department of Childhood; Youth & Sport; The Open University
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Morgan G. On language acquisition in speech and sign: development of combinatorial structure in both modalities. Front Psychol 2014; 5:1217. [PMID: 25426085 PMCID: PMC4227467 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01217] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2014] [Accepted: 10/07/2014] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Languages are composed of a conventionalized system of parts which allow speakers and signers to generate an infinite number of form-meaning mappings through phonological and morphological combinations. This level of linguistic organization distinguishes language from other communicative acts such as gestures. In contrast to signs, gestures are made up of meaning units that are mostly holistic. Children exposed to signed and spoken languages from early in life develop grammatical structure following similar rates and patterns. This is interesting, because signed languages are perceived and articulated in very different ways to their spoken counterparts with many signs displaying surface resemblances to gestures. The acquisition of forms and meanings in child signers and talkers might thus have been a different process. Yet in one sense both groups are faced with a similar problem: “how do I make a language with combinatorial structure”? In this paper I argue first language development itself enables this to happen and by broadly similar mechanisms across modalities. Combinatorial structure is the outcome of phonological simplifications and productivity in using verb morphology by children in sign and speech.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gary Morgan
- Language and Communication Science, City University London, London UK
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Vos RC, Dallmeijer AJ, Verhoef M, Van Schie PEM, Voorman JM, Wiegerink DJHG, Geytenbeek JJM, Roebroeck ME, Becher JG. Developmental trajectories of receptive and expressive communication in children and young adults with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 2014; 56:951-9. [PMID: 24773274 DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.12473] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/07/2014] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
AIM The aim of this study was to determine the developmental trajectories of expressive (speech) and receptive (spoken and written language) communication by type of motor disorder and intellectual disability in individuals with cerebral palsy (CP). METHOD The development of 418 participants (261 males, 157 females; mean age 9y 6mo [SD 6y 2mo], range 1-24y; Gross Motor Function Classification System (GMFCS) level I [n=206], II [n=57], III [n=59], IV [n=54], V [n=42]) was followed for 2 to 4 years in a longitudinal study. Communication performance was measured using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. The type of motor disorder was differentiated by type of CP as unilateral spastic (USCP, n=161), bilateral spastic (BSCP, n=202), and non-spastic (NSCP, n=55), while intellectual disability was determined by IQ or school type (regular or special). A multilevel analysis was then used to model the developmental trajectories. RESULTS The most favourable development of expressive communication was seen in USCP (vs BSCP β [SE]-2.74 [1.06], NSCP β [SE]-2.67 [1.44]). The difference between the development trajectory levels of children with and without intellectual disability was smaller for children with USCP than for those with BSCP and NSCP. For receptive communication, the most favourable development was found for all children with USCP and for BSCP or NSCP without intellectual disability (vs intellectual disability β [SE]-4.00 [1.16]). Development of written language was most favourable for children without intellectual disability (vs intellectual disability β [SE]-23.11 [2.85]). INTERPRETATION The development of expressive communication was found to be most closely related to type of motor disorder, whereas the development of receptive communication was found to be most closely related to intellectual disability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rimke C Vos
- Department of Rehabilitation Medicine and EMGO+ Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
|
38
|
Henning E, Ragpot L. Pre-school children’s bridge to symbolic knowledge: first literature framework for a learning and cognition lab at a South African university. SOUTH AFRICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2014. [DOI: 10.1177/0081246314545199] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
The authors present the theoretical groundwork for a research project on learning and cognitive development of number concepts in the early years of childhood. Giving a background sketch of the genesis of a learning and cognition lab at a university in the metropolitan heartland of South Africa, they present their initial literature framework for inquiries into children’s symbolic learning of number in the pre-school years. They argue that conceptual development of young children is a neglected area in childhood cognition research in South Africa. The study of some of the literature for the first project of the new lab is then introduced with a view of identifying a few of the main components of a conceptual framework for what will become a multiple-year study. The authors propose that this literature can serve as foundation for examining a linguistically diverse group of children’s responses on experimental tasks and in clinical interviews in four or more languages. The designs of these inquiries are imminent. They suggest that the views of leading authors such as Elizabeth Spelke, Susan Carey, and Stanislas Dehaene can shed much light on their understanding of early number concept development of South African children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth Henning
- Centre for Education Practice Research, University of Johannesburg, South Africa
| | - Lara Ragpot
- Department of Childhood Education, University of Johannesburg, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Song L, Spier ET, Tamis-Lemonda CS. Reciprocal influences between maternal language and children's language and cognitive development in low-income families. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2014; 41:305-26. [PMID: 23360640 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000912000700] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/22/2023]
Abstract
We examined reciprocal associations between early maternal language use and children's language and cognitive development in seventy ethnically diverse, low-income families. Mother-child dyads were videotaped when children were aged 2;0 and 3;0. Video transcripts were analyzed for quantity and lexical diversity of maternal and child language. Child cognitive development was assessed at both ages and child receptive vocabulary was assessed at age 3;0. Maternal language related to children's lexical diversity at each age, and maternal language at age 2;0, was associated with children's receptive vocabulary and cognitive development at age 3;0. Furthermore, children's cognitive development at age 2;0 was associated with maternal language at age 3;0 controlling for maternal language at age 2;0, suggesting bi-directionality in mother-child associations. The quantity and diversity of the language children hear at home has developmental implications for children from low-income households. In addition, children's early cognitive skills further feed into their subsequent language experiences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lulu Song
- Brooklyn College of the City University of New York, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
40
|
Abstract
From at least two months onwards, infants can form perceptual categories. During the first year of life, object knowledge develops from the ability to represent individual object features to representing correlations between attributes and to integrate information from different sources. At the end of the first year, these representations are shaped by labels, opening the way to conceptual knowledge. Here, we review the development of object knowledge and object categorization over the first year of life. We then present an artificial neural network model that models the transition from early perceptual categorization to categories mediated by labels. The model informs a current debate on the role of labels in object categorization by suggesting that although labels do not act as object features they nevertheless affect perceived similarity of perceptually distinct objects sharing the same label. The model presents the first step of an integrated account from early perceptual categorization to language-based concept learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gert Westermann
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YW, UK
| | - Denis Mareschal
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck College, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, UK
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
MEG correlates of learning novel objects properties in children. PLoS One 2013; 8:e69696. [PMID: 23936082 PMCID: PMC3729701 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069696] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/18/2012] [Accepted: 06/13/2013] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Learning the functional properties of objects is a core mechanism in the development of conceptual, cognitive and linguistic knowledge in children. The cerebral processes underlying these learning mechanisms remain unclear in adults and unexplored in children. Here, we investigated the neurophysiological patterns underpinning the learning of functions for novel objects in 10-year-old healthy children. Event-related fields (ERFs) were recorded using magnetoencephalography (MEG) during a picture-definition task. Two MEG sessions were administered, separated by a behavioral verbal learning session during which children learned short definitions about the “magical” function of 50 unknown non-objects. Additionally, 50 familiar real objects and 50 other unknown non-objects for which no functions were taught were presented at both MEG sessions. Children learned at least 75% of the 50 proposed definitions in less than one hour, illustrating children's powerful ability to rapidly map new functional meanings to novel objects. Pre- and post-learning ERFs differences were analyzed first in sensor then in source space. Results in sensor space disclosed a learning-dependent modulation of ERFs for newly learned non-objects, developing 500–800 msec after stimulus onset. Analyses in the source space windowed over this late temporal component of interest disclosed underlying activity in right parietal, bilateral orbito-frontal and right temporal regions. Altogether, our results suggest that learning-related evolution in late ERF components over those regions may support the challenging task of rapidly creating new semantic representations supporting the processing of the meaning and functions of novel objects in children.
Collapse
|
42
|
Coventry K, Guijarro-Fuentes P, Valdés B. On the First and Second Language Acquisition of Spatial Language. SPATIAL COGNITION AND COMPUTATION 2012. [DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2012.713058] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
43
|
A lifespan perspective on semantic processing of concrete concepts: does a sensory/motor model have the potential to bridge the gap? COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2011; 11:551-72. [DOI: 10.3758/s13415-011-0053-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
44
|
Narasimhan B, Gullberg M. The role of input frequency and semantic transparency in the acquisition of verb meaning: evidence from placement verbs in Tamil and Dutch. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2011; 38:504-532. [PMID: 20609281 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000910000164] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/29/2023]
Abstract
We investigate how Tamil- and Dutch-speaking adults and four- to five-year-old children use caused posture verbs ('lay/stand a bottle on a table') to label placement events in which objects are oriented vertically or horizontally. Tamil caused posture verbs consist of morphemes that individually label the causal and result subevents (nikka veyyii 'make stand'; paDka veyyii 'make lie'), occurring in situational and discourse contexts where object orientation is at issue. Dutch caused posture verbs are less semantically transparent: they are monomorphemic (zetten 'set/stand'; leggen 'lay'), often occurring in contexts where factors other than object orientation determine use. Caused posture verbs occur rarely in Tamil input corpora; in Dutch input, they are used frequently. Elicited production data reveal that Tamil four-year-olds use infrequent placement verbs appropriately whereas Dutch children use high-frequency placement verbs inappropriately even at age five. Semantic transparency exerts a stronger influence than input frequency in constraining children's verb meaning acquisition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Marianne Gullberg
- Radboud University Nijmegen and Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Muentener P, Lakusta L. The intention-to-CAUSE bias: evidence from children's causal language. Cognition 2011; 119:341-55. [PMID: 21435638 PMCID: PMC3565381 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2009] [Revised: 01/20/2011] [Accepted: 01/30/2011] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
The current study explored causal language in 3.5- to 4-year-old children by manipulating the type of agent (human acting intentionally or unintentionally, or inanimate object) and the type of effect (motion or state change) in causal events. Experiment 1 found that the type of agent, but not the type of effect, influenced children's production of causal language. Children produced more causal language for intentionally caused events than for either unintentionally- or object-caused events, independent of the type of effect. Experiment 2, which tested children's judgments of descriptions for the events, found a similar pattern. Children preferred causal descriptions more for the intentionally caused events than the unintentionally- and the object-caused events. Experiment 3 found no evidence of bias in children's non-linguistic representations of the events. Taken together, these results suggest an intention-to-CAUSE bias in children's mapping of conceptual representations of causality into linguistic structure. We discuss the implications of these results for the acquisition of causal language and for the development of conceptual representations of causality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paul Muentener
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, United States, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
46
|
McGregor KK, Rohlfing KJ, Bean A, Marschner E. Gesture as a support for word learning: the case of under. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2009; 36:807-828. [PMID: 18947455 PMCID: PMC3328788 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000908009173] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/27/2023]
Abstract
ABSTRACTForty children, aged 1 ; 8-2 ; 0, participated in one of three training conditions meant to enhance their comprehension of the spatial term under: the +Gesture group viewed a symbolic gesture for under during training; those in the +Photo group viewed a still photograph of objects in the under relationship; those in the Model Only group did not receive supplemental symbolic support. Children's knowledge of under was measured before, immediately after, and two to three days after training. A gesture advantage was revealed when the gains exhibited by the groups on untrained materials (but not trained materials) were compared at delayed post-test (but not immediate post-test). Gestured input promoted more robust knowledge of the meaning of under, knowledge that was less tied to contextual familiarity and more prone to consolidation. Gestured input likely reduced cognitive load while emphasizing both the location and the movement relevant to the meaning of under.
Collapse
|
47
|
|
48
|
Rodekamp E, Harder T, Kohlhoff R, Dudenhausen JW, Plagemann A. Impact of breast-feeding on psychomotor and neuropsychological development in children of diabetic mothers: role of the late neonatal period. J Perinat Med 2007; 34:490-6. [PMID: 17140300 DOI: 10.1515/jpm.2006.095] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
AIM Previous data from our Kaulsdorf Cohort Study (KCS) suggest that early neonatal ingestion (1st week) of breast milk from diabetic mothers (diabetic breast milk, DBM) may increase the risk of being overweight and delay speech development in offspring of diabetic mothers (ODM). Late neonatal DBM ingestion (2nd-4th week), however, not independently influenced the risk of overweight. We investigated whether late neonatal DBM ingestion might independently influence neuro-development. METHODS Achievement of developmental milestones according to late neonatal DBM intake was analyzed in 242 ODM. RESULTS No impact of DBM ingestion on psychomotor parameters was observed. In contrast, it negatively influenced onset of speaking (no DBM: median 44.0 weeks, range 31.0-72.0; some DBM: 48.0, 24.0-100.0; DBM only: 52.0, 28.0-84.0; P=0.037) and halved the probability of reaching this milestone at any time point (hazard ratio: 0.53, 95% confidence interval: 0.31-0.91). However, adjustment for DBM volume ingested during the early neonatal period weakened the hazard ratio towards non-significance. In the fully adjusted model, the hazard ratio was halved, but insignificant. CONCLUSIONS Our results underscore that neonatal DBM ingestion, particularly during the first week of life, may delay speech development, an important indicator of cognitive development. Further studies are urgently recommended on consequences of breast-feeding for neurodevelopment in ODM.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elke Rodekamp
- Clinic of Obstetrics, Research Groups Experimental Obstetrics, Charité - University Medicine Berlin, Germany
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
49
|
Abstract
Spatial relations often are desired answers that a geographic information system (GIS) should generate in response to a user's query. Current GIS's provide only rudimentary support for processing and interpreting natural-language-like spatial relations, because their models and representations are primarily quantitative, while natural-language spatial relations are usually dominated by qualitative properties. Studies of the use of spatial relations in natural language showed that topology accounts for a significant portion of the geometric properties. This article develops a formal model that captures
metric details
for the description of natural-language spatial relations. The metric details are expressed as refinements of the categories identified by the 9-intersection, a model for topological spatial relations, and provide a more precise measure than does topology alone as to whether a geometric configuration matches with a spatial term or not. Similarly, these measures help in identifying the spatial term that describes a particular configuration. Two groups of metric details are derived:
splitting ratios
as the normalized values of lengths and areas of intersections; and
closeness measures
as the normalized distances between disjoint object parts. The resulting model of topological and metric properties was calibrated for 64 spatial terms in English, providing values for the best fit as well as value ranges for the significant parameters of each term. Three examples demonstrate how the framework and its calibrated values are used to determine the best spatial term for a relationship between two geometric objects.
Collapse
|