1
|
Pichkar Y, Surowiec A, Creanza N. Genetic and linguistic comparisons reveal complex sex-biased transmission of language features. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2024; 121:e2322881121. [PMID: 39556737 PMCID: PMC11621847 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2322881121] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2024] [Accepted: 08/26/2024] [Indexed: 11/20/2024] Open
Abstract
The history of people's movements and interactions shapes both genetic and linguistic variation. Genes and languages are transmitted separately and their distributions reflect different aspects of human history, but some demographic processes can cause them to be similarly distributed. In particular, forms of societal organization, including movements in and out of a community, may have shaped the transmission of both genes and languages. If children were more likely to learn their mother's language than their father's when their parents were from populations that spoke different languages or dialects, then language variation might show a closer association with maternally transmitted genetic markers than autosomal ones; this association could be further reinforced if children reside with predominantly maternal kin. We analyze the worldwide relationship between linguistic and genomic variation, leveraging the sex-biased transmission of X chromosomes to assess whether language has tended to be preferentially transmitted along the male or female line. In addition, we measure the effects of postmarital residence with female kin, matrilineal descent, and endogamy on the covariation of mitochondrial DNA and languages, using mtDNA because genomic data were available for very few populations with these ethnographic traits. We find that while there is little evidence for a consistent or widespread sex bias in the transmission of language, such biased transmission may have occurred locally in several parts of the world and might have been influenced by population-level ethnographic characteristics, such as female-based descent or residence patterns. Our results highlight the complex relationships between genes, language, ethnography, and geography.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yakov Pichkar
- Department of Biological Sciences, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN37240
- Evolutionary Studies Initiative, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN37240
| | - Alexandra Surowiec
- Department of Biological Sciences, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN37240
- Department of Anthropology, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA95616
| | - Nicole Creanza
- Department of Biological Sciences, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN37240
- Evolutionary Studies Initiative, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN37240
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Yang C, Zhang X, Yan S, Yang S, Wu B, You F, Cui Y, Xie N, Wang Z, Jin L, Xu S, Zhang M. Large-scale lexical and genetic alignment supports a hybrid model of Han Chinese demic and cultural diffusions. Nat Hum Behav 2024; 8:1163-1176. [PMID: 38740988 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-024-01886-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2023] [Accepted: 04/11/2024] [Indexed: 05/16/2024]
Abstract
The Han Chinese history is shaped by substantial demographic activities and sociocultural transmissions. However, it remains challenging to assess the contributions of demic and cultural diffusion to Han culture and language, primarily due to the lack of rigorous examination of genetic-linguistic congruence. Here we digitized a large-scale linguistic inventory comprising 1,018 lexical traits across 926 dialect varieties. Using phylogenetic analysis and admixture inference, we revealed a north-south gradient of lexical differences that probably resulted from historical migrations. Furthermore, we quantified extensive horizontal language transfers and pinpointed central China as a dialectal melting pot. Integrating genetic data from 30,408 Han Chinese individuals, we compared the lexical and genetic landscapes across 26 provinces. Our results support a hybrid model where demic diffusion predominantly impacts central China, while cultural diffusion and language assimilation occur in southwestern and coastal regions, respectively. This interdisciplinary study sheds light on the complex social-genetic history of the Han Chinese.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chengkun Yang
- State Key Laboratory of Genetic Engineering, Center for Evolutionary Biology, Human Phenome Institute, Zhangjiang Fudan International Innovation Center, School of Life Science, Fudan University, Shanghai, China
| | - Xiaoxi Zhang
- School of Life Science and Technology, ShanghaiTech University, Shanghai, China
- Key Laboratory of Computational Biology, Shanghai Institute of Nutrition and Health, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shanghai, China
| | - Shi Yan
- School of Ethnology and Sociology, Minzu University of China, Beijing, China
| | - Sizhe Yang
- State Key Laboratory of Genetic Engineering, Center for Evolutionary Biology, Human Phenome Institute, Zhangjiang Fudan International Innovation Center, School of Life Science, Fudan University, Shanghai, China
| | - Baihui Wu
- State Key Laboratory of Genetic Engineering, Center for Evolutionary Biology, Human Phenome Institute, Zhangjiang Fudan International Innovation Center, School of Life Science, Fudan University, Shanghai, China
| | - Fengshuo You
- Department of Chinese Language and Literature, Fudan University, Shanghai, China
| | - Yue Cui
- Department of Cultural Heritage and Museology, Fudan University, Shanghai, China
| | - Ni Xie
- Department of Linguistics and Modern Languages, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China
| | - Zhiyi Wang
- Department of Chinese Language and Literature, Fudan University, Shanghai, China
| | - Li Jin
- State Key Laboratory of Genetic Engineering, Center for Evolutionary Biology, Human Phenome Institute, Zhangjiang Fudan International Innovation Center, School of Life Science, Fudan University, Shanghai, China.
| | - Shuhua Xu
- State Key Laboratory of Genetic Engineering, Center for Evolutionary Biology, Human Phenome Institute, Zhangjiang Fudan International Innovation Center, School of Life Science, Fudan University, Shanghai, China.
- School of Life Science and Technology, ShanghaiTech University, Shanghai, China.
- Key Laboratory of Computational Biology, Shanghai Institute of Nutrition and Health, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shanghai, China.
| | - Menghan Zhang
- Institute of Modern Languages and Linguistics, Fudan University, Shanghai, China.
- Research Institute of Intelligent Complex Systems, Fudan University, Shanghai, China.
- Ministry of Education Key Laboratory of Contemporary Anthropology, Fudan University, Shanghai, China.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Passmore S, Wood ALC, Barbieri C, Shilton D, Daikoku H, Atkinson QD, Savage PE. Global musical diversity is largely independent of linguistic and genetic histories. Nat Commun 2024; 15:3964. [PMID: 38729968 PMCID: PMC11087526 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-48113-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/28/2023] [Accepted: 04/19/2024] [Indexed: 05/12/2024] Open
Abstract
Music is a universal yet diverse cultural trait transmitted between generations. The extent to which global musical diversity traces cultural and demographic history, however, is unresolved. Using a global musical dataset of 5242 songs from 719 societies, we identify five axes of musical diversity and show that music contains geographical and historical structures analogous to linguistic and genetic diversity. After creating a matched dataset of musical, genetic, and linguistic data spanning 121 societies containing 981 songs, 1296 individual genetic profiles, and 121 languages, we show that global musical similarities are only weakly and inconsistently related to linguistic or genetic histories, with some regional exceptions such as within Southeast Asia and sub-Saharan Africa. Our results suggest that global musical traditions are largely distinct from some non-musical aspects of human history.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sam Passmore
- Graduate School of Media and Governance, Keio University, Fujisawa, Japan.
- Evolution of Cultural Diversity Initiative (ECDI), Australian National University, Canberra, Australia.
| | | | - Chiara Barbieri
- Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Zurich, 8057, Switzerland
- Centre for the Interdisciplinary Study of Language Evolution (ISLE), University of Zurich, Zurich, 8050, Switzerland
- Department of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Cagliari, 09126, Cagliari, Italy
| | - Dor Shilton
- Cohn Institute for the History and Philosophy of Science and Ideas, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
- Edelstein Centre for the History and Philosophy of Science, Technology, and Medicine, Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
| | - Hideo Daikoku
- Graduate School of Media and Governance, Keio University, Fujisawa, Japan
| | | | - Patrick E Savage
- School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand.
- Faculty of Environment and Information Studies, Keio University, Fujisawa, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Wood ALC, Kirby KR, Ember CR, Silbert S, Passmore S, Daikoku H, McBride J, Paulay F, Flory MJ, Szinger J, D’Arcangelo G, Bradley KK, Guarino M, Atayeva M, Rifkin J, Baron V, El Hajli M, Szinger M, Savage PE. The Global Jukebox: A public database of performing arts and culture. PLoS One 2022; 17:e0275469. [PMID: 36322519 PMCID: PMC9629617 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0275469] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/26/2022] [Accepted: 09/14/2022] [Indexed: 11/07/2022] Open
Abstract
Standardized cross-cultural databases of the arts are critical to a balanced scientific understanding of the performing arts, and their role in other domains of human society. This paper introduces the Global Jukebox as a resource for comparative and cross-cultural study of the performing arts and culture. The Global Jukebox adds an extensive and detailed global database of the performing arts that enlarges our understanding of human cultural diversity. Initially prototyped by Alan Lomax in the 1980s, its core is the Cantometrics dataset, encompassing standardized codings on 37 aspects of musical style for 5,776 traditional songs from 1,026 societies. The Cantometrics dataset has been cleaned and checked for reliability and accuracy, and includes a full coding guide with audio training examples (https://theglobaljukebox.org/?songsofearth). Also being released are seven additional datasets coding and describing instrumentation, conversation, popular music, vowel and consonant placement, breath management, social factors, and societies. For the first time, all digitized Global Jukebox data are being made available in open-access, downloadable format (https://github.com/theglobaljukebox), linked with streaming audio recordings (theglobaljukebox.org) to the maximum extent allowed while respecting copyright and the wishes of culture-bearers. The data are cross-indexed with the Database of Peoples, Languages, and Cultures (D-PLACE) to allow researchers to test hypotheses about worldwide coevolution of aesthetic patterns and traditions. As an example, we analyze the global relationship between song style and societal complexity, showing that they are robustly related, in contrast to previous critiques claiming that these proposed relationships were an artifact of autocorrelation (though causal mechanisms remain unresolved).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna L. C. Wood
- Association for Cultural Equity (ACE), Hunter College, New York City, NY, United States of America
- Centro Studi Alan Lomax, Palermo, Italy
| | - Kathryn R. Kirby
- Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Jena, Germany
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| | - Carol R. Ember
- Human Relations Area Files at Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States of America
| | - Stella Silbert
- Association for Cultural Equity (ACE), Hunter College, New York City, NY, United States of America
| | - Sam Passmore
- Faculty of Environment and Information Studies, Keio University, Fujisawa, Japan
- Evolution of Cultural Diversity Initiative, School of Culture, History and Language, College of Asia and the Pacific, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Australia
| | - Hideo Daikoku
- Graduate School of Media and Governance, Keio University, Fujisawa, Japan
| | - John McBride
- Center for Soft & Living Matter, Institute for Basic Science, Daejeon, South Korea
| | - Forrestine Paulay
- Association for Cultural Equity (ACE), Hunter College, New York City, NY, United States of America
- Laban/Bartenieff Institute for Movement Studies, New York, NY, United States of America
| | - Michael J. Flory
- Research Design and Analysis Service, New York State Institute for Basic Research in Developmental Disabilities, Staten Island, NY, United States of America
| | - John Szinger
- Association for Cultural Equity (ACE), Hunter College, New York City, NY, United States of America
| | | | - Karen Kohn Bradley
- Laban/Bartenieff Institute for Movement Studies, New York, NY, United States of America
| | - Marco Guarino
- American Studies Program, University of Texas, Austin, Texas, United States of America
| | - Maisa Atayeva
- Paul H. Nitze School of Advanced International Studies, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, United States of America
| | - Jesse Rifkin
- Association for Cultural Equity (ACE), Hunter College, New York City, NY, United States of America
| | - Violet Baron
- University of Indiana, Folklore & Ethnomusicology, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America
| | - Miriam El Hajli
- Association for Cultural Equity (ACE), Hunter College, New York City, NY, United States of America
| | - Martin Szinger
- Association for Cultural Equity (ACE), Hunter College, New York City, NY, United States of America
| | - Patrick E. Savage
- Faculty of Environment and Information Studies, Keio University, Fujisawa, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Zhang M, Gong T. Structural Variability Shows Power-Law Based Organization of Vowel Systems. Front Psychol 2022; 13:801908. [PMID: 35237211 PMCID: PMC8882920 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.801908] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2021] [Accepted: 01/18/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Speech sounds are an essential vehicle of information exchange and meaning expression in approximately 7,000 spoken languages in the world. What functional constraints and evolutionary mechanisms lie behind linguistic diversity of sound systems is under ongoing debate; in particular, it remains conflicting whether there exists any universal relationship between these constraints despite of diverse sounds systems cross-linguistically. Here, we conducted cross-linguistic typological and phylogenetic analyses to address the characteristics of constraints on linguistic diversity of vowel systems. First, the typological analysis revealed a power-law based dependence between the global structural dispersion and the local focalization of vowel systems and validated that such dependence was independent of geographic region, language family, and linguistic affiliation. Second, the phylogenetic analysis further illustrated that the observed dependence resulted from correlated evolutions of these two structural properties, which proceeded in an adaptive process. These results provide empirical evidence that self-organization mechanisms helped shape vowel systems and common functional constraints took effect on the evolution of vowel systems in the world’s languages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Menghan Zhang
- Institute of Modern Languages and Linguistics, Fudan University, Shanghai, China
- Ministry of Education Key Laboratory of Contemporary Anthropology, Department of Anthropology and Human Genetics, School of Life Sciences, Fudan University, Shanghai, China
- Key Innovation Group of Digital Humanities Resource and Research, Shanghai Normal University, Shanghai, China
- *Correspondence: Menghan Zhang,
| | - Tao Gong
- School of Foreign Languages, Zhejiang University of Finance and Economics, Hangzhou, China
- Google LLC, New York, NY, United States
- Tao Gong,
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Savage PE, Passmore S, Chiba G, Currie TE, Suzuki H, Atkinson QD. Sequence alignment of folk song melodies reveals cross-cultural regularities of musical evolution. Curr Biol 2022; 32:1395-1402.e8. [PMID: 35120658 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.01.039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2021] [Revised: 09/07/2021] [Accepted: 01/13/2022] [Indexed: 01/18/2023]
Abstract
Culture evolves,1-5 but the existence of cross-culturally general regularities of cultural evolution is debated.6-8 As a diverse but universal cultural phenomenon, music provides a novel domain to test for the existence of such regularities.9-12 Folk song melodies can be thought of as culturally transmitted sequences of notes that change over time under the influence of cognitive and acoustic/physical constraints.9-15 Modeling melodies as evolving sequences constructed from an "alphabet" of 12 scale degrees16 allows us to quantitatively test for the presence of cross-cultural regularities using a sample of 10,062 melodies from musically divergent Japanese and English (British/American) folk song traditions.17,18 Our analysis identifies 328 pairs of highly related melodies, finding that note changes are more likely when they have smaller impacts on a song's melody. Specifically, (1) notes with stronger rhythmic functions are less likely to change, and (2) note substitutions are most likely between neighboring notes. We also find that note insertions/deletions ("indels") are more common than note substitutions, unlike genetic evolution where the reverse is true. Our results are consistent across English and Japanese samples despite major differences in their scales and tonal systems. These findings demonstrate that even a creative art form such as music is subject to evolutionary constraints analogous to those governing the evolution of genes, languages, and other domains of culture.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patrick E Savage
- Faculty of Environment and Information Studies, Keio University, Shonan Fujisawa Campus, Endo, Fujisawa, Kanagawa 252-0882, Japan; School of Anthropology and Museum Archaeology, University of Oxford, Banbury Road, Oxford OX2 6PE, UK; Department of Musicology, Tokyo University of the Arts, Uenokoen, Taito, Tokyo 110-8714, Japan.
| | - Sam Passmore
- Faculty of Environment and Information Studies, Keio University, Shonan Fujisawa Campus, Endo, Fujisawa, Kanagawa 252-0882, Japan
| | - Gakuto Chiba
- Faculty of Environment and Information Studies, Keio University, Shonan Fujisawa Campus, Endo, Fujisawa, Kanagawa 252-0882, Japan
| | - Thomas E Currie
- Centre for Ecology & Conservation, College of Life & Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Penryn Campus, Cornwall TR10 9FE, UK
| | - Haruo Suzuki
- Faculty of Environment and Information Studies, Keio University, Shonan Fujisawa Campus, Endo, Fujisawa, Kanagawa 252-0882, Japan
| | - Quentin D Atkinson
- School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Private Bag 92019, Symonds Street, Auckland 1010, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Leisterer-Peoples SM, Ross CT, Greenhill SJ, Hardecker S, Haun DBM. Games and enculturation: A cross-cultural analysis of cooperative goal structures in Austronesian games. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0259746. [PMID: 34818365 PMCID: PMC8612520 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0259746] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/16/2021] [Accepted: 10/25/2021] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
While most animals play, only humans play games. As animal play serves to teach offspring important life-skills in a safe scenario, human games might, in similar ways, teach important culturally relevant skills. Humans in all cultures play games; however, it is not clear whether variation in the characteristics of games across cultural groups is related to group-level attributes. Here we investigate specifically whether the cooperativeness of games covaries with socio-ecological differences across cultural groups. We hypothesize that cultural groups that engage in frequent inter-group conflict, cooperative sustenance acquisition, or that have less stratified social structures, might more frequently play cooperative games as compared to groups that do not share these characteristics. To test these hypotheses, we gathered data from the ethnographic record on 25 ethnolinguistic groups in the Austronesian language family. We show that cultural groups with higher levels of inter-group conflict and cooperative land-based hunting play cooperative games more frequently than other groups. Additionally, cultural groups with higher levels of intra-group conflict play competitive games more frequently than other groups. These findings indicate that games are not randomly distributed among cultures, but rather relate to the socio-ecological settings of the cultural groups that practice them. We argue that games serve as training grounds for group-specific norms and values and thereby have an important function in enculturation during childhood. Moreover, games might server an important role in the maintenance of cultural diversity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah M. Leisterer-Peoples
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
- Leipzig Research Center for Early Child Development, Universität Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Cody T. Ross
- Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Simon J. Greenhill
- Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Jena, Germany
- ARC Center of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, ANU College of Asia and the Pacific, Australian National University, Canberra, Australia
| | | | - Daniel B. M. Haun
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
- Leipzig Research Center for Early Child Development, Universität Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Youngblood M, Baraghith K, Savage PE. Phylogenetic reconstruction of the cultural evolution of electronic music via dynamic community detection (1975–1999). EVOL HUM BEHAV 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2021.06.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
9
|
Matsumae H, Ranacher P, Savage PE, Blasi DE, Currie TE, Koganebuchi K, Nishida N, Sato T, Tanabe H, Tajima A, Brown S, Stoneking M, Shimizu KK, Oota H, Bickel B. Exploring correlations in genetic and cultural variation across language families in northeast Asia. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2021; 7:eabd9223. [PMID: 34407936 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abd9223] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/15/2020] [Accepted: 05/20/2021] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
Culture evolves in ways that are analogous to, but distinct from, genomes. Previous studies examined similarities between cultural variation and genetic variation (population history) at small scales within language families, but few studies have empirically investigated these parallels across language families using diverse cultural data. We report an analysis comparing culture and genomes from in and around northeast Asia spanning 11 language families. We extract and summarize the variation in language (grammar, phonology, lexicon), music (song structure, performance style), and genomes (genome-wide SNPs) and test for correlations. We find that grammatical structure correlates with population history (genetic history). Recent contact and shared descent fail to explain the signal, suggesting relationships that arose before the formation of current families. Our results suggest that grammar might be a cultural indicator of population history while also demonstrating differences among cultural and genetic relationships that highlight the complex nature of human history.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hiromi Matsumae
- Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, 8057 Zurich, Switzerland.
- Kihara Institute for Biological Research, Yokohama City University, 641-12 Maioka-cho, Totsuka-ku, Yokohama, Kanagawa 244-0813, Japan
| | - Peter Ranacher
- Department of Geography, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstr. 190, 8057 Zurich, Switzerland.
- URPP Language and Space, University of Zurich, Freiestrasse 16, 8032 Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Patrick E Savage
- Faculty of Environment and Information Studies, Keio University, Shonan Fujisawa Campus, 5322 Endo, Fujisawa, Kanagawa 252-0882, Japan.
- Department of Musicology, Tokyo University of the Arts, 110-8714 Tokyo, Japan
| | - Damián E Blasi
- Department of Comparative Language Science, University of Zurich, Plattenstrasse 54, 8032 Zurich, Switzerland
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Peabody Museum, 5th Floor, 11 Divinity Avenue, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
- Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Kahlaische Str. 10, 07745 Jena, Germany
- Linguistic Convergence Laboratory, School of Linguistics, Faculty of Humanities, Higher School of Economics University, 21/4 Staraya Basmannaya Ulitsa, Building 5, Moscow, Russian Federation
- Human Relations Area Files, 755 Prospect Street, New Haven, CT, USA
| | - Thomas E Currie
- Human Behaviour & Cultural Evolution Group, Centre for Ecology & Conservation, Department of Biosciences, University of Exeter, Penryn Campus, Penryn, Cornwall TR10 9FE, UK
| | - Kae Koganebuchi
- Kitasato University Graduate School of Medical Science, Sagamihara, Kanagawa 252-0374, Japan
| | - Nao Nishida
- Genome Medical Science Project, Research Center for Hepatitis and Immunology, National Center for Global Health and Medicine, Chiba 272-8516, Japan
| | - Takehiro Sato
- Department of Bioinformatics and Genomics, Graduate School of Advanced Preventive Medical Sciences, Kanazawa University, 13-1 Takara-machi, Kanazawa, Ishikawa 920-8640, Japan
| | - Hideyuki Tanabe
- Department of Evolutionary Studies of Biosystems, School of Advanced Sciences, The Graduate University for Advanced Studies, SOKENDAI, Shonan Village, Hayama, Kanagawa 240-0193, Japan
| | - Atsushi Tajima
- Department of Bioinformatics and Genomics, Graduate School of Advanced Preventive Medical Sciences, Kanazawa University, 13-1 Takara-machi, Kanazawa, Ishikawa 920-8640, Japan
| | - Steven Brown
- Department of Psychology, Neuroscience & Behaviour, McMaster University, 1280 Main Street West, Hamilton, Ontario L8S 4K1, Canada
| | - Mark Stoneking
- Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, D04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Kentaro K Shimizu
- Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, 8057 Zurich, Switzerland
- Kihara Institute for Biological Research, Yokohama City University, 641-12 Maioka-cho, Totsuka-ku, Yokohama, Kanagawa 244-0813, Japan
- Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Language Evolution (ISLE), Plattenstrasse 54, 8032 Zürich, Switzerland
| | - Hiroki Oota
- Kitasato University Graduate School of Medical Science, Sagamihara, Kanagawa 252-0374, Japan.
- Kitasato University School of Medicine, Sagamihara, Kanagawa 252-0374, Japan
- Department of Biological Sciences, Graduate School of Science, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo 113-0033, Japan
| | - Balthasar Bickel
- Department of Comparative Language Science, University of Zurich, Plattenstrasse 54, 8032 Zurich, Switzerland.
- Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Language Evolution (ISLE), Plattenstrasse 54, 8032 Zürich, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Li M, DeRosa KL, Mann H, Spathis R, Roome A, Castellanos D, Gowen K, de Smet TS, Echard J, Casey K, Shamoon-Pour M, Garruto RM, Lum K. Recent kuru-induced female gene flow disrupted the coevolution of genes and languages in the Papua New Guinea highlands. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 2020; 172:87-98. [PMID: 32150286 DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.24047] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/09/2019] [Revised: 01/06/2020] [Accepted: 02/28/2020] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES The island of New Guinea was settled by modern human over 50,000 years ago, and is currently characterized by a complex landscape and contains one-seventh of the world's languages. The Eastern Highlands of New Guinea were also the home to the devastating prion disease called kuru that primarily affected Fore-speaking populations, with some 68% of cases involving adult females. We characterized the mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) diversity of highlanders from Papua New Guinea (PNG) to: (a) gain insight into the coevolution of genes and languages in situ over mountainous landscapes; and (b) evaluate the recent influence of kuru mortality on the pattern of female gene flow. MATERIALS AND METHODS We sequenced the mtDNA hypervariable segment 1 of 870 individuals from the Eastern and Southern Highlands of PNG using serums collected in the 1950s to 1960s. These highlanders were selected from villages representing 15 linguistic groups within the Trans-New Guinea phylum. Genetic, linguistic, and geographic distances were calculated separately and correlations among those distance matrices were assessed using the Mantel test. RESULTS Geographic, genetic, and linguistic patterns were independently correlated with each other (p < .05). Increased mtDNA diversity in kuru-affected populations and low Fst estimates between kuru-affected linguistic groups were observed. DISCUSSION In general, the genetic structure among the Highland populations was shaped by both geography and language, and language is a good predictor of mtDNA affinity in the PNG Highlands. High kuru female mortality increased female gene flow locally, disrupting coevolutionary pattern among genes, languages, and geography.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mian Li
- Department of Biological Sciences, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
- Laboratory of Evolutionary Anthropology and Health, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
| | - Kate L DeRosa
- Laboratory of Evolutionary Anthropology and Health, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
- Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
| | - Hayley Mann
- Laboratory of Evolutionary Anthropology and Health, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
- Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
| | - Rita Spathis
- Laboratory of Evolutionary Anthropology and Health, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
- Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
- Laboratory of Biomedical Anthropology and Neurosciences, Binghamton University, Binghamton, NY, United States
| | - Amanda Roome
- Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
- Laboratory of Biomedical Anthropology and Neurosciences, Binghamton University, Binghamton, NY, United States
- Bassett Healthcare Research Institute, Cooperstown, New York, United States
| | - Daniel Castellanos
- Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
| | - Kyle Gowen
- Laboratory of Evolutionary Anthropology and Health, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
- Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
| | - Timothy S de Smet
- Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
- Department of Geological Sciences and Environmental Studies, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
| | - Jessica Echard
- Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
| | - Kalen Casey
- Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
| | - Michel Shamoon-Pour
- Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
| | - Ralph M Garruto
- Department of Biological Sciences, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
- Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
- Laboratory of Biomedical Anthropology and Neurosciences, Binghamton University, Binghamton, NY, United States
| | - Koji Lum
- Department of Biological Sciences, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
- Laboratory of Evolutionary Anthropology and Health, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
- Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Beller S, Bender A, Jordan F. Editors' Review and Introduction: The Cultural Evolution of Cognition. Top Cogn Sci 2020; 12:644-653. [PMID: 32248636 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12498] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2019] [Accepted: 11/24/2019] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
Abstract
This topic addresses a question of key interest to cognitive science, namely which factors may have triggered, constrained, or shaped the course of cognitive evolution. It highlights the relevance of culture as a driving force in this process, with a special focus on social learning and language, conceptual tools, and material culture. In so doing, the topic combines two goals: to provide an overview of current empirical and theoretical work leading this field, tailored for a wider cognitive science audience, and to investigate the potential for integrating multiple perspectives across several timescales and levels of analysis, from the microlevel of individual behavior to the macrolevel of cultural change and language diversification. One key purpose is to assess the extent to which the different research approaches can cross-fertilize each other, thereby also contributing to the advancement of cognitive science more broadly.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sieghard Beller
- Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen.,SFF Centre for Early Sapiens Behaviour (SapienCE), University of Bergen
| | - Andrea Bender
- Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen.,SFF Centre for Early Sapiens Behaviour (SapienCE), University of Bergen
| | - Fiona Jordan
- Department of Archaeology and Anthropology, University of Bristol
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Bender A. What Early Sapiens Cognition Can Teach Us: Untangling Cultural Influences on Human Cognition Across Time. Front Psychol 2020; 11:99. [PMID: 32116913 PMCID: PMC7025490 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00099] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2019] [Accepted: 01/14/2020] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Evidence of cultural influences on cognition is accumulating, but untangling these cultural influences from one another or from non-cultural influences has remained a challenging task. As between-group differences are neither a sufficient nor a necessary indicator of cultural impact, cross-cultural comparisons in isolation are unable to furnish any cogent conclusions. This shortfall can be compensated by taking a diachronic perspective that focuses on the role of culture for the emergence and evolution of our cognitive abilities. Three strategies for reconstructing early human cognition are presented: the chaîne opératoire approach and its extension to brain-imaging studies, large-scale extrapolations, and phylogenetic comparative methods. While these strategies are reliant on our understanding of present-day cognition, they conversely also have the potential to advance this understanding in fundamental ways.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Bender
- SFF Centre for Early Sapiens Behaviour (SapienCE), University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
- Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Affiliation(s)
- Asifa Majid
- Department of Psychology, University of York, York, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Bender A, Beller S. The Cultural Fabric of Human Causal Cognition. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2019; 14:922-940. [DOI: 10.1177/1745691619863055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Causal cognition emerges early in development and confers an important advantage for survival. But does this mean that it is universal in humans? Our cross-disciplinary review suggests a broad evolutionary basis for core components of causal cognition but also underlines the essential role of culturally transmitted content as being uniquely human. The multiple ways in which both content and the key mechanisms of cultural transmission generate cultural diversity suggest that causal cognition in humans is not only colored by their specific cultural background but also shaped more fundamentally by the very fact that humans are a cultural species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Bender
- Department of Psychosocial Science and SFF Centre for Early Sapiens Behaviour (SapienCE), University of Bergen
| | - Sieghard Beller
- Department of Psychosocial Science and SFF Centre for Early Sapiens Behaviour (SapienCE), University of Bergen
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Rácz P, Passmore S, Sheard C, Jordan FM. Usage frequency and lexical class determine the evolution of kinship terms in Indo-European. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2019; 6:191385. [PMID: 31824734 PMCID: PMC6837234 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.191385] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2019] [Accepted: 09/23/2019] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
Languages do not replace their vocabularies at an even rate: words endure longer if they are used more frequently. This effect, which has parallels in evolutionary biology, has been demonstrated for the core vocabulary, a set of common, unrelated meanings. The extent to which it replicates in closed lexical classes remains to be seen, and may indicate how general this effect is in language change. Here, we use phylogenetic comparative methods to investigate the history of 10 kinship categories, a type of closed lexical class of content words, across 47 Indo-European languages. We find that their rate of replacement is correlated with their usage frequency, and this relationship is stronger than in the case of the core vocabulary, even though the envelope of variation is comparable across the two cases. We also find that the residual variation in the rate of replacement of kinship terms is related to genealogical distance of referent to kin. We argue that this relationship is the result of social changes and corresponding shifts in the entire semantic class of kinship terms, shifts typically not present in the core vocabulary. Thus, an understanding of the scope and limits of social change is needed to understand changes in kinship systems, and broader context is necessary to model cultural evolution in particular and the process of system change in general.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Péter Rácz
- Cognitive Development Center, Central European University, Budapest 1051, Hungary
- Department of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1UU, UK
| | - Sam Passmore
- Department of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1UU, UK
| | - Catherine Sheard
- School of Biology, University of St Andrews, St Andrews KY16 9TJ, UK
- School of Earth Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Fiona M. Jordan
- Department of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1UU, UK
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Bradley S, Thébault KPY. Models on the move: Migration and imperialism. STUDIES IN HISTORY AND PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE 2019; 77:81-92. [PMID: 31701883 DOI: 10.1016/j.shpsa.2017.11.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/24/2016] [Revised: 05/04/2017] [Accepted: 11/30/2017] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
We introduce 'model migration' as a species of cross-disciplinary knowledge transfer whereby the representational function of a model is radically changed to allow application to a new disciplinary context. Controversies and confusions that often derive from this phenomenon will be illustrated in the context of econophysics and phylogeographic linguistics. Migration can be usefully contrasted with the concept of 'imperialism', which has been influentially discussed in the context of geographical economics. In particular, imperialism, unlike migration, relies upon extension of the original model via an expansion of the domain of phenomena it is taken to adequately describe. The success of imperialism thus requires expansion of the justificatory sanctioning of the original idealising assumptions to a new disciplinary context. Contrastingly, successful migration involves the radical representational re-interpretation of the original model, rather than its extension. Migration thus requires 're-sanctioning' of new 'counterpart idealisations' to allow application to an entirely different class of phenomena. Whereas legitimate scientific imperialism should be based on the pursuit of some form of ontological unification, no such requirement is needed to legitimate the practice of model migration. The distinction between migration and imperialism will thus be shown to have significant normative as well as descriptive value.
Collapse
|
17
|
Bender A. The Role of Culture and Evolution for Human Cognition. Top Cogn Sci 2019; 12:1403-1420. [DOI: 10.1111/tops.12449] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2019] [Revised: 06/20/2019] [Accepted: 07/16/2019] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Bender
- Department of Psychosocial Science & SFF Centre for Early Sapiens Behaviour (SapienCE), University of Bergen
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Pacheco Coelho MT, Pereira EB, Haynie HJ, Rangel TF, Kavanagh P, Kirby KR, Greenhill SJ, Bowern C, Gray RD, Colwell RK, Evans N, Gavin MC. Drivers of geographical patterns of North American language diversity. Proc Biol Sci 2019; 286:20190242. [PMID: 30914010 PMCID: PMC6452074 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2019.0242] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2019] [Accepted: 03/06/2019] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Although many hypotheses have been proposed to explain why humans speak so many languages and why languages are unevenly distributed across the globe, the factors that shape geographical patterns of cultural and linguistic diversity remain poorly understood. Prior research has tended to focus on identifying universal predictors of language diversity, without accounting for how local factors and multiple predictors interact. Here, we use a unique combination of path analysis, mechanistic simulation modelling, and geographically weighted regression to investigate the broadly described, but poorly understood, spatial pattern of language diversity in North America. We show that the ecological drivers of language diversity are not universal or entirely direct. The strongest associations imply a role for previously developed hypothesized drivers such as population density, resource diversity, and carrying capacity with group size limits. The predictive power of this web of factors varies over space from regions where our model predicts approximately 86% of the variation in diversity, to areas where less than 40% is explained.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marco Túlio Pacheco Coelho
- Department of Human Dimensions of Natural Resources, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, USA
- Departamento de Ecologia, ICB, Universidade Federal de Goiás, 74.690-900 Goiânia, Goiás, Brazil
| | - Elisa Barreto Pereira
- Departamento de Ecologia, ICB, Universidade Federal de Goiás, 74.690-900 Goiânia, Goiás, Brazil
| | - Hannah J. Haynie
- Department of Human Dimensions of Natural Resources, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, USA
| | - Thiago F. Rangel
- Departamento de Ecologia, ICB, Universidade Federal de Goiás, 74.690-900 Goiânia, Goiás, Brazil
| | - Patrick Kavanagh
- Department of Human Dimensions of Natural Resources, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, USA
| | - Kathryn R. Kirby
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology and Department of Geography and Planning, University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Jena, Germany
| | - Simon J. Greenhill
- Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Jena, Germany
- CoEDL (ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language), Australian National University, Canberra, Australia
| | - Claire Bowern
- Department of Linguistics, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
| | - Russell D. Gray
- Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Jena, Germany
| | - Robert K. Colwell
- Departamento de Ecologia, ICB, Universidade Federal de Goiás, 74.690-900 Goiânia, Goiás, Brazil
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269, USA
- University of Colorado Museum of Natural History, Boulder, CO 80309, USA
| | - Nicholas Evans
- CoEDL (ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language), Australian National University, Canberra, Australia
| | - Michael C. Gavin
- Department of Human Dimensions of Natural Resources, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, USA
- Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Jena, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Jäger G. Global-scale phylogenetic linguistic inference from lexical resources. Sci Data 2018; 5:180189. [PMID: 30299438 PMCID: PMC6176785 DOI: 10.1038/sdata.2018.189] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/21/2018] [Accepted: 07/17/2018] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Automatic phylogenetic inference plays an increasingly important role in computational historical linguistics. Most pertinent work is currently based on expert cognate judgments. This limits the scope of this approach to a small number of well-studied language families. We used machine learning techniques to compile data suitable for phylogenetic inference from the ASJP database, a collection of almost 7,000 phonetically transcribed word lists over 40 concepts, covering two thirds of the extant world-wide linguistic diversity. First, we estimated Pointwise Mutual Information scores between sound classes using weighted sequence alignment and general-purpose optimization. From this we computed a dissimilarity matrix over all ASJP word lists. This matrix is suitable for distance-based phylogenetic inference. Second, we applied cognate clustering to the ASJP data, using supervised training of an SVM classifier on expert cognacy judgments. Third, we defined two types of binary characters, based on automatically inferred cognate classes and on sound-class occurrences. Several tests are reported demonstrating the suitability of these characters for character-based phylogenetic inference.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gerhard Jäger
- Tübingen University, Institute of Linguistics, Wilhelmstr. 19, 72074 Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Teixidor-Toneu I, Jordan FM, Hawkins JA. Comparative phylogenetic methods and the cultural evolution of medicinal plant use. NATURE PLANTS 2018; 4:754-761. [PMID: 30202108 DOI: 10.1038/s41477-018-0226-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2018] [Revised: 07/05/2018] [Accepted: 07/20/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
Human life depends on plant biodiversity and the ways in which plants are used are culturally determined. Whilst anthropologists have used phylogenetic comparative methods (PCMs) to gain an increasingly sophisticated understanding of the evolution of political, religious, social and material culture, plant use has been almost entirely neglected. Medicinal plants are of special interest because of their role in maintaining people's health across the world. PCMs in particular, and cultural evolutionary theory in general, provide a framework in which to study the diversity of medicinal plant applications cross-culturally, and to infer changes in plant use over time. These methods can be applied to single medicinal plants as well as the entire set of plants used by a culture for medicine, and they account for the non-independence of data when testing for floristic, cultural or other drivers of plant use. With cultural, biological and linguistic diversity under threat, gaining a deeper and broader understanding of the variation of medicinal plant use through time and space is pressing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Irene Teixidor-Toneu
- University of Reading, School of Biological Sciences, Reading, Berkshire, UK
- Universitetet i Oslo, Naturhistorisk Museum, Oslo, Norway
| | - Fiona M Jordan
- University of Bristol, Department of Anthropology & Archaeology, Bristol, UK
| | - Julie A Hawkins
- University of Reading, School of Biological Sciences, Reading, Berkshire, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Honkola T, Ruokolainen K, Syrjänen KJJ, Leino UP, Tammi I, Wahlberg N, Vesakoski O. Evolution within a language: environmental differences contribute to divergence of dialect groups. BMC Evol Biol 2018; 18:132. [PMID: 30176802 PMCID: PMC6122686 DOI: 10.1186/s12862-018-1238-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/21/2017] [Accepted: 08/13/2018] [Indexed: 02/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Background The processes leading to the diversity of over 7000 present-day languages have been the subject of scholarly interest for centuries. Several factors have been suggested to contribute to the spatial segregation of speaker populations and the subsequent linguistic divergence. However, their formal testing and the quantification of their relative roles is still missing. We focussed here on the early stages of the linguistic divergence process, that is, the divergence of dialects, with a special focus on the ecological settings of the speaker populations. We adopted conceptual and statistical approaches from biological microevolution and parallelled intra-lingual variation with genetic variation within a species. We modelled the roles of geographical distance, differences in environmental and cultural conditions and in administrative history on linguistic divergence at two different levels: between municipal dialects (cf. in biology, between individuals) and between dialect groups (cf. in biology, between populations). Results We found that geographical distance and administrative history were important in separating municipal dialects. However, environmental and cultural differences contributed markedly to the divergence of dialect groups. In biology, increase in genetic differences between populations together with environmental differences may suggest genetic differentiation of populations through adaptation to the local environment. However, our interpretation of this result is not that language itself adapts to the environment. Instead, it is based on Homo sapiens being affected by its environment, and its capability to adapt culturally to various environmental conditions. The differences in cultural adaptations arising from environmental heterogeneity could have acted as nonphysical barriers and limited the contacts and communication between groups. As a result, linguistic differentiation may emerge over time in those speaker populations which are, at least partially, separated. Conclusions Given that the dialects of isolated speaker populations may eventually evolve into different languages, our result suggests that cultural adaptation to local environment and the associated isolation of speaker populations have contributed to the emergence of the global patterns of linguistic diversity. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (10.1186/s12862-018-1238-6) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Terhi Honkola
- Department of Biology, FI-20014 University of Turku, Turku, Finland. .,Institute of Estonian and General Linguistics, Jakobi 2, University of Tartu, 51014, Tartu, Estonia.
| | - Kalle Ruokolainen
- Department of Geography and Geology, FI-20014 University of Turku, Turku, Finland
| | - Kaj J J Syrjänen
- Faculty of Communication Sciences, FI-33014 University of Tampere, Tampere, Finland
| | - Unni-Päivä Leino
- Faculty of Communication Sciences, FI-33014 University of Tampere, Tampere, Finland
| | - Ilpo Tammi
- Faculty of Communication Sciences, FI-33014 University of Tampere, Tampere, Finland.,Council of Tampere Region, FI-33201 Tampere, Tampere, Finland
| | - Niklas Wahlberg
- Department of Biology, FI-20014 University of Turku, Turku, Finland.,Department of Biology, Sölvegatan 37, Lund University, 223 62, Lund, Sweden
| | - Outi Vesakoski
- Department of Biology, FI-20014 University of Turku, Turku, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Greenhill SJ, Hua X, Welsh CF, Schneemann H, Bromham L. Population Size and the Rate of Language Evolution: A Test Across Indo-European, Austronesian, and Bantu Languages. Front Psychol 2018; 9:576. [PMID: 29755387 PMCID: PMC5934942 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00576] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/28/2017] [Accepted: 04/05/2018] [Indexed: 12/25/2022] Open
Abstract
What role does speaker population size play in shaping rates of language evolution? There has been little consensus on the expected relationship between rates and patterns of language change and speaker population size, with some predicting faster rates of change in smaller populations, and others expecting greater change in larger populations. The growth of comparative databases has allowed population size effects to be investigated across a wide range of language groups, with mixed results. One recent study of a group of Polynesian languages revealed greater rates of word gain in larger populations and greater rates of word loss in smaller populations. However, that test was restricted to 20 closely related languages from small Oceanic islands. Here, we test if this pattern is a general feature of language evolution across a larger and more diverse sample of languages from both continental and island populations. We analyzed comparative language data for 153 pairs of closely-related sister languages from three of the world's largest language families: Austronesian, Indo-European, and Niger-Congo. We find some evidence that rates of word loss are significantly greater in smaller languages for the Indo-European comparisons, but we find no significant patterns in the other two language families. These results suggest either that the influence of population size on rates and patterns of language evolution is not universal, or that it is sufficiently weak that it may be overwhelmed by other influences in some cases. Further investigation, for a greater number of language comparisons and a wider range of language features, may determine which of these explanations holds true.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Simon J Greenhill
- ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia.,Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History (MPG), Jena, Germany
| | - Xia Hua
- ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia.,Research School of Biology, Macroevolution and Macroecology, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia
| | - Caela F Welsh
- Research School of Biology, Macroevolution and Macroecology, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia
| | - Hilde Schneemann
- ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia.,Research School of Biology, Macroevolution and Macroecology, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia
| | - Lindell Bromham
- ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia.,Research School of Biology, Macroevolution and Macroecology, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Alemi R, Batouli SAH, Behzad E, Ebrahimpoor M, Oghabian MA. Not single brain areas but a network is involved in language: Applications in presurgical planning. Clin Neurol Neurosurg 2018; 165:116-128. [PMID: 29334640 DOI: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2018.01.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/21/2017] [Revised: 01/03/2018] [Accepted: 01/08/2018] [Indexed: 01/22/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Language is an important human function, and is a determinant of the quality of life. In conditions such as brain lesions, disruption of the language function may occur, and lesion resection is a solution for that. Presurgical planning to determine the language-related brain areas would enhance the chances of language preservation after the operation; however, availability of a normative language template is essential. PATIENTS AND METHODS In this study, using data from 60 young individuals who were meticulously checked for mental and physical health, and using fMRI and robust imaging and data analysis methods, functional brain maps for the language production, perception and semantic were produced. RESULTS The obtained templates showed that the language function should be considered as the product of the collaboration of a network of brain regions, instead of considering only few brain areas to be involved in that. CONCLUSION This study has important clinical applications, and extends our knowledge on the neuroanatomy of the language function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Razieh Alemi
- Department of Neuroscience and Addiction Studies, School of Advanced Technologies in Medicine, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran; Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, Canada
| | - Seyed Amir Hossein Batouli
- Department of Neuroscience and Addiction Studies, School of Advanced Technologies in Medicine, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran; Neuroimaging and Analysis Group, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Ebrahim Behzad
- Department of Neuroscience and Addiction Studies, School of Advanced Technologies in Medicine, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Mitra Ebrahimpoor
- Neuroimaging and Analysis Group, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Mohammad Ali Oghabian
- Neuroimaging and Analysis Group, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran; Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering Department, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran.
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Abstract
The extent to which nonhuman animals can learn actual human language is a controversial question, but many nonhuman species have acquired elements of a two-way communication system that is, and was, sophisticated enough to enable its use in evaluating cognitive capacities. This article is a personal view of the history of these animal language studies.
Collapse
|
25
|
Beller S, Bender A. Theory, the Final Frontier? A Corpus-Based Analysis of the Role of Theory in Psychological Articles. Front Psychol 2017. [PMID: 28642728 PMCID: PMC5462967 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00951] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Contemporary psychology regards itself as an empirical science, at least in most of its subfields. Theory building and development are often considered critical to the sciences, but the extent to which psychology can be cast in this way is under debate. According to those advocating a strong role of theory, studies should be designed to test hypotheses derived from theories (theory-driven) and ideally should yield findings that stimulate hypothesis formation and theory building (theory-generating). The alternative position values empirical findings over theories as the lasting legacy of science. To investigate which role theory actually plays in current research practice, we analyse references to theory in the complete set of 2,046 articles accepted for publication in Frontiers of Psychology in 2015. This sample of articles, while not representative in the strictest sense, covers a broad range of sub-disciplines, both basic and applied, and a broad range of article types, including research articles, reviews, hypothesis & theory, and commentaries. For the titles, keyword lists, and abstracts in this sample, we conducted a text search for terms related to empiricism and theory, assessed the frequency and scope of usage for six theory-related terms, and analyzed their distribution over different article types and subsections of the journal. The results indicate substantially lower frequencies of theoretical than empirical terms, with references to a specific (named) theory in less than 10% of the sample and references to any of even the most frequently mentioned theories in less than 0.5% of the sample. In conclusion, we discuss possible limitations of our study and the prospect of theoretical advancement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sieghard Beller
- Department of Psychosocial Science, Faculty of Psychology, University of BergenBergen, Norway
| | - Andrea Bender
- Department of Psychosocial Science, Faculty of Psychology, University of BergenBergen, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Wu Q, Liu W, Li C, Li X, Zhou P. A Price Paid for Our Internal Strife: Escalated Intragroup Aggression and the Evolution of Ingroup Derogation. Front Psychol 2016; 7:1453. [PMID: 27713725 PMCID: PMC5031712 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01453] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2016] [Accepted: 09/09/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
From evolutionary reasoning, we derived a novel hypothesis that ingroup derogation is an adaptation to a special ecological condition in which the greater threat of aggression is incurred by ingroup members. This hypothesis was tested and supported across five studies. Specifically, the computational modeling found that ingroup derogation could easily evolve if the chance of death incurred by intragroup conflicts was no less than 10%. Further behavioral experiments on Chinese participants showed that the ingroup derogation mechanism responded to heuristic social category cues and it responded more strongly when participants subjectively felt more vulnerable to interpersonal aggression, or when there were contextual cues of aggression in the immediate environment. Additional results showed that Chinese participants responded more strongly to aggression cues originating from ingroup members and that they endorsed more ingroup derogation attitudes even when the ingroup and outgroup members were both displaying cues of aggression. In addition, the results also revealed that the Chinese participants perceived more intentions of aggression from ingroup members than from outgroup members even in the absence of any clear signs of those intentions, and such a bias was positively correlated with ingroup derogation attitudes. Taken together, these results suggest that ingroup derogation is related to the evolved response of intragroup aggression management system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Qi Wu
- Cognition and Human Behavior Key Laboratory of Hunan Province, Department of Psychology, Hunan Normal University Changsha, China
| | - Wang Liu
- Cognition and Human Behavior Key Laboratory of Hunan Province, Department of Psychology, Hunan Normal University Changsha, China
| | - Chen Li
- Cognition and Human Behavior Key Laboratory of Hunan Province, Department of Psychology, Hunan Normal University Changsha, China
| | - Xiongfeng Li
- Cognition and Human Behavior Key Laboratory of Hunan Province, Department of Psychology, Hunan Normal University Changsha, China
| | - Ping Zhou
- Cognition and Human Behavior Key Laboratory of Hunan Province, Department of Psychology, Hunan Normal University Changsha, China
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Bender A, Beller S. Current Perspectives on Cognitive Diversity. Front Psychol 2016; 7:509. [PMID: 27148118 PMCID: PMC4828464 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00509] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/10/2015] [Accepted: 03/24/2016] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
To what extent is cognition influenced by a person’s cultural background? This question has remained controversial in large fields of the cognitive sciences, including cognitive psychology, and is also underexplored in anthropology. In this perspective article, findings from a recent wave of cross-cultural studies will be outlined with respect to three aspects of cognition: perception and categorization, number representation and counting, and explanatory frameworks and beliefs. Identifying similarities and differences between these domains allows for general conclusions regarding cognitive diversity and helps to highlight the importance of culturally shaped content for a comprehensive understanding of cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Bender
- Department of Psychosocial Science, Faculty of Psychology, University of Bergen Bergen, Norway
| | - Sieghard Beller
- Department of Psychosocial Science, Faculty of Psychology, University of Bergen Bergen, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Bender A, Beller S. The Power of 2: How an Apparently Irregular Numeration System Facilitates Mental Arithmetic. Cogn Sci 2016; 41:158-187. [DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12337] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2014] [Revised: 04/21/2015] [Accepted: 10/06/2015] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Bender
- Department of Psychosocial Science; University of Bergen
| | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Ross RM, Atkinson QD. Folktale transmission in the Arctic provides evidence for high bandwidth social learning among hunter–gatherer groups. EVOL HUM BEHAV 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
30
|
Overmann KA. Numerosity Structures the Expression of Quantity in Lexical Numbers and Grammatical Number. CURRENT ANTHROPOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.1086/683092] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
31
|
Bender A, Beller S, Nersessian NJ. Diversity as Asset. Top Cogn Sci 2015; 7:677-88. [DOI: 10.1111/tops.12161] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/09/2015] [Accepted: 07/06/2015] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Bender
- Department of Psychosocial Science; University of Bergen
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Longobardi G, Ghirotto S, Guardiano C, Tassi F, Benazzo A, Ceolin A, Barbujani G. Across language families: Genome diversity mirrors linguistic variation within Europe. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 2015; 157:630-40. [PMID: 26059462 PMCID: PMC5095809 DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.22758] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2015] [Revised: 04/02/2015] [Accepted: 04/14/2015] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
Abstract
Objectives: The notion that patterns of linguistic and biological variation may cast light on each other and on population histories dates back to Darwin's times; yet, turning this intuition into a proper research program has met with serious methodological difficulties, especially affecting language comparisons. This article takes advantage of two new tools of comparative linguistics: a refined list of Indo‐European cognate words, and a novel method of language comparison estimating linguistic diversity from a universal inventory of grammatical polymorphisms, and hence enabling comparison even across different families. We corroborated the method and used it to compare patterns of linguistic and genomic variation in Europe. Materials and Methods: Two sets of linguistic distances, lexical and syntactic, were inferred from these data and compared with measures of geographic and genomic distance through a series of matrix correlation tests. Linguistic and genomic trees were also estimated and compared. A method (Treemix) was used to infer migration episodes after the main population splits. Results: We observed significant correlations between genomic and linguistic diversity, the latter inferred from data on both Indo‐European and non‐Indo‐European languages. Contrary to previous observations, on the European scale, language proved a better predictor of genomic differences than geography. Inferred episodes of genetic admixture following the main population splits found convincing correlates also in the linguistic realm. Discussion: These results pave the ground for previously unfeasible cross‐disciplinary analyses at the worldwide scale, encompassing populations of distant language families. Am J Phys Anthropol 157:630–640, 2015. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Giuseppe Longobardi
- Department of Language and Linguistic Science, University of York, York, UK.,Department of Humanities, University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy
| | - Silvia Ghirotto
- Department of Life Sciences and Biotechnology, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| | - Cristina Guardiano
- Department of Communication and Economics, University of Modena-Reggio Emilia, Modena, Italy
| | - Francesca Tassi
- Department of Life Sciences and Biotechnology, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| | - Andrea Benazzo
- Department of Life Sciences and Biotechnology, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| | - Andrea Ceolin
- Department of Language and Linguistic Science, University of York, York, UK
| | - Guido Barbujani
- Department of Life Sciences and Biotechnology, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Abstract
The effect of population size on patterns and rates of language evolution is controversial. Do languages with larger speaker populations change faster due to a greater capacity for innovation, or do smaller populations change faster due to more efficient diffusion of innovations? Do smaller populations suffer greater loss of language elements through founder effects or drift, or do languages with more speakers lose features due to a process of simplification? Revealing the influence of population size on the tempo and mode of language evolution not only will clarify underlying mechanisms of language change but also has practical implications for the way that language data are used to reconstruct the history of human cultures. Here, we provide, to our knowledge, the first empirical, statistically robust test of the influence of population size on rates of language evolution, controlling for the evolutionary history of the populations and formally comparing the fit of different models of language evolution. We compare rates of gain and loss of cognate words for basic vocabulary in Polynesian languages, an ideal test case with a well-defined history. We demonstrate that larger populations have higher rates of gain of new words whereas smaller populations have higher rates of word loss. These results show that demographic factors can influence rates of language evolution and that rates of gain and loss are affected differently. These findings are strikingly consistent with general predictions of evolutionary models.
Collapse
|
34
|
Lee S, Hasegawa T. Oceanic barriers promote language diversification in the Japanese Islands. J Evol Biol 2014; 27:1905-12. [PMID: 24953224 DOI: 10.1111/jeb.12442] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2014] [Accepted: 05/30/2014] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Good barriers make good languages. Scholars have long speculated that geographical barriers impede linguistic contact between speech communities and promote language diversification in a manner similar to the process of allopatric speciation. This hypothesis, however, has seldom been tested systematically and quantitatively. Here, we adopt methods from evolutionary biology and attempt to quantify the influence of oceanic barriers on the degree of lexical diversity in the Japanese Islands. Measuring the degree of beta diversity from basic vocabularies, we find that geographical proximity and, more importantly, isolation by surrounding ocean, independently explains a significant proportion of lexical variation across Japonic languages. Further analyses indicate that our results are neither a by-product of using a distance matrix derived from a Bayesian language phylogeny nor an epiphenomenon of accelerated evolutionary rates in languages spoken by small communities. Moreover, we find that the effect of oceanic barriers is reproducible with the Ainu languages, indicating that our analytic approach as well as the results can be generalized beyond Japonic language family. The findings we report here are the first quantitative evidence that physical barriers formed by ocean can influence language diversification and points to an intriguing common mechanism between linguistic and biological evolution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Lee
- Department of Biological Sciences, Graduate School of Science, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
| | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Gong T, Shuai L, Zhang M. Modelling language evolution: Examples and predictions. Phys Life Rev 2014; 11:280-302. [DOI: 10.1016/j.plrev.2013.11.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2013] [Accepted: 11/13/2013] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
|
36
|
Sinnemäki K. Cognitive processing, language typology, and variation. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2014; 5:477-487. [PMID: 26308657 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1294] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2013] [Revised: 03/06/2014] [Accepted: 03/30/2014] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Abstract
UNLABELLED Linguistic typological preferences have often been linked to cognitive processing preferences but often without recourse to typologically relevant experiments on cognitive processing. This article reviews experimental work on the possible parallels between preferences in cognitive processing and language typology. I summarize the main theoretical accounts of the processing-typology connection and show that typological distributions arise diachronically from preferred paths of language change, which may be affected by the degree to which alternative structures are preferred (e.g., easier) in acquisition or usage. The surveyed experimental evidence shows that considerable support exists for many linguistic universals to reflect preferences in cognitive processing. Artificial language learning experiments emerge as a promising method for researching the processing-typology connection, as long as its limitations are taken into account. I further show that social and cultural differences in cognition may have an effect on typological distributions and that to account for this variation a multidisciplinary approach to the processing-typology connection has to be developed. Lastly, since the body of experimental research does not adequately represent the linguistic diversity of the world's languages, it remains as an urgent task for the field to better account for this diversity in future work. WIREs Cogn Sci 2014, 5:477-487. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1294 For further resources related to this article, please visit the WIREs website. CONFLICT OF INTEREST The author has declared no conflicts of interest for this article.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kaius Sinnemäki
- Helsinki Collegium for Advanced Studies, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Gong T, Shuai L, Zhang M. Key issues for the prosperity of modelling research of language evolution: Reply to comments on "Modelling language evolution: Examples and predictions". Phys Life Rev 2014; 11:324-8. [PMID: 24747021 DOI: 10.1016/j.plrev.2014.04.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2014] [Accepted: 04/01/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Tao Gong
- Department of Linguistics, University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.
| | - Lan Shuai
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Menghan Zhang
- Institute of Linguistics, Shanghai Normal University, Shanghai, China
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Hall ML, Ferreira VS, Mayberry RI. Investigating constituent order change with elicited pantomime: a functional account of SVO emergence. Cogn Sci 2014; 38:943-72. [PMID: 24641486 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12105] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/13/2012] [Revised: 05/13/2013] [Accepted: 06/18/2013] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
One of the most basic functions of human language is to convey who did what to whom. In the world's languages, the order of these three constituents (subject [S], verb [V], and object [O]) is uneven, with SOV and SVO being most common. Recent experiments using experimentally elicited pantomime provide a possible explanation of the prevalence of SOV, but extant explanations for the prevalence of SVO could benefit from further empirical support. Here, we test whether SVO might emerge because (a) SOV is not well suited for describing reversible events (a woman pushing a boy) and (b) pressures to be efficient and mention subjects before objects conspire to rule out many other alternatives. We tested this by asking participants to describe reversible and non-reversible events in pantomime, and we instructed some participants to be consistent in the form of their gestures and to teach them to the experimenter. These manipulations led to the emergence of SVO in speakers of both English (SVO) and Turkish (SOV).
Collapse
|
39
|
Linguistic phylogenies support back-migration from Beringia to Asia. PLoS One 2014; 9:e91722. [PMID: 24621925 PMCID: PMC3951421 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091722] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/14/2013] [Accepted: 02/15/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent arguments connecting Na-Dene languages of North America with Yeniseian languages of Siberia have been used to assert proof for the origin of Native Americans in central or western Asia. We apply phylogenetic methods to test support for this hypothesis against an alternative hypothesis that Yeniseian represents a back-migration to Asia from a Beringian ancestral population. We coded a linguistic dataset of typological features and used neighbor-joining network algorithms and Bayesian model comparison based on Bayes factors to test the fit between the data and the linguistic phylogenies modeling two dispersal hypotheses. Our results support that a Dene-Yeniseian connection more likely represents radiation out of Beringia with back-migration into central Asia than a migration from central or western Asia to North America.
Collapse
|
40
|
Affiliation(s)
- Tom Güldemann
- Humboldt University Berlin / Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Leipzig
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Brown S, Savage PE, Ko AMS, Stoneking M, Ko YC, Loo JH, Trejaut JA. Correlations in the population structure of music, genes and language. Proc Biol Sci 2013; 281:20132072. [PMID: 24225453 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2072] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/03/2023] Open
Abstract
We present, to our knowledge, the first quantitative evidence that music and genes may have coevolved by demonstrating significant correlations between traditional group-level folk songs and mitochondrial DNA variation among nine indigenous populations of Taiwan. These correlations were of comparable magnitude to those between language and genes for the same populations, although music and language were not significantly correlated with one another. An examination of population structure for genetics showed stronger parallels to music than to language. Overall, the results suggest that music might have a sufficient time-depth to retrace ancient population movements and, additionally, that it might be capturing different aspects of population history than language. Music may therefore have the potential to serve as a novel marker of human migrations to complement genes, language and other markers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven Brown
- Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Behaviour, McMaster University, , 1280 Main Street West, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada , L8S 4K1, Department of Musicology, Tokyo University of the Arts, , Tokyo, Japan, Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, , Leipzig, Germany, Graduate Institute of Clinical Medical Science, China Medical University, , Taichung, Taiwan, Republic of China, Mackay Memorial Hospital, , New Taipei City, Taiwan, Republic of China
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
42
|
de Boer M, Toni I, Willems RM. What drives successful verbal communication? Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:622. [PMID: 24101898 PMCID: PMC3787272 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00622] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2013] [Accepted: 09/09/2013] [Indexed: 01/10/2023] Open
Abstract
There is a vast amount of potential mappings between behaviors and intentions in communication: a behavior can indicate a multitude of different intentions, and the same intention can be communicated with a variety of behaviors. Humans routinely solve these many-to-many referential problems when producing utterances for an Addressee. This ability might rely on social cognitive skills, for instance, the ability to manipulate unobservable summary variables to disambiguate ambiguous behavior of other agents (“mentalizing”) and the drive to invest resources into changing and understanding the mental state of other agents (“communicative motivation”). Alternatively, the ambiguities of verbal communicative interactions might be solved by general-purpose cognitive abilities that process cues that are incidentally associated with the communicative interaction. In this study, we assess these possibilities by testing which cognitive traits account for communicative success during a verbal referential task. Cognitive traits were assessed with psychometric scores quantifying motivation, mentalizing abilities, and general-purpose cognitive abilities, taxing abstract visuo-spatial abilities. Communicative abilities of participants were assessed by using an on-line interactive task that required a speaker to verbally convey a concept to an Addressee. The communicative success of the utterances was quantified by measuring how frequently a number of Evaluators would infer the correct concept. Speakers with high motivational and general-purpose cognitive abilities generated utterances that were more easily interpreted. These findings extend to the domain of verbal communication the notion that motivational and cognitive factors influence the human ability to rapidly converge on shared communicative innovations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miriam de Boer
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
43
|
Roberts S, Winters J. Linguistic diversity and traffic accidents: lessons from statistical studies of cultural traits. PLoS One 2013; 8:e70902. [PMID: 23967132 PMCID: PMC3743834 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070902] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2013] [Accepted: 06/24/2013] [Indexed: 01/18/2023] Open
Abstract
The recent proliferation of digital databases of cultural and linguistic data, together with new statistical techniques becoming available has lead to a rise in so-called nomothetic studies [1]–[8]. These seek relationships between demographic variables and cultural traits from large, cross-cultural datasets. The insights from these studies are important for understanding how cultural traits evolve. While these studies are fascinating and are good at generating testable hypotheses, they may underestimate the probability of finding spurious correlations between cultural traits. Here we show that this kind of approach can find links between such unlikely cultural traits as traffic accidents, levels of extra-martial sex, political collectivism and linguistic diversity. This suggests that spurious correlations, due to historical descent, geographic diffusion or increased noise-to-signal ratios in large datasets, are much more likely than some studies admit. We suggest some criteria for the evaluation of nomothetic studies and some practical solutions to the problems. Since some of these studies are receiving media attention without a widespread understanding of the complexities of the issue, there is a risk that poorly controlled studies could affect policy. We hope to contribute towards a general skepticism for correlational studies by demonstrating the ease of finding apparently rigorous correlations between cultural traits. Despite this, we see well-controlled nomothetic studies as useful tools for the development of theories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Seán Roberts
- Seán Roberts Max Plank Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| | | |
Collapse
|
44
|
Abstract
It is generally difficult to define reasonable parameters and interpret their values in mathematical models of social phenomena. Rather than directly fitting abstract parameters against empirical data, we should define some concrete parameters to denote the sociocultural factors relevant for particular phenomena, and compute the values of these parameters based upon the corresponding empirical data. Taking the example of modeling studies of language competition, we propose a language diffusion principle and two language inheritance principles to compute two critical parameters, namely the impacts and inheritance rates of competing languages, in our language competition model derived from the Lotka-Volterra competition model in evolutionary biology. These principles assign explicit sociolinguistic meanings to those parameters and calculate their values from the relevant data of population censuses and language surveys. Using four examples of language competition, we illustrate that our language competition model with thus-estimated parameter values can reliably replicate and predict the dynamics of language competition, and it is especially useful in cases lacking direct competition data.
Collapse
|
45
|
Berwick RC, Friederici AD, Chomsky N, Bolhuis JJ. Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends Cogn Sci 2013; 17:89-98. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 287] [Impact Index Per Article: 23.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/19/2012] [Revised: 12/07/2012] [Accepted: 12/11/2012] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
46
|
|
47
|
Baronchelli A, Chater N, Pastor-Satorras R, Christiansen MH. The biological origin of linguistic diversity. PLoS One 2012; 7:e48029. [PMID: 23118922 PMCID: PMC3484145 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2012] [Accepted: 09/26/2012] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
In contrast with animal communication systems, diversity is characteristic of almost every aspect of human language. Languages variously employ tones, clicks, or manual signs to signal differences in meaning; some languages lack the noun-verb distinction (e.g., Straits Salish), whereas others have a proliferation of fine-grained syntactic categories (e.g., Tzeltal); and some languages do without morphology (e.g., Mandarin), while others pack a whole sentence into a single word (e.g., Cayuga). A challenge for evolutionary biology is to reconcile the diversity of languages with the high degree of biological uniformity of their speakers. Here, we model processes of language change and geographical dispersion and find a consistent pressure for flexible learning, irrespective of the language being spoken. This pressure arises because flexible learners can best cope with the observed high rates of linguistic change associated with divergent cultural evolution following human migration. Thus, rather than genetic adaptations for specific aspects of language, such as recursion, the coevolution of genes and fast-changing linguistic structure provides the biological basis for linguistic diversity. Only biological adaptations for flexible learning combined with cultural evolution can explain how each child has the potential to learn any human language.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Baronchelli
- Laboratory for the Modeling of Biological and Socio-technical Systems, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Nick Chater
- Behavioural Science Group, Warwick Business School, University of Warwick, Coventry, United Kingdom
| | - Romualdo Pastor-Satorras
- Departament de Física i Enginyeria Nuclear, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Morten H. Christiansen
- Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, United States of America
- Santa Fe Institute, Santa Fe, New Mexico, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen C. Levinson
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, PB 310 Nijmegen 6500AH, and Donders Institute, PB 9104 Nijmegen 6500 HE, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|