1
|
Holzmeister F, Johannesson M, Camerer CF, Chen Y, Ho TH, Hoogeveen S, Huber J, Imai N, Imai T, Jin L, Kirchler M, Ly A, Mandl B, Manfredi D, Nave G, Nosek BA, Pfeiffer T, Sarafoglou A, Schwaiger R, Wagenmakers EJ, Waldén V, Dreber A. Examining the replicability of online experiments selected by a decision market. Nat Hum Behav 2025; 9:316-330. [PMID: 39562799 PMCID: PMC11860227 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-024-02062-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2023] [Accepted: 10/11/2024] [Indexed: 11/21/2024]
Abstract
Here we test the feasibility of using decision markets to select studies for replication and provide evidence about the replicability of online experiments. Social scientists (n = 162) traded on the outcome of close replications of 41 systematically selected MTurk social science experiments published in PNAS 2015-2018, knowing that the 12 studies with the lowest and the 12 with the highest final market prices would be selected for replication, along with 2 randomly selected studies. The replication rate, based on the statistical significance indicator, was 83% for the top-12 and 33% for the bottom-12 group. Overall, 54% of the studies were successfully replicated, with replication effect size estimates averaging 45% of the original effect size estimates. The replication rate varied between 54% and 62% for alternative replication indicators. The observed replicability of MTurk experiments is comparable to that of previous systematic replication projects involving laboratory experiments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Felix Holzmeister
- Department of Economics, University of Innsbruck, Innsbruck, Austria
| | - Magnus Johannesson
- Department of Economics, Stockholm School of Economics, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Colin F Camerer
- Division of the Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA, USA
| | - Yiling Chen
- John A. Paulson School of Engineering and Applied Sciences, Harvard University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Teck-Hua Ho
- Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
| | - Suzanne Hoogeveen
- Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Juergen Huber
- Department of Banking and Finance, University of Innsbruck, Innsbruck, Austria
| | - Noriko Imai
- Institute of Social and Economic Research, Osaka University, Osaka, Japan
| | - Taisuke Imai
- Institute of Social and Economic Research, Osaka University, Osaka, Japan
| | - Lawrence Jin
- Lee Kuan Yew School of Public Policy, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
| | - Michael Kirchler
- Department of Banking and Finance, University of Innsbruck, Innsbruck, Austria
| | - Alexander Ly
- Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Machine Learning, Centrum Wiskunde and Informatica, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | | | - Dylan Manfredi
- Marketing Department, Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Gideon Nave
- Marketing Department, Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Brian A Nosek
- Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA, USA
- Center for Open Science, Charlottesville, VA, USA
| | - Thomas Pfeiffer
- Institute for Advanced Study, Massey University, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Alexandra Sarafoglou
- Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Rene Schwaiger
- Department of Banking and Finance, University of Innsbruck, Innsbruck, Austria
| | - Eric-Jan Wagenmakers
- Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | | | - Anna Dreber
- Department of Economics, University of Innsbruck, Innsbruck, Austria.
- Department of Economics, Stockholm School of Economics, Stockholm, Sweden.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Genschow O. It Is Belief in Dualism, and Not Free Will, That Best Predicts Helping: A Conceptual Replication and Extension of Baumeister et al. (2009). PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2024; 50:645-656. [PMID: 36631739 PMCID: PMC10903128 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221137209] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2021] [Accepted: 10/16/2022] [Indexed: 01/13/2023]
Abstract
Previous research found that experimentally reducing people's belief in free will affects social behaviors. However, more recent investigations could not replicate several findings in this literature. An explanation for the mixed findings is that free will beliefs are related to social behaviors on a correlational level, but experimental manipulations are not able to detect this relation. To test this interpretation, we conceptually replicated and extended a landmark study in the free will belief literature originally conducted by Baumeister et al. In five studies (total N = 1,467), we investigated whether belief in free will predicts helping behavior in comparison to other beliefs related to free will. Overall, our results support the original findings, as belief in free will correlated with helping behavior. However, the results also show that the best predictor of helping behavior is not belief in free will but belief in dualism. Theoretical implications are discussed.
Collapse
|
3
|
Cracco E, Liepelt R, Brass M, Genschow O. Top-Down Modulation of Motor Priming by Belief About Animacy. Exp Psychol 2023; 70:355-365. [PMID: 38602116 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000605] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/12/2024]
Abstract
Research has shown that people automatically imitate others and that this tendency is stronger when the other person is a human compared with a nonhuman agent. However, a controversial question is whether automatic imitation is also modulated by whether people believe the other person is a human. Although early research supported this hypothesis, not all studies reached the same conclusion and a recent meta-analysis found that there is currently neither evidence in favor nor against an influence of animacy beliefs on automatic imitation. One of the most prominent studies supporting such an influence is the study by Liepelt and Brass (2010), who found that automatic imitation was stronger when participants believed an ambiguous, gloved hand to be human, as opposed to wooden. In this registered report, we provide a high-powered replication of this study (N = 199). In contrast to Liepelt and Brass (2010), we did not find an effect of animacy beliefs on automatic imitation. However, we did find a correlation between automatic imitation and perceived self-other similarity. Together, these results suggest that the gloved hand procedure does not reliably influence automatic imitation, but interindividual differences in perceived similarity do.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emiel Cracco
- Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Belgium
| | - Roman Liepelt
- Department of General Psychology: Judgment, Decision Making, Action, Faculty of Psychology, FernUniversität in Hagen, Germany
| | - Marcel Brass
- Department of Psychology, Humboldt University of Berlin, Germany
| | - Oliver Genschow
- Department of Cognitive, Social- and Economic Psychology, Institute for Management and Organization, Leuphana University, Lüneburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
St Quinton T, Trafimow D, Genschow O. The role of free will beliefs in social behavior: Priority areas for future research. Conscious Cogn 2023; 115:103586. [PMID: 37837797 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2023.103586] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2023] [Revised: 09/14/2023] [Accepted: 10/07/2023] [Indexed: 10/16/2023]
Abstract
Recent research has examined the consequences that holding views about free will has on social behavior. Specifically, through manipulating people's belief in free will, researchers have tested the psychological and behavioral consequences of free will belief change. However, findings of such manipulations have been shown to be relatively small and inconsistent. The purpose of this paper is to outline four key areas for researchers in this area to consider. We believe considering these areas will give a more nuanced understanding of the role of free will beliefs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tom St Quinton
- School of Psychology and Therapeutic Studies, Leeds Trinity University, Leeds, UK.
| | - David Trafimow
- Department of Psychology, New Mexico State University, NM, USA.
| | - Oliver Genschow
- Institute for Management and Organization, Leuphana University Lüneburg, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Peng W, Cracco E, Troje NF, Brass M. Does belief in free will influence biological motion perception? PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2023; 87:751-767. [PMID: 35831473 PMCID: PMC9281248 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-022-01704-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/14/2021] [Accepted: 06/22/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Previous research suggests that belief in free will correlates with intentionality attribution. However, whether belief in free will is also related to more basic social processes is unknown. Based on evidence that biological motion contains intentionality cues that observers spontaneously extract, we investigate whether people who believe more in free will, or in related constructs, such as dualism and determinism, would be better at picking up such cues and therefore at detecting biological agents hidden in noise, or would be more inclined to detect intentionality cues and therefore to detect biological agents even when there are none. Signal detection theory was used to measure participants' ability to detect biological motion from scrambled background noise (d') and their response bias (c) in doing so. In two experiments, we found that belief in determinism and belief in dualism, but not belief in free will, were associated with biological motion perception. However, no causal effect was found when experimentally manipulating free will-related beliefs. In sum, our results show that biological motion perception, a low-level social process, is related to high-level beliefs about dualism and determinism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wei Peng
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium.
| | - Emiel Cracco
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium.,Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Gent, Belgium
| | - Nikolaus F Troje
- Department of Biology and Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Marcel Brass
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium.,Department of Psychology, Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
St Quinton T, Trafimow D. Implications of the TASI taxonomy for understanding inconsistent effects pertaining to free will beliefs. PHILOSOPHICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2023. [DOI: 10.1080/09515089.2023.2184335] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/06/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Tom St Quinton
- School of Psychology and Therapeutic Studies, Leeds Trinity University, Leeds, UK
| | - David Trafimow
- Department of Psychology, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, New Mexico, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Genschow O, Cracco E, Schneider J, Protzko J, Wisniewski D, Brass M, Schooler JW. Manipulating Belief in Free Will and Its Downstream Consequences: A Meta-Analysis. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 2023; 27:52-82. [PMID: 35676864 DOI: 10.1177/10888683221087527] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/30/2023]
Abstract
Ever since some scientists and popular media put forward the idea that free will is an illusion, the question has risen what would happen if people stopped believing in free will. Psychological research has investigated this question by testing the consequences of experimentally weakening people's free will beliefs. The results of these investigations have been mixed, with successful experiments and unsuccessful replications. This raises two fundamental questions: Can free will beliefs be manipulated, and do such manipulations have downstream consequences? In a meta-analysis including 145 experiments (95 unpublished), we show that exposing individuals to anti-free will manipulations decreases belief in free will and increases belief in determinism. However, we could not find evidence for downstream consequences. Our findings have important theoretical implications for research on free will beliefs and contribute to the discussion of whether reducing people's belief in free will has societal consequences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - John Protzko
- Central Connecticut State University, New Britain, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Katzir M, Genschow O. Automatic or controlled: How does disbelief in free will influence cognitive functioning? Br J Psychol 2022; 113:1121-1142. [PMID: 35706418 PMCID: PMC9796308 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12578] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2020] [Revised: 05/16/2022] [Accepted: 05/23/2022] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Most people believe in free will. Past research has indicated that reducing this belief has numerous downstream consequences including everyday outcomes as well as neural and cognitive correlates associated with a reduction of self-control. However, the exact mechanisms through which a reduction in free will belief affects self-control are still a matter of investigation. In the present registered report, we used a task switching paradigm to examine whether reducing belief in free will makes people less controlled or whether it enhances their reliance on automatic impulses. Using Bayesian sequential analysis, we failed to conceptually replicate the previous link between free will belief and cognitive control. Our registered report plan mostly accumulated substantial evidence supporting the null hypothesis. That is, diminished belief in free will does neither impact control nor automaticity. Theoretical implications of this finding are discussed.
Collapse
|
9
|
Stinnett AJ, Rodriguez JE, Littlefield AK, Alquist JL. Distinguishing free will from moral responsibility when measuring free will beliefs: The FWS-II. PHILOSOPHICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/09515089.2022.2139232] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/31/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Alec J. Stinnett
- Department of Psychology, Texas Tech University, 2500 Broadway, Lubbock, TX 79409 USA
| | - Jordan E. Rodriguez
- Department of Psychology, Texas Tech University, 2500 Broadway, Lubbock, TX 79409 USA
| | - Andrew K. Littlefield
- Department of Psychology, Texas Tech University, 2500 Broadway, Lubbock, TX 79409 USA
| | - Jessica L. Alquist
- Department of Psychology, Texas Tech University, 2500 Broadway, Lubbock, TX 79409 USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Chen O, Guan F, Hu Y, Wu T, Liu L, Sheng J, Chen J. The relationship between belief and prosocial behavior. CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1007/s12144-020-00943-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
11
|
Genschow O, Lange J. Belief in Free Will Is Related to Internal Attribution in Self-Perception. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGICAL AND PERSONALITY SCIENCE 2022. [DOI: 10.1177/19485506211057711] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Past research indicates that individuals’ belief in free will is related to attributing others’ behavior to internal causes. An open question is whether belief in free will is related to the attribution of one’s own action. To answer this question, we tested two opposing predictions against each other by assessing the relation of belief in free will with the self-serving bias—individuals’ tendency to attribute personal success more strongly to internal forces and failure to external forces. The resource hypothesis predicts that a higher endorsement in free will belief relates to a lower self-serving bias. The intention attribution hypothesis predicts that belief in free will relates to higher internal attributions, as compared with external attributions, irrespective of success and failure. Meta-analytic evidence across five high-powered studies (total N = 1,137) supports the intention attribution hypothesis, but not the resource hypothesis (materials and data are available on the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/2a89c/ ).
Collapse
|
12
|
Ozkok H, Tatar BH, Ayyıldız TN, Eskin M, Erdem G, Turan B. To Blame or Not to Blame: the Role of Belief in Free Will in Substance Use and Suicide Stigma. Int J Ment Health Addict 2022. [DOI: 10.1007/s11469-022-00755-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022] Open
|
13
|
Agency and self-other asymmetries in perceived bias and shortcomings: Replications of the Bias Blind Spot and link to free will beliefs. JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING 2021. [DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500008470] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
Abstract
AbstractBias Blind Spot (BBS) is the phenomenon that people tend to perceive themselves as less susceptible to biases than others. In three pre-registered experiments (overall N = 969), we replicated two experiments of the first demonstration of the phenomenon by Pronin et al. (2002). We found support of the BBS hypotheses, with effects in line with findings in the original study: Participants rated themselves as less susceptible to biases than others (d = –1.00 [–1.33, –0.67]). Deviating from the original, we found an unexpected effect that participants rated themselves as having fewer shortcomings (d = –0.34 [–0.46, –0.23]), though there was support for the target’s main premise that BBS was stronger for biases than for shortcomings (d = –0.43 [–0.56, –0.29]). Extending the replications, we found that beliefs in own free will were positively associated with BBS (r ∼ 0.17–0.22) and that beliefs in both own and general free will were positively associated with self-other asymmetry related to personal shortcomings (r ∼ 0.16–0.24). Materials, datasets, and code are available on https://osf.io/3df5s/.
Collapse
|
14
|
Isham EA, Lomayesva S. Involuntary classroom transition moderates the effect of Present Hedonistic perspective on the belief in free will. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2021; 186:111321. [PMID: 34658472 PMCID: PMC8506235 DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111321] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2021] [Revised: 08/19/2021] [Accepted: 09/29/2021] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Mitigation plans during the early stages of COVID-19 provided a unique, antagonistic environment in which drastic changes occurred quickly and did so with minimal freedom of choice (e.g., involuntary transition from in-person to online classroom). As such, individuals of different beliefs and perspectives would respond differently to these mitigations. We examined the interaction between the Present-Hedonistic (PH) perspective and involuntary classroom transition on the belief in free will (N = 131). PH-oriented individuals exhibit a strong desire for choice while also welcome new opportunities and change. Importantly, the perceived freedom of choice and capacity for change also serve as foundational constructs to the belief in free will. Our results revealed that involuntary transition weakened the free will belief in those with lower PH but did not affect those of higher PH orientation. These findings suggest that the interplay between the perception of choice and capacity for change account for how individuals responded to the COVID-19 pandemic mitigation plans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eve A Isham
- Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, 1503 E. University Blvd. Building 68, Tucson, AZ 85721, United States of America
| | - Sara Lomayesva
- New College of Interdisciplinary Arts and Sciences, Arizona State University, P.O. Box 37100, Phoenix, AZ 85069-7100, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Goto T. Comparing the Psychometric Properties of Two Japanese-Translated Scales of the Free Will and Determinism-Plus Scale. Front Psychol 2021; 12:720601. [PMID: 34675838 PMCID: PMC8523834 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.720601] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/04/2021] [Accepted: 09/03/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The free will and determinism-plus scale (FAD-Plus) is one of the most widely used scales to assess the lay belief of people in the existence of free will and deterministic world views. Past research has translated FAD-Plus into various languages for non-English speaking populations, and there exist two Japanese translations of FAD-Plus: the FAD+ and the FAD-J. This study aimed to compare the psychometric properties of FAD+ and FAD-J. Results revealed that while both FAD+ and FAD-J consist of the same four subscales as the original FAD-Plus, some differences exist in the item-level psychometric characteristics. In general, as for the construct validity, although results supported that both scales can assess almost the same construct in terms of the functionalities, FAD-J tends to be slightly more likely to replicate the correlations obtained in the previous research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Takayuki Goto
- School of Human Cultures, The University of Shiga Prefecture, Hikone, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Westfal M, Crusius J, Genschow O. Imitation and interindividual differences: Belief in free will is not related to automatic imitation. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2021; 219:103374. [PMID: 34329885 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103374] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2020] [Revised: 06/01/2021] [Accepted: 07/14/2021] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
It is well known that individuals have the tendency to automatically imitate each other and that such imitative behavior is fostered by perceiving intentions in others' actions. That is, past research has shown that perceiving an action as internally driven enhances the shared representation of observed and executed actions increasing automatic imitation. An interpersonal factor that increases the perception that a behavior is internally driven is belief in free will. Consequently, we hypothesized that the more individuals believe in free will, the more they automatically imitate others. To test this prediction, we conducted two high-powered (total N = 642) and preregistered studies in which we assessed automatic imitation with the imitation-inhibition task. Contrary to our predictions, belief in free will did not correlate with automatic imitation. This finding contributes to current findings challenging the assumption that automatic imitation is modulated by interindividual differences. Further theoretical implications are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mareike Westfal
- University of Cologne, Social Cognition Center Cologne, Richard-Strauss-Str. 2, 50931 Köln, Germany.
| | - Jan Crusius
- University of Cologne, Social Cognition Center Cologne, Richard-Strauss-Str. 2, 50931 Köln, Germany.
| | - Oliver Genschow
- University of Cologne, Social Cognition Center Cologne, Richard-Strauss-Str. 2, 50931 Köln, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Taylor M, Maranges HM, Chen SK, Vonasch AJ. Direct and indirect freedom in addiction: Folk free will and blame judgments are sensitive to the choice history of drug users. Conscious Cogn 2021; 94:103170. [PMID: 34320424 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2021.103170] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2021] [Revised: 06/10/2021] [Accepted: 07/01/2021] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
People view addiction as a source of diminished free will and moral responsibility. Yet, people are also sensitive to the personal histories of moral actors, including, perhaps, the way by which people became addicted. Across two studies (N = 806), we compare people's moral intuitions about cases in which the actor becomes addicted by force or by choice. We find that perceptions of reduced free will partially mediate an association between choice (vs. no choice) in addiction and moral blame for a bad act (Study 1). We replicate this pattern and show that blame judgments are stronger when the bad act is related (vs. unrelated) to obtaining the addictive substance (Study 2). Our work is novel in demonstrating that lay people evince relatively nuanced intuitions about the role of free will in addiction and morality-they track direct and indirect paths to choices when making free will and blame judgments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew Taylor
- Department of Philosophy, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States.
| | - Heather M Maranges
- Department of Psychology and Social Justice Centre, Concordia University, Canada
| | - Susan K Chen
- Department of Psychology, York University, Canada
| | - Andrew J Vonasch
- School of Psychology, Speech, and Hearing, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Zhang X, Zhao X. Relational mobility promotes subjective well‐being through control over interpersonal relationships among the Chinese. ASIAN JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12426] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Xian Zhao
- Rotman School of Management University of Toronto Toronto ON Canada
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Free to blame? Belief in free will is related to victim blaming. Conscious Cogn 2021; 88:103074. [PMID: 33445078 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2020.103074] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2020] [Revised: 12/18/2020] [Accepted: 12/22/2020] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
The more people believe in free will, the harsher their punishment of criminal offenders. A reason for this finding is that belief in free will leads individuals to perceive others as responsible for their behavior. While research supporting this notion has mainly focused on criminal offenders, the perspective of the victims has been neglected so far. We filled this gap and hypothesized that individuals' belief in free will is positively correlated with victim blaming-the tendency to make victims responsible for their bad luck. In three studies, we found that the more individuals believe in free will, the more they blame victims. Study 3 revealed that belief in free will is correlated with victim blaming even when controlling for just world beliefs, religious worldviews, and political ideology. The results contribute to a more differentiated view of the role of free will beliefs and attributed intentions.
Collapse
|
20
|
Strohmaier N, Pluut H, den Bos K, Adriaanse J, Vriesendorp R. Hindsight bias and outcome bias in judging directors’ liability and the role of free will beliefs. JOURNAL OF APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2020. [DOI: 10.1111/jasp.12722] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Niek Strohmaier
- Department of Business Studies Leiden University Leiden the Netherlands
| | - Helen Pluut
- Department of Business Studies Leiden University Leiden the Netherlands
| | - Kees den Bos
- Department of Psychology and School of Law Utrecht University Utrecht the Netherlands
| | - Jan Adriaanse
- Department of Business Studies Leiden University Leiden the Netherlands
| | - Reinout Vriesendorp
- Department of Company Law and Department of Business Studies Leiden University Leiden the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Eben C, Chen Z, Cracco E, Brass M, Billieux J, Verbruggen F. Are post-error adjustments influenced by beliefs in free will? A failure to replicate Rigoni, Wilquin, Brass and Burle, 2013. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2020; 7:200664. [PMID: 33391784 PMCID: PMC7735345 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.200664] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2020] [Accepted: 10/16/2020] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
In this pre-registered study, we tried to replicate the study by Rigoni et al. 2013 Cognition 127, 264-269. In the original study, the authors manipulated the participants' belief in free will in a between-subject design and subsequently measured post-error slowing (i.e. slower responses after an incorrect trial compared with a correct trial) as a marker of cognitive control. They found less post-error slowing in the group with reduced belief in free will (anti-free will group) compared with a control group in which belief in free will was not manipulated. In the present study, we used the same task procedure and the same free will manipulation (Crick text) in an attempt to replicate these findings. However, we used an online procedure and a larger sample size in order to address concerns about statistical power. Similar to the original study, we also used a questionnaire to measure beliefs in free will as an independent manipulation check. We found a difference in the scores on the questionnaire, thus a reduced belief in free will, after reading the Crick text. However, we did not find any difference in post-error slowing between the anti-free will and control groups. Our findings are in line with several other recent findings suggesting that the Crick text manipulation affects the participants' self-reported belief in free will but not their behaviour. The present study can be considered a high-powered failed replication attempt.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Charlotte Eben
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Zhang Chen
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Emiel Cracco
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Marcel Brass
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
- School of Mind and Brain/Department of Psychology, Humboldt Universität zu Berlin, Germany
| | - Joël Billieux
- Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
| | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Fillon A, Kutscher L, Feldman G. Impact of past behaviour normality: meta-analysis of exceptionality effect. Cogn Emot 2020; 35:129-149. [PMID: 32924775 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2020.1816910] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Exceptionality effect is the phenomenon that people associate stronger negative affect with a negative outcome when it is a result of an exception (abnormal behaviour) compared to when it is a result of routine (normal behaviour). In this pre-registered meta-analysis, we examined exceptionality effect in 48 studies (N = 4212). An analysis of 35 experimental studies (n = 3332) showed medium to strong effect (g = 0.60, 95% confidence intervals (CI) [0.41, 0.79]) for past behaviour across several measures (regret/affect: g = 0.66, counterfactual thought: g = 0.39, self-blame: g = 0.44, victim compensation: g = 0.39, offender punishment: g = 0.51). An analysis of 13 one-sample studies presenting a comparison of exceptional and routine behaviours simultaneously (n = 1217) revealed a very strong exceptionality effect (converted g = 1.98, CI [1.57, 2.38]). We tested several theoretical moderators: norm strength, event controllability, outcome rarity, action versus inaction, and status quo. We found that exceptionality effect was stronger when the routine was aligned with the status quo option and with action rather than for inaction. All materials are available on: https://osf.io/542c7/.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adrien Fillon
- Department of Social Psychology, Aix-Marseille University, Aix-en-Provence, France
| | - Lucas Kutscher
- Department of Work and Social Psychology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Gilad Feldman
- Department of Work and Social Psychology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands.,Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Reynolds JP, Vasiljevic M, Pilling M, Hall MG, Ribisl KM, Marteau TM. Communicating Evidence about the Causes of Obesity and Support for Obesity Policies: Two Population-Based Survey Experiments. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL RESEARCH AND PUBLIC HEALTH 2020; 17:ijerph17186539. [PMID: 32911776 PMCID: PMC7559841 DOI: 10.3390/ijerph17186539] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2020] [Revised: 09/01/2020] [Accepted: 09/03/2020] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
Public support for numerous obesity policies is low, which is one barrier to their implementation. One reason for this low support is the tendency to ascribe obesity to failings of willpower as opposed to the environment. Correlational evidence supports this position. However, the experimental evidence is mixed. In two experimental studies, participants were randomised to receive no message, messages about the environment's influence on obesity (Study 1 & 2), or messages about the environment's influence on human behaviour (Study 1). We investigated whether communicating these messages changed support for obesity policies and beliefs about the causes of obesity. Participants were recruited from nationally representative samples in Great Britain (Study 1 & 2) and the USA (Study 2) (total n = 4391). Study 2 was designed to replicate existing research. Neither study found evidence that communicating the messages increased support for obesity policies or strengthened beliefs about the environment's role in obesity. Study 2, therefore, did not replicate two earlier experimental studies. Instead, the studies reported here suggest that people's beliefs about the causes of obesity are resistant to change in response to evidence and are, therefore, not a promising avenue to increase support for obesity policies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James P. Reynolds
- Behaviour and Health Research Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 0SR, UK; (M.V.); (M.P.)
- Correspondence: (J.P.R.); (T.M.M.)
| | - Milica Vasiljevic
- Behaviour and Health Research Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 0SR, UK; (M.V.); (M.P.)
- Department of Psychology, Durham University, Durham DH1 3LE, UK
| | - Mark Pilling
- Behaviour and Health Research Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 0SR, UK; (M.V.); (M.P.)
| | - Marissa G. Hall
- Lineberger Comprehensive Cancer Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27514, USA; (M.G.H.); (K.M.R.)
- Department of Health Behaviour, UNC Gillings School of Global Public Health, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 275599, USA
| | - Kurt M. Ribisl
- Lineberger Comprehensive Cancer Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27514, USA; (M.G.H.); (K.M.R.)
- Department of Health Behaviour, UNC Gillings School of Global Public Health, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 275599, USA
| | - Theresa M. Marteau
- Behaviour and Health Research Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 0SR, UK; (M.V.); (M.P.)
- Correspondence: (J.P.R.); (T.M.M.)
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Schneider IK, Novin S, van Harreveld F, Genschow O. Benefits of being ambivalent: The relationship between trait ambivalence and attribution biases. BRITISH JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2020; 60:570-586. [PMID: 32893893 DOI: 10.1111/bjso.12417] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2020] [Revised: 08/07/2020] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Abstract
Ambivalence refers to the experience of having both positive and negative thoughts and feelings at the same time about the same object, person, or issue. Although ambivalence research has focused extensively on negative consequences, recently, scholars turned their lens to the positive effects of ambivalence, demonstrating beneficial effects on judgements and decision-making processes. So far, this work has focused on state ambivalence, which is ambivalence as a direct response to a specific stimulus. However, there are substantial individual differences in ambivalence: Some people are just more ambivalent than others. Taking a first step in understanding how these individual differences relate to judgement and decision-making, we examine the relationship between trait ambivalence and cognitive bias in social judgements tasks. Specifically, we look at two of the most pervasive and consequential attribution biases in person perception: correspondence bias and self-serving bias. We find a negative relationship between trait ambivalence and correspondence bias. The higher individuals are in trait ambivalence, the smaller their bias towards attributing behaviour to a person's disposition (Study 1A and B). We find the same for self-serving bias (Study 2A and B). In sum, we show that trait ambivalence is negatively related to cognitive bias in person perception.
Collapse
|
25
|
Nadelhoffer T, Shepard J, Crone DL, Everett JAC, Earp BD, Levy N. Does encouraging a belief in determinism increase cheating? Reconsidering the value of believing in free will. Cognition 2020; 203:104342. [PMID: 32593841 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104342] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/03/2019] [Revised: 05/21/2020] [Accepted: 05/26/2020] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
A key source of support for the view that challenging people's beliefs about free will may undermine moral behavior is two classic studies by Vohs and Schooler (2008). These authors reported that exposure to certain prompts suggesting that free will is an illusion increased cheating behavior. In the present paper, we report several attempts to replicate this influential and widely cited work. Over a series of five studies (sample sizes of N = 162, N = 283, N = 268, N = 804, N = 982) (four preregistered) we tested the relationship between (1) anti-free-will prompts and free will beliefs and (2) free will beliefs and immoral behavior. Our primary task was to closely replicate the findings from Vohs and Schooler (2008) using the same or highly similar manipulations and measurements as the ones used in their original studies. Our efforts were largely unsuccessful. We suggest that manipulating free will beliefs in a robust way is more difficult than has been implied by prior work, and that the proposed link with immoral behavior may not be as consistent as previous work suggests.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Nadelhoffer
- Duke University, Kenan Institute for Ethics, Campus Box 90432, Durham, NC 27708, USA.
| | - Jason Shepard
- Life University, 1269 Barclay Circle, Marietta, GA 30060, USA
| | - Damien L Crone
- University of Melbourne, Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, 12th floor Redmond Barry Building, Parkville Campus, Melbourne, VIC 3010, Australia
| | - Jim A C Everett
- University of Kent, School of Psychology, Keynes College, Canterbury CT2 7NP, United Kingdom
| | - Brian D Earp
- Oxford University, Uehiro Centre for Practical Ethics, 16-17 St Ebbes St, Oxford OX1 1PT, United Kingdom
| | - Neil Levy
- Macquarie University, Department of Philosophy, Level 2 North, Australian Hearing Hub, NSW 2109, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Genschow O, Hawickhorst H, Rigoni D, Aschermann E, Brass M. Professional Judges’ Disbelief in Free Will Does Not Decrease Punishment. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGICAL AND PERSONALITY SCIENCE 2020. [DOI: 10.1177/1948550620915055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
There is a debate in psychology and philosophy on the societal consequences of casting doubts about individuals’ belief in free will. Research suggests that experimentally reducing free will beliefs might affect how individuals evaluate others’ behavior. Past research has demonstrated that reduced free will beliefs decrease laypersons’ tendency toward retributive punishment. This finding has been used as an argument for the idea that promoting anti-free will viewpoints in the public media might have severe consequences for the legal system because it may move judges toward softer retributive punishments. However, actual implications for the legal system can only be drawn by investigating professional judges. In the present research, we investigated whether judges ( N = 87) are affected by reading anti-free will messages. The results demonstrate that although reading anti-free will texts reduces judges’ belief in free will, their recommended sentences are not influenced by their (manipulated) belief in free will.
Collapse
|
27
|
Mercier B, Wiwad D, Piff PK, Aknin LB, Robinson AR, Shariff A. Does Belief in Free Will Increase Support for Economic Inequality? COLLABRA-PSYCHOLOGY 2020. [DOI: 10.1525/collabra.303] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Does belief in free will increase support for economic inequality? Five studies using diverse measures and methods tested this question. Study 1 finds belief in free will is associated with increased support for inequality. Study 2 manipulates belief in free will and does not find evidence that this changes support for inequality. Studies 3 and 4 find that people are more willing to support inequality in a hypothetical universe where free will exists compared to one where it does not (dz = 0.10–0.13), indicating that people believe the existence of free will justifies inequality. However, a between-subjects design in Study 5 fails to replicate this finding. Overall, our results suggest that if belief in free will increases support for economic inequality, the effect is likely small and potentially sensitive to the methods used to detect it.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brett Mercier
- Department of Psychological Science, University of California, Irvine, Social and Behavioral Sciences Gateway Irvine, CA, US
| | - Dylan Wiwad
- Department of Psychology, Kellogg School of Management, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, US
| | - Paul K. Piff
- Department of Psychological Science, University of California, Irvine, Social and Behavioral Sciences Gateway Irvine, CA, US
| | - Lara B. Aknin
- Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, University Drive, Burnaby, BC, CA
| | - Angela R. Robinson
- Department of Psychological Science, University of California, Irvine, Social and Behavioral Sciences Gateway Irvine, CA, US
| | - Azim Shariff
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, West Mall, Vancouver, BC, CA
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Seto E, Kim J, Hicks JA. The Illusion of Time: Testing the Bidirectional Relationship Between Belief in Free Will and Temporal Horizons. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGICAL AND PERSONALITY SCIENCE 2020. [DOI: 10.1177/1948550619900060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
The current research examined the bidirectional relationship between belief in free will and perceptions of time. Studies 1 and 2 explored the association between free will beliefs (FWB) and future time perspective (FTP) in correlational and longitudinal studies, respectively. Results indicated a positive relationship between the two constructs. Study 3 manipulated FTP and assessed FWB. Study 4 manipulated FWB and assessed FTP about current goal-pursuit. While manipulating FTP did not directly influence FWB, attenuating FWB led participants to perceive time as more limited to accomplish their goals. Exploratory mediation suggests that the perception of increased paths to successful goal-pursuit accounts for this relationship. The interplay between FWB and FTP is discussed.
Collapse
|
29
|
Chandrashekar SP. It’s in Your Control: Free Will Beliefs and Attribution of Blame to Obese People and People with Mental Illness. COLLABRA: PSYCHOLOGY 2020. [DOI: 10.1525/collabra.305] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
People’s belief in free will is shown to influence the perception of personal control in self and others. The current study tested the hypothesis that individuals who believe in free will attribute stronger personal blame to obese people and to people with mental illness (schizophrenia) for their adverse health outcomes. Results from a sample of 1110 participants showed that the belief in free will subscale is positively correlated with perceptions of the controllability of these adverse health conditions. The findings suggest that free will beliefs are correlated with attribution of blame to people with obesity and mental health issues. The study contributes to the understanding of the possible negative implications of people’s free will beliefs.
Collapse
|
30
|
Brass M, Furstenberg A, Mele AR. Why neuroscience does not disprove free will. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2019; 102:251-263. [PMID: 31059730 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.04.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/24/2019] [Revised: 04/30/2019] [Accepted: 04/30/2019] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
While the question whether free will exists or not has concerned philosophers for centuries, empirical research on this question is relatively young. About 35 years ago Benjamin Libet designed an experiment that challenged the common intuition of free will, namely that conscious intentions are causally efficacious. Libet demonstrated that conscious intentions are preceded by a specific pattern of brain activation, suggesting that unconscious processes determine our decisions and we are only retrospectively informed about these decisions. Libet-style experiments have ever since dominated the discourse about the existence of free will and have found their way into the public media. Here we review the most important challenges to the common interpretation of Libet-style tasks and argue that the common interpretation is questionable. Brain activity preceding conscious decisions reflects the decision process rather than its outcome. Furthermore, the decision process is configured by conditional intentions that participants form at the beginning of the experiment. We conclude that Libet-style tasks do not provide a serious challenge to our intuition of free will.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marcel Brass
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, Ghent, 9000, Belgium.
| | - Ariel Furstenberg
- Racah Institute of Physics, Edmond and Lily Safra Center for Brain Sciences, The Hebrew University Jerusalem, Edmond J. Safra Campus, Jerusalem, 9190401, Israel.
| | - Alfred R Mele
- Department of Philosophy, Florida State University, 151 Dodd Hall, Tallahassee, 32306-1500, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
The hand of God or the hand of Maradona? Believing in free will increases perceived intentionality of others’ behavior. Conscious Cogn 2019; 70:80-87. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.02.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/24/2018] [Revised: 02/20/2019] [Accepted: 02/21/2019] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
32
|
Seitz RJ, Paloutzian RF, Angel HF. Believing is representation mediated by the dopamine brain system. Eur J Neurosci 2018; 49:1212-1214. [PMID: 30586210 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.14317] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2018] [Revised: 11/15/2018] [Accepted: 12/18/2018] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Rüdiger J Seitz
- Medical Faculty, Heinrich-Heine-University Düsseldorf, LVR-Klinikum Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany
| | | | - Hans-Ferdinand Angel
- Institute of Catechetic and Pedagogic of Religion, Karl-Franzens University Graz, Graz, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Tabb K, Lebowitz MS, Appelbaum PS. Behavioral Genetics and Attributions of Moral Responsibility. Behav Genet 2018; 49:128-135. [PMID: 30094665 DOI: 10.1007/s10519-018-9916-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2018] [Accepted: 07/27/2018] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
While considerable research has examined how genetic explanations for behavior impact assessments of moral responsibility, results across studies have been inconsistent. Some studies suggest that genetic accounts diminish ascriptions of responsibility, but others show no effect. Nonetheless, conclusions from behavior genetics are increasingly mobilized on behalf of defendants in court, suggesting a widespread intuition that this sort of information is relevant to assessments of blameworthiness. In this paper, we consider two sorts of reasons why this kind of intuition, if it exists, is not consistently revealed in empirical studies. On the one hand, people may have complex and internally conflicting intuitions about the relationship between behavior genetics and moral responsibility. On the other hand, it may be that people are motivated to think about the role of genetics in behavior differently depending on the moral valence of the actions in question.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathryn Tabb
- Center for Research on Ethical, Legal and Social Implications of Psychiatric, Neurologic and Behavioral Genetics, Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, NY, USA.
- Department of Philosophy, Columbia University, 708 Philosophy Hall, MC: 4971, 1150 Amsterdam Avenue, New York, NY, 10027, USA.
| | - Matthew S Lebowitz
- Center for Research on Ethical, Legal and Social Implications of Psychiatric, Neurologic and Behavioral Genetics, Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, NY, USA
| | - Paul S Appelbaum
- Center for Research on Ethical, Legal and Social Implications of Psychiatric, Neurologic and Behavioral Genetics, Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Koppel S, Fondacaro M, Na C. Cast into doubt: Free will and the justification for punishment. BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES & THE LAW 2018; 36:490-505. [PMID: 30004135 DOI: 10.1002/bsl.2356] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2018] [Revised: 03/10/2018] [Accepted: 03/10/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
Criminal punishment is justified on either retributive or consequential grounds. The retributive justification is premised on a common-sense view of free will: offenders can freely choose to commit crimes and so deserve blame for their actions. The consequentialist justification, in contrast, is not necessarily premised on the free will concept, but rather justifies punishment when it is the most cost-effective way of preventing crime. Science elucidating the mechanistic causes of human behavior has thrown the notion of free will into doubt, leading some to predict a shift in public support away from retribution towards consequentialism. Past research shows that free will doubt weakens support for retribution, but less is known about its effects on support for consequentialism, or about whether these effects differ across the crime severity spectrum. In this study, we explore the effects of free will doubt on support for retribution and consequentialism in response to three different categories of crime - drug crime, property crime, and violent crime - which have been shown to evoke varying levels of emotion. We find clear inconsistencies across the crime spectrum. For high affect crime, free will doubt weakens support for retribution via blame, and increases support for consequentialism. For low affect crime, free will doubt weakens support for retribution to an even greater extent, yet also decreases support for consequentialism via blame. These findings suggest that, as science reveals the mechanistic causes of criminal behavior, support for criminal punishment will decrease, especially with respect to less serious crimes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Mark Fondacaro
- John Jay College of Criminal Justice, New York, NY, USA
- CUNY Graduate Center, Psychology and Law Doctoral Training Area, New York, NY, USA
| | - Chongmin Na
- John Jay College of Criminal Justice, New York, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
A free will needs a free mind: Belief in substance dualism and reductive physicalism differentially predict belief in free will and determinism. Conscious Cogn 2018; 63:280-293. [PMID: 30001841 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.07.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2017] [Revised: 05/23/2018] [Accepted: 07/06/2018] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
In this article, we show that lay people's beliefs about how minds relate to bodies are more complex than past research suggests, and that treating them as a multidimensional construct helps explain inconclusive findings from the literature regarding their relation to beliefs about whether humans possess a free will. In two studies, we found that items previously used to assess a unidimensional belief in how minds relate to bodies indeed capture two distinguishable constructs (belief in substance dualism and reductive physicalism) that differently predict belief in free will and two types of determinism (Studies 1 and 2). Additionally, we found that two fundamental personality traits pertaining to people's preference for experiential versus rational information processing predict those metaphysical beliefs that were theorized to be based on subjective phenomenological experience and rational deliberation, respectively (Study 2). In sum, beliefs about mind-body relations are a multidimensional construct with unique predictive abilities.
Collapse
|