1
|
Killen M, Burkholder AR, Brey E, Cooper D, Pauker K. Children and adolescents rectify unequal allocations of leadership duties in the classroom. Child Dev 2024; 95:1950-1966. [PMID: 38922931 PMCID: PMC11579645 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14123] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/28/2024]
Abstract
Little is known about how children and adolescents evaluate unequal teacher allocations of leadership duties based on ethnicity-race and gender in the classroom. U.S. boys and girls, White (40.7%), Multiracial (18.5%), Black/African American (16.0%), Latine (14.2%), Asian (5.5%), Pacific Islander (0.4%), and other (4.7%) ethnic-racial backgrounds, 8-14 years, N = 275, evaluated teacher allocations of high-status leadership positions favoring specific ethnic-racial or gender groups during 2018-2021. Adolescents, more than children, negatively evaluated unequal teacher allocations of leadership duties that resulted in group-based inequalities, expected peers who shared the identity of a group disadvantaged by the teacher's allocation to view it more negatively than others, and rectified inequalities. Understanding perceptions of teacher-based bias provides an opportunity for interventions designed to create fair and just classrooms that motivate all students to achieve.
Collapse
|
2
|
Schwartz F, Chernyak N. The good, the rich, and the powerful: How young children compensate victims of moral transgressions depending on moral character, wealth, and social dominance. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 247:106045. [PMID: 39167858 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106045] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/22/2023] [Revised: 07/15/2024] [Accepted: 07/23/2024] [Indexed: 08/23/2024]
Abstract
Theories of justice suggest that it serves two main purposes: punishment and restoration. Although punishment emerges early and has been well-documented, little is known about the contexts in which young children engage in restorative practices like compensation for victims. The current study investigated whether children's engagement in compensation and punishment (which often involve a redistribution of resources) was sensitive to characteristics of the perpetrator and victim known to shape distributive justice decisions (decisions about how resources should be distributed), such as social dominance, resource inequality, and moral character. A total of 54 children aged 3 to 7 years completed a series of moral judgment experiments. Each experiment featured interactions between a perpetrator and a victim, ending with the perpetrator stealing the victim's toy. In Experiment 1 (N = 44), social dominance did not affect punishment or compensation overall, but older children compensated the dominant victim (but not the subordinate victim) less than younger children. In Experiment 2 (N = 42), children compensated the poor victim more than the rich victim, but they did not punish the rich perpetrator more than the poor perpetrator. In Experiment 3 (N = 45), children compensated the victim with a good moral character more than the victim with a bad moral character, and the victim's moral character did not influence punishment. Altogether, these findings offer new insights into how children resort to compensation for victims as a complement to, rather than an alternative to, punishment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Flora Schwartz
- Laboratoire Cognition, Langues, Langage, Ergonomie (CLLE), Université de Toulouse, CNRS, 31058 Toulouse Cedex 9, France.
| | - Nadia Chernyak
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92617, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Menendez D, Labotka D, Umscheid VA, Gelman SA. The social aspects of illness: Children's and parents' explanations of the relation between social categories and illness in a predominantly white U.S. sample. Child Dev 2024; 95:1676-1692. [PMID: 38730563 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14110] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/13/2024]
Abstract
The COVID-19 pandemic in the United States has had a disproportionate impact on Black, low-income, and elderly individuals. We recruited 175 predominantly white children ages 5-12 and their parents (N = 112) and asked which of two individuals (differing in age, gender, race, social class, or personality) was more likely to get sick with either COVID-19 or the common cold and why. Children and parents reported that older adults were more likely to get sick than younger adults, but reported few differences based on gender, race, social class, or personality. Children predominantly used behavioral explanations, but older children used more biological and structural explanations. Thus, children have some understanding of health disparities, and their understanding increases with age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Menendez
- University of California-Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, California, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Vasil N, Srinivasan M, Ellwood-Lowe ME, Delaney S, Gopnik A, Lombrozo T. Structural explanations lead young children and adults to rectify resource inequalities. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 242:105896. [PMID: 38520769 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105896] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2023] [Revised: 02/14/2024] [Accepted: 02/14/2024] [Indexed: 03/25/2024]
Abstract
Decisions about how to divide resources have profound social and practical consequences. Do explanations regarding the source of existing inequalities influence how children and adults allocate new resources? When 3- to 6-year-old children (N = 201) learned that inequalities were caused by structural forces (stable external constraints affecting access to resources) as opposed to internal forces (effort), they rectified inequalities, overriding previously documented tendencies to perpetuate inequality or divide resources equally. Adults (N = 201) were more likely than children to rectify inequality spontaneously; this was further strengthened by a structural explanation but reversed by an effort-based explanation. Allocation behaviors were mirrored in judgments of which allocation choices by others were appropriate. These findings reveal how explanations powerfully guide social reasoning and action from childhood through adulthood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ny Vasil
- California State University, East Bay, Hayward, CA 94542, USA.
| | | | | | - Sierra Delaney
- University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA
| | - Alison Gopnik
- University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Glidden J, Yee KM, Killen M. Morally-Relevant Theory of Mind is Related to Viewing Gender Inequalities as Unacceptable. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2024; 70:101450. [PMID: 39328307 PMCID: PMC11423782 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101450] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/28/2024]
Abstract
Previous research has shown that morally-relevant theory of mind enables children to avoid blaming a peer for an accidental transgression. The current study investigated whether this form of theory of mind helps children recognize that gender inequalities are unfair and create negative emotional experiences. Further, the study examined this ability across three perspectives (for themselves, for those who have been advantaged by inequality, and for those who have been disadvantaged by inequality). Participants were 141 children (M Age = 6.67 years, 49% female, 32% ethnic/racial minority) recruited from the mid-Atlantic region of the U.S. Experience with the negative consequences of gender bias and more advanced mental state understanding was associated with more negative evaluations of gender inequalities and more neutral attributions of others' emotions. These findings shed light on the role of different forms of mental state understanding in children's evaluations of inequalities based on gender.
Collapse
|
6
|
Kaufman EM, Glidden JT, Killen M. Teachers' Beliefs, Values, and Likelihood of Talking about Group-Based Discrimination. TEACHING AND TEACHER EDUCATION 2024; 139:104444. [PMID: 38800089 PMCID: PMC11127248 DOI: 10.1016/j.tate.2023.104444] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/29/2024]
Abstract
A teacher survey (N = 290) based on the social reasoning developmental model investigated (1) beliefs and values about using class time for discussing discrimination and reported frequency of discussing discrimination based on different social identities and (2) factors predicting teachers' frequency for discussing racial discrimination in the classroom. Most participants reported beliefs that all five identities were worth discussing in class, yet teachers most strongly believed that wealth, race, and native language affect students' education. Higher beliefs that prejudice can change, school support, and beliefs that race affects students' education predicted higher teacher reports for talking about racial discrimination.
Collapse
|
7
|
Altschul DM. Whither dominance? An enduring evolutionary legacy of primate sociality. PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE 2024; 7:e1. [PMID: 38384663 PMCID: PMC10877274 DOI: 10.1017/pen.2023.13] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2023] [Revised: 10/03/2023] [Accepted: 10/23/2023] [Indexed: 02/23/2024]
Abstract
This article discusses dominance personality dimensions found in primates, particularly in the great apes, and how they compare to dominance in humans. Dominance traits are seen in virtually all primate species, and these dimensions reflect how adept an individual is at ascending within a social hierarchy. Among great apes, dominance is one of the most prominent personality factors but, in humans, dominance is usually modeled as a facet of extraversion. Social, cultural, and cognitive differences between humans and our closest ape relatives are explored, alongside humanity's hierarchical and egalitarian heritage. The basic characteristics of dominance in humans and nonhuman great apes are then described, alongside the similarities and differences between great apes. African apes live in societies each with its own hierarchical organization. Humans were a possible exception for some of our history, but more recently, hierarchies have dominated. The general characteristics of high-dominance humans, particularly those living in industrialized nations, are described. Dominance itself can be subdivided into correlated subfactors: domineering, prestige, and leadership. Various explanations have been posed for why dominance has declined in prominence within human personality factor structures, and several possibilities are evaluated. The value of dominance in personality research is discussed: dominance has links to, for instance, age, sex, aggression, self-esteem, locus of control, stress, health, and multiple socioeconomic status indicators. The piece concludes with recommendations for researchers who wish to assess dominance in personality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Drew M. Altschul
- The University of Edinburgh, UK
- Scottish Primate Research Group, UK
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Amemiya J, Widjanarko K, Chung I, Bian L, Heyman GD. Children can represent complex social status hierarchies: Evidence from Indonesia. Child Dev 2023; 94:1730-1744. [PMID: 37357502 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13951] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/02/2022] [Revised: 04/17/2023] [Accepted: 04/28/2023] [Indexed: 06/27/2023]
Abstract
Children's ethnicity-status associations are often studied in societies where one ethnic group possesses status across multiple dimensions, such as political influence and wealth. This study examined children's (6-12 years) and adults' representations of more complex hierarchies in Indonesia (N = 341; 38% Native Indonesian, 33% Chinese Indonesian, and 27% other ethnicities; 55% female, 36% male; 2021-2022), a society where ethnic groups hold distinct forms of status (on average, Native Indonesians have political influence; Chinese Indonesians have wealth). By 6.5 years, children associated Native Indonesians with political influence and Chinese Indonesians with wealth. Intersectional analyses indicated that ethnicity-status associations were stronger for male than female targets. Children of all ethnicities preferred Chinese Indonesians and preferences were predicted by wealth judgments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jamie Amemiya
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, USA
| | - Kiara Widjanarko
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, USA
| | - Irene Chung
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, USA
| | - Lin Bian
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, USA
| | - Gail D Heyman
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Fisher-Grafy H, Meyer S. Exploring Intergroup Peer Exclusion: Validation of the Latency Social-Psychological Developmental Questionnaire (LSPD). CHILDREN 2023; 10:children10030543. [PMID: 36980101 PMCID: PMC10046949 DOI: 10.3390/children10030543] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2023] [Revised: 03/01/2023] [Accepted: 03/11/2023] [Indexed: 03/14/2023]
Abstract
Intergroup peer exclusion has been studied mainly from a pathological aspect. Currently, methods of diagnosis and treatment focus on this pathological point of view. Qualitative research has revealed that social intergroup peer exclusion has a role in the developmental task of the latency stage. The study’s main aim was to develop and validate a quick and easy quantitative questionnaire for use in a school setting that reflects the social developmental variables exposed in previous qualitative research. The 32-item Latency Social-Psychological Developmental questionnaire (LSPD) and the Loneliness and Social Dissatisfaction Questionnaire were administered to 20 Grade 4 and Grade 5 classes at four co-ed public elementary schools (N = 373 participants). Factor analysis revealed six developmental factors, and correlations were found between these factors and loneliness. The LSPD is a tool for assessing latency stage development among children who experienced exclusion as well as the developmental status of the entire class. The LSPD can assist in identifying specific development areas to focus on in treatment and intervention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hanna Fisher-Grafy
- Department of Education, Talpiot College of Education, Holon 58500, Israel
- Correspondence: ; Tel.: +972-54-429-1460
| | - Sonya Meyer
- Department of Occupational Therapy, Ariel University, Ariel 40700, Israel;
| |
Collapse
|