1
|
Perea-García JO, Teuben A, Caspar KR. Look past the cooperative eye hypothesis: reconsidering the evolution of human eye appearance. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 2025. [PMID: 40366110 DOI: 10.1111/brv.70033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2024] [Revised: 04/24/2025] [Accepted: 04/28/2025] [Indexed: 05/15/2025]
Abstract
The external appearance of the human eye has been prominently linked to the evolution of complex sociocognitive functions in our species. The cooperative eye hypothesis (CEH) proposes that human eyeballs, with their weakly expressed conjunctival and scleral pigmentation, are uniquely conspicuous and evolved under selective pressures to behave cooperatively, therefore signalling attentiveness to conspecifics. Non-human primates are instead assumed to display less-salient eye morphologies that help mask their gaze to facilitate competitive, rather than cooperative actions. Here, we argue that the CEH, although continuing to be influential, lacks robust empirical support. Over the past two decades, multidisciplinary research has undermined its original rationale and central premises: human eye pigmentation does not uniquely stand out among primates, it is not uniform at species level and the available evidence does not conclusively suggest that it facilitates gaze following to notable extents. Hence, the CEH currently provides a theoretical framework that risks confusing, rather than informing, inferences about the evolution of human external eye appearance and its selective drivers. In a call to move past it, we review alternative hypotheses with the potential to elucidate the emergence of the human ocular phenotype from the considerable spectrum of diversity found within the primate order.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juan Olvido Perea-García
- Center for Language Evolution Studies, Nicolaus Copernicus University in Toruń, Fosa Staromiejska 3, Toruń, 87-100, Poland
- University Institute for Health and Biomedical Research (IUIBS), Universidad Las Palmas de Gran Canaria, Las Palmas, Spain
| | - Aurora Teuben
- University of Amsterdam, Science Park 904, Amsterdam, 1098 XH, The Netherlands
| | - Kai R Caspar
- Institute of Cell Biology, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Universitätsstr. 1, Düsseldorf, D-40225, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Perea-García JO, Massen JJM, Ostner J, Schülke O, Castellano-Navarro A, Gazagne E, José-Domínguez JM, Beltrán-Francés V, Kaburu S, Ruppert N, Micheletta J, Gupta S, Majolo B, Maréchal L, Pflüger LS, Böhm PM, Bourjade M, Duran E, Hobaiter C, Monteiro A. Photoregulatory functions drive variation in eye coloration across macaque species. Sci Rep 2024; 14:29115. [PMID: 39582017 PMCID: PMC11586437 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-80643-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/12/2024] [Accepted: 11/21/2024] [Indexed: 11/26/2024] Open
Abstract
Primates, the most colorful mammalian radiation, have previously served as an interesting model to test the functions and evolutionary drivers of variation in eye color. We assess the contribution of photo-regulatory and communicative functions to the external eye appearance of nine macaque species representing all the branches of their radiation. Macaques' well described social structure and wide geographical distribution make them interesting to explore. We find that (1) the posterior option of the anterior eyeball is more pigmented closer to the equator, suggesting photoprotective functions. We also find that (2) the temporal side of the eyeball is more heavily pigmented than the nasal side. This suggests that eyeball pigmentation in macaques is distributed to reduce damage to the corneal limbus. The inclusion of a translocated population of M. fuscata in our analyses also suggests that external eye appearance may change quickly, perhaps owing to phenotypic plasticity. We find no evidence that communicative functions drive variation in external eye appearance in macaques. These results suggest that the amount of light in a species' environment drives variation in eye coloration across macaque species. Furthermore, the geographical distribution of macaques hints at important factors that have yet to be accounted for, such as the reflectivity of the terrain a given species inhabits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juan Olvido Perea-García
- Department of Biological Sciences, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore.
- Center for Language Evolution Studies, Nicolaus Copernicus University, Toruń, Poland.
| | - Jorg J M Massen
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Department of Biology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Julia Ostner
- Department Behavioral Ecology, JFB Institute for Zoology and Anthropology, Georg-August University, Kellnerweg 6, 37077, Göttingen, Goettingen, Germany
- Research Group Primate Social Evolution, German Primate Center Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Goettingen, Germany
- Leibniz Science Campus Primate Cognition, Goettingen, Germany
| | - Oliver Schülke
- Department Behavioral Ecology, JFB Institute for Zoology and Anthropology, Georg-August University, Kellnerweg 6, 37077, Göttingen, Goettingen, Germany
- Research Group Primate Social Evolution, German Primate Center Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Goettingen, Germany
- Leibniz Science Campus Primate Cognition, Goettingen, Germany
| | - Alba Castellano-Navarro
- Ethology and Animal Welfare Section, Universidad Cardenal Herrera-CEU, CEU Universities, Tirant lo Blanc 8, Alfara del Patriarca, Valencia, 46115, Spain
- Institute of Biology, Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany
- Unit of Research SPHERES, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
| | - Eva Gazagne
- Unit of Research SPHERES, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
- Conservation Ecology Program, King Mongkut's University of Technology, Bangkhuntien, Thailand
| | - Juan Manuel José-Domínguez
- Conservation Ecology Program, King Mongkut's University of Technology, Bangkhuntien, Thailand
- Physical Anthropology Laboratory, Department of Legal Medicine, Toxicology and Physical Anthropology, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
| | | | - Stefano Kaburu
- School of Animal, Rural and Environmental Sciences, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, NG25 0QF, UK
| | - Nadine Ruppert
- School of Biological Sciences, Universiti Sains Malaysia, Gelugor, 11800, Pulau Pinang, Malaysia
- Malaysian Primatological Society, Kulim, 09000, Kedah, Malaysia
| | - Jérôme Micheletta
- Department of Psychology, Centre for Comparative and Evolutionary Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, UK
| | - Shreejata Gupta
- Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive (LPC), Laboratoire Parole et Langage (LPL), CNRS, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France
| | | | | | - Lena S Pflüger
- Department of Behavioral and Cognitive Biology, University of Vienna, Djerassiplatz 1, Vienna, 1030, Austria
- Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto University, 41-2 Kanrin Aichi, Inuyama, 484-8506, Japan
| | - Pia M Böhm
- Department of Behavioral and Cognitive Biology, University of Vienna, Djerassiplatz 1, Vienna, 1030, Austria
| | - Marie Bourjade
- CLLE, Université de Toulouse, CNRS, Toulouse, France
- Uaso Ngiro Baboon Project, Nairobi, Kenya
| | - Elif Duran
- Department of Psychology, Izmir University of Economics, Izmir, Turkey
| | | | - Antónia Monteiro
- Department of Biological Sciences, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore.
- Science division, Yale-NUS College, Singapore, Singapore.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Perea-García JO, Berris D, Tan J, Kret ME. Pupil size and iris brightness interact to affect prosocial behaviour and affective responses. Cogn Emot 2024:1-16. [PMID: 39540646 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2427340] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2024] [Revised: 10/25/2024] [Accepted: 11/04/2024] [Indexed: 11/16/2024]
Abstract
Despite the tight link between the visibility of the iris and pupil, the perceived effects of these two have been studied largely in isolation. We demonstrate, across two experimental studies, that the effects of perceived pupil size are dependent on the visibility of the iris. In a first study, our participants donated more and had more positive impressions of portraits of non-human primates when these were manipulated to appear having larger pupils. Post-hoc inspection of our data suggested that the difference was greater for species with more conspicuous irises. In a second study, we concomitantly manipulated iris brightness and pupil size. Brighter irises and larger pupils elicited greater donations. Participants rated photographs with brighter irises as cuter, more attractive and friendlier, but only when they had dilated pupils. Our results have methodological implications for studies manipulating eye appearance, and help interpret results from previous studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Daisy Berris
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Jingzhi Tan
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Mariska E Kret
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
- Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Duran E, Perea-García JO, Piepenbrock D, Veefkind C, Kret ME, Massen JJM. Preliminary evidence that eye appearance in parrots (Psittaciformes) co-varies with latitude and altitude. Sci Rep 2024; 14:12859. [PMID: 38834673 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-63599-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2023] [Accepted: 05/30/2024] [Indexed: 06/06/2024] Open
Abstract
External eye appearance in avian taxa has been proposed to be driven by social and ecological functions. Recent research in primates suggests, instead, that, photoprotective functions are important drivers of external eye appearance. Using similar methods, we examined the variation in external eye appearance of 132 parrot species (Psittaciformes) in relation to their ecology and sociality. Breeding systems, flock size and sexual dimorphism, as well as species' latitude and maximum living altitude, and estimated UV-B incidence in species' ranges were used to explore the contribution of social and ecological factors in driving external eye appearance. We measured the hue and brightness of visible parts of the eye and the difference in measurements of brightness between adjacent parts of the eye. We found no link between social variables and our measurements. We did, however, find a negative association between the brightness of the inner part of the iris and latitude and altitude. Darker inner irises were more prevalent farther away from the equator and for those species living at higher altitudes. We found no link between UV-B and brightness measurements of the iris, or tissue surrounding the eye. We speculate that these results are consistent with an adaptation for visual functions. While preliminary, these results suggest that external eye appearance in parrots is influenced by ecological, but not social factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elif Duran
- Department of Psychology, Izmir University of Economics, Izmir, Turkey
| | - Juan Olvido Perea-García
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, Leiden, 2333 AK, The Netherlands.
| | - Diede Piepenbrock
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Department of Biology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Celine Veefkind
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Department of Biology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Mariska E Kret
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, Leiden, 2333 AK, The Netherlands
- Leiden Institute of Brain and Cognition (LIBC), Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Jorg J M Massen
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Department of Biology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
- Vogelpark Avifauna, Alphen Aan Den Rijn, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Konno A, Aoki H, Suzuki E, Furuta S, Ueda S. Are dark-eyed dogs favoured by humans? Domestication as a potential driver of iris colour difference between dogs and wolves. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2023; 10:230854. [PMID: 38126061 PMCID: PMC10731317 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.230854] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2023] [Accepted: 11/27/2023] [Indexed: 12/23/2023]
Abstract
Comparative studies have shown that the eye morphology of primates has been shaped by a variety of selection pressures (e.g. communication, environmental factors). To comprehensively elucidate the complex links between ocular morphology and its evolutionary drive, attention should be paid to other phylogenetic groups. Here, we address a new question regarding the evolution of eye colour patterns in the oldest domesticated animal, namely, the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). In this study, we conducted an image analysis of dogs and their closest relatives, grey wolves (Canis lupus), to compare the colours of their irises, with the aim of assessing whether eye colours of dogs affect how humans perceived dogs. We found that the irises of dogs were significantly darker than those of wolves. We also found that facial images of dark-eyed dogs were perceived as more friendly and immature, potentially eliciting caregiving responses from humans. Our findings are consistent with our expectation that humans favour dark-eyed dogs over light-eyed ones and provide an updated hypothesis that dogs with dark eyes may have evolved by acquiring a facial trait that sends a non-threatening gaze signal to humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Akitsugu Konno
- Department of Animal Sciences, Teikyo University of Science, Yamanashi, Japan
| | - Hitomi Aoki
- Department of Animal Sciences, Teikyo University of Science, Yamanashi, Japan
| | - Emiri Suzuki
- Department of Animal Sciences, Teikyo University of Science, Yamanashi, Japan
| | - Seiya Furuta
- Department of Animal Sciences, Teikyo University of Science, Yamanashi, Japan
| | - Sayoko Ueda
- The Mt. Fuji Institute for Nature and Biology, Showa University, Yamanashi, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Caspar KR, Hüttner L, Begall S. Scleral appearance is not a correlate of domestication in mammals. ZOOLOGICAL LETTERS 2023; 9:12. [PMID: 37248525 PMCID: PMC10228120 DOI: 10.1186/s40851-023-00210-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/09/2023] [Accepted: 05/15/2023] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
Numerous hypotheses try to explain the unusual appearance of the human eye with its bright sclera and transparent conjunctiva and how it could have evolved from a dark-eyed phenotype, as is present in many non-human primates. Recently, it has been argued that pigmentation defects induced by self-domestication may have led to bright-eyed ocular phenotypes in humans and some other primate lineages, such as marmosets. However, it has never been systematically studied whether actual domesticated mammals consistently deviate from wild mammals in regard to their conjunctival pigmentation and if this trait might therefore be part of a domestication syndrome. Here, we test this idea by drawing phylogenetically informed comparisons from a photographic dataset spanning 13 domesticated mammal species and their closest living wild relatives (n ≥ 15 photos per taxon). We did not recover significant differences in scleral appearance or irido-scleral contrast between domesticated and wild forms, suggesting that conjunctival depigmentation, unlike cutaneous pigmentation disorders, is not a general correlate of domestication. Regardless of their domestication status, macroscopically depigmented conjunctivae were observed in carnivorans and lagomorphs, whereas ungulates generally displayed darker eyes. For some taxa, we observed pronounced intraspecific variation, which should be addressed in more exhaustive future studies. Based on our dataset, we also present preliminary evidence for a general increase of conjunctival pigmentation with eye size in mammals. Our findings suggest that conjunctival depigmentation in humans is not a byproduct of self-domestication, even if we assume that our species has undergone such a process in its recent evolutionary history.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kai R Caspar
- Institute of Cell Biology, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany.
- Department of General Zoology, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany.
- Department of Game Management and Wildlife Biology, Faculty of Forestry and Wood Sciences, Czech University of Life Sciences, Praha, Czech Republic.
| | - Lisa Hüttner
- Department of General Zoology, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany
| | - Sabine Begall
- Department of General Zoology, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Chong I, Ramezanpour H, Thier P. Causal Manipulation of Gaze-Following in the Macaque Temporal Cortex. Prog Neurobiol 2023; 226:102466. [PMID: 37211234 DOI: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2023.102466] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/23/2022] [Revised: 05/09/2023] [Accepted: 05/17/2023] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
Gaze-following, the ability to shift one's own attention to places or objects others are looking at, is essential for social interactions. Single unit recordings from the monkey cortex and neuroimaging work on the human and monkey brain suggest that a distinct region in the temporal cortex, the gaze-following patch (GFP), underpins this ability. Since previous studies of the GFP have relied on correlational techniques, it remains unclear whether gaze-following related activity in the GFP indicates a causal role rather than being just a reverberation of behaviorally relevant information produced elsewhere. To answer this question, we applied focal electrical and pharmacological perturbation to the GFP. Both approaches, when applied to the GFP, disrupted gaze-following if the monkeys had been instructed to follow gaze, along with the ability to suppress it if vetoed by the context. Hence the GFP is necessary for gaze-following as well as its cognitive control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ian Chong
- Cognitive Neurology Laboratory, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany.
| | - Hamidreza Ramezanpour
- Cognitive Neurology Laboratory, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany; Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Peter Thier
- Cognitive Neurology Laboratory, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany; Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Clark IR, Lee KC, Poux T, Langergraber KE, Mitani JC, Watts D, Reed J, Sandel AA. White sclera is present in chimpanzees and other mammals. J Hum Evol 2023; 176:103322. [PMID: 36706647 PMCID: PMC9998187 DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2022.103322] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2022] [Revised: 12/22/2022] [Accepted: 12/23/2022] [Indexed: 01/27/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Isabelle R Clark
- Department of Anthropology, University of Texas at Austin, 2201 Speedway, Austin, TX 78712, USA
| | - Kevin C Lee
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change and Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, 900 S. Cady Mall, Tempe, AZ 85281, USA
| | - Tucker Poux
- Tufts University, 419 Boston Ave, Medford, MA 02155, USA
| | - Kevin E Langergraber
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change and Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, 900 S. Cady Mall, Tempe, AZ 85281, USA
| | - John C Mitani
- Department of Anthropology, University of Michigan, 1085 S. University Ave., Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
| | - David Watts
- Department of Anthropology, Yale University, P.O. Box 208277, New Haven, CT 06520, USA
| | - James Reed
- Underdog Films Ltd, 2 St. Paul's Road, Bristol, BS8 1LT, UK
| | - Aaron A Sandel
- Department of Anthropology, University of Texas at Austin, 2201 Speedway, Austin, TX 78712, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Capparini C, To MPS, Reid VM. Should I follow your virtual gaze? Infants' gaze following over video call. J Exp Child Psychol 2023; 226:105554. [PMID: 36208491 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105554] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/07/2022] [Revised: 09/12/2022] [Accepted: 09/12/2022] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
From 10 months of age, human infants start to understand the function of the eyes in the looking behavior of others to the point where they preferentially orient toward an object if the social partner has open eyes rather than closed eyes. Thus far, gaze following has been investigated in controlled laboratory paradigms. The current study investigated this early ability using a remote live testing procedure, testing infants in their everyday environment while manipulating whether the experimenter could or could not see some target objects. A total of 32 11- and 12-month-old infants' looking behavior was assessed, varying the experimenter's eye status condition (open eyes vs closed eyes) in a between-participant design. Results showed that infants followed the gaze of a virtual social partner and that they preferentially followed open eyes rather than closed eyes. These data generalize past laboratory findings to a noisier home environment and demonstrate gaze processing capacities of infants to a virtual partner interacting with the participants in a live setup.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chiara Capparini
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YF, United Kingdom; Center for Research in Cognition & Neuroscience (CRCN), Université Libre de Bruxelles, Bruxelles 1050, Belgium.
| | - Michelle P S To
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YF, United Kingdom
| | - Vincent M Reid
- School of Psychology, University of Waikato, Hamilton 3240, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Wolf W, Thielhelm J, Tomasello M. Five-year-old children show cooperative preferences for faces with white sclera. J Exp Child Psychol 2023; 225:105532. [PMID: 35988359 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105532] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/22/2021] [Revised: 07/29/2022] [Accepted: 07/29/2022] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
The cooperative eye hypothesis posits that human eye morphology evolved to facilitate cooperation. Although it is known that young children prefer stimuli with eyes that contain white sclera, it is unknown whether white sclera influences children's perception of a partner's cooperativeness specifically. In the current studies, we used an online methodology to present 5-year-old children with moving three-dimensional face models in which facial morphology was manipulated. Children found "alien" faces with human eyes more cooperative than faces with dark sclera (Study 2) but not faces with enlarged irises (Study 1). For more human-like faces (Study 3), children found human eyes more cooperative than either enlarged irises or dark sclera and found faces with enlarged irises cuter (but not more cooperative) than eyes with dark sclera. Together, these results provide strong support for the cooperative eye hypothesis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wouter Wolf
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA; Department of Developmental Psychology, Utrecht University, 3584 CS Utrecht, the Netherlands.
| | - Julia Thielhelm
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA
| | - Michael Tomasello
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA; Department of Developmental and Comparative Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Kano F. Evolution of the uniformly white sclera in humans: critical updates. Trends Cogn Sci 2023; 27:10-12. [PMID: 36229339 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.09.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2022] [Revised: 09/19/2022] [Accepted: 09/20/2022] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
The human eye characteristically has exposed and uniformly white sclera, which is hypothesized to have evolved to enhance eye-gaze signaling for conspecific communication. Although recent studies have put this hypothesis into question, current morphological and experimental evidence supports its key premise, albeit with recommendations for critical updates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fumihiro Kano
- Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, 78464, Konstanz, Germany; Max-Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, 78315, Radolfzell, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Zeiträg C, Jensen TR, Osvath M. Gaze following: A socio-cognitive skill rooted in deep time. Front Psychol 2022; 13:950935. [PMID: 36533020 PMCID: PMC9756811 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.950935] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2022] [Accepted: 10/07/2022] [Indexed: 11/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Social gaze has received much attention in social cognition research in both human and non-human animals. Gaze following appears to be a central skill for acquiring social information, such as the location of food and predators, but can also draw attention to important social interactions, which in turn promotes the evolution of more complex socio-cognitive processes such as theory of mind and social learning. In the past decades, a large number of studies has been conducted in this field introducing differing methodologies. Thereby, various factors influencing the results of gaze following experiments have been identified. This review provides an overview of the advances in the study of gaze following, but also highlights some limitations within the research area. The majority of gaze following studies on animals have focused on primates and canids, which limits evolutionary interpretations to only a few and closely related evolutionary lineages. This review incorporates new insights gained from previously understudied taxa, such as fishes, reptiles, and birds, but it will also provide a brief outline of mammal studies. We propose that the foundations of gaze following emerged early in evolutionary history. Basic, reflexive co-orienting responses might have already evolved in fishes, which would explain the ubiquity of gaze following seen in the amniotes. More complex skills, such as geometrical gaze following and the ability to form social predictions based on gaze, seem to have evolved separately at least two times and appear to be correlated with growing complexity in brain anatomy such as increased numbers of brain neurons. However, more studies on different taxa in key phylogenetic positions are needed to better understand the evolutionary history of this fundamental socio-cognitive skill.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claudia Zeiträg
- Department of Philosophy and Cognitive Science, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Zijlstra TW, van Berlo E, Kret ME. Attention Towards Pupil Size in Humans and Bonobos ( Pan paniscus). AFFECTIVE SCIENCE 2022; 3:761-771. [PMID: 36519142 PMCID: PMC9743857 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-022-00146-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2022] [Accepted: 08/07/2022] [Indexed: 05/29/2023]
Abstract
Previous work has established that humans have an attentional bias towards emotional signals, and there is some evidence that this phenomenon is shared with bonobos, our closest relatives. Although many emotional signals are explicit and overt, implicit cues such as pupil size also contain emotional information for observers. Pupil size can impact social judgment and foster trust and social support, and is automatically mimicked, suggesting a communicative role. While an attentional bias towards more obvious emotional expressions has been shown, it is unclear whether this also extends to a more subtle implicit cue, like changes in pupil size. Therefore, the current study investigated whether attention is biased towards pupils of differing sizes in humans and bonobos. A total of 150 human participants (141 female), with a mean age of 19.13 (ranging from 18 to 32 years old), completed an online dot-probe task. Four female bonobos (6 to 17 years old) completed the dot-probe task presented via a touch screen. We used linear mixed multilevel models to examine the effect of pupil size on reaction times. In humans, our analysis showed a small but significant attentional bias towards dilated pupils compared to intermediate-sized pupils and intermediate-sized pupils when compared to small pupils. Our analysis did not show a significant effect in bonobos. These results suggest that the attentional bias towards emotions in humans can be extended to a subtle unconsciously produced signal, namely changes in pupil size. Due to methodological differences between the two experiments, more research is needed before drawing a conclusion regarding bonobos. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s42761-022-00146-1.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- T. W. Zijlstra
- Cognitive Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, the Netherlands
- Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition (LIBC), Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - E. van Berlo
- Cognitive Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, the Netherlands
- Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition (LIBC), Leiden, the Netherlands
- Institute for Biodiversity and Ecosystem Dynamics, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - M. E. Kret
- Cognitive Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, the Netherlands
- Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition (LIBC), Leiden, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Horstmann G, Linke L. Perception of direct gaze in a video-conference setting: the effects of position and size. Cogn Res Princ Implic 2022; 7:67. [PMID: 35867185 PMCID: PMC9307695 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-022-00418-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/04/2022] [Accepted: 06/28/2022] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
A common problem in video conferences is gaze direction. In face-to-face communication, it is common that speaker and listener intermittently look at each other. In a video-conference setting, where multiple participants are on the screen, things are complicated and not necessarily optimal. If the listener feels looked at when the speaker looks into the camera, how tolerant is the listener for slight deviations? And does this depend on the position of the speaker’s tile on the screen, or the size of the tile? In a first experiment, participants from a student population judged whether they are looked at, while vertical gaze direction of the looker was varied. Furthermore, the position of the tile on the screen varied. The results showed that a slightly upward directed gaze was optimal for the direct gaze judgment, with a width of ± 4 degrees. Optimal gaze direction was somewhat higher for tiles at the bottom of the screen. A second experiment tested the effect of size on the perception of horizontal gaze directions. Size was found to increase the gaze cone. The paper concludes with some recommendations for a setup of video conference systems, optimized for perceived gaze contact.
Collapse
|
15
|
Wacewicz S, Perea-García JO, Lewandowski Z, Danel DP. The adaptive significance of human scleral brightness: an experimental study. Sci Rep 2022; 12:20261. [PMID: 36424405 PMCID: PMC9691750 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-24403-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/24/2022] [Accepted: 11/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/27/2022] Open
Abstract
Homogeneously depigmented sclerae have long been proposed to be uniquely human-an adaptation to enable cooperative behaviour by facilitating interpersonal coordination through gaze following. However, recent evidence has shown that deeply pigmented sclerae also afford gaze following if surrounding a bright iris. Furthermore, while current scleral depigmentation is clearly adaptive in modern humans, it is less clear how the evolutionarily intermediate stages of scleral pigmentation may have been adaptive. In sum, it is unclear why scleral depigmentation became the norm in humans, while not so in sister species like chimpanzees, or why some extant species display intermediate degrees of pigmentation (as our ancestors presumably did at some point). We created realistic facial images of 20 individually distinct hominins with diverse facial morphologies, each face in the (i) humanlike bright sclera and (ii) generalised apelike dark sclera version. Participants in two online studies rated the bright-sclera hominins as younger, healthier, more attractive and trustworthy, but less aggressive than the dark-sclera hominins. Our results support the idea that the appearance of more depigmented sclerae promoted perceived traits that fostered trust, increasing fitness for those individuals and resulting in depigmentation as a fixed trait in extant humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Slawomir Wacewicz
- Center for Language Evolution Studies, Nicolaus Copernicus University, Toruń, Poland.
| | - Juan Olvido Perea-García
- The Cognitive Psychology Unit, Faculty of Social Sciences, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Zdzisław Lewandowski
- Department of Human Biology, Wroclaw University of Health and Sport Sciences, Wroclaw, Poland
| | - Dariusz P Danel
- Department of Anthropology, Hirszfeld Institute of Immunology and Experimental Therapy, Polish Academy of Sciences, Wroclaw, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Perea-García JO, Ramarajan K, Kret ME, Hobaiter C, Monteiro A. Ecological factors are likely drivers of eye shape and colour pattern variations across anthropoid primates. Sci Rep 2022; 12:17240. [PMID: 36243745 PMCID: PMC9569326 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-20900-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2022] [Accepted: 09/20/2022] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
External eye appearance across primate species is diverse in shape and colouration, yet we still lack an explanation for the drivers of such diversity. Here we quantify substantial interspecific variation in eye shape and colouration across 77 primate species representing all extant genera of anthropoid primates. We reassess a series of hypotheses aiming to explain ocular variation in horizontal elongation and in colouration across species. Heavier body weight and terrestrial locomotion are associated with elongated eye outlines. Species living closer to the equator present more pigmented conjunctivae, suggesting photoprotective functions. Irises become bluer in species living further away from the equator, adding to existing literature supporting a circadian clock function for bluer irises. These results shift the current focus from communicative, to ecological factors in driving variation in external eye appearance in anthropoid primates. They also highlight the possibility that similar ecological factors contributed to selection for blue eyes in ancestral human populations living in northern latitudes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juan Olvido Perea-García
- Department of Biological Sciences, National University of Singapore, Singapore, 117557, Singapore.
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, 2333 AK, Leiden, The Netherlands.
| | | | - Mariska E Kret
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, 2333 AK, Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Catherine Hobaiter
- Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution and Scottish Primate Research Group, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, Fife, Scotland, UK
| | - Antónia Monteiro
- Department of Biological Sciences, National University of Singapore, Singapore, 117557, Singapore
- Science Division, Yale-NUS College, Singapore, Singapore
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Mearing AS, Burkart JM, Dunn J, Street SE, Koops K. The evolutionary drivers of primate scleral coloration. Sci Rep 2022; 12:14119. [PMID: 35982191 PMCID: PMC9388658 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-18275-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/22/2022] [Accepted: 08/08/2022] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
The drivers of divergent scleral morphologies in primates are currently unclear, though white sclerae are often assumed to underlie human hyper-cooperative behaviours. Humans are unusual in possessing depigmented sclerae whereas many other extant primates, including the closely-related chimpanzee, possess dark scleral pigment. Here, we use phylogenetic generalized least squares (PGLS) analyses with previously generated species-level scores of proactive prosociality, social tolerance (both n = 15 primate species), and conspecific lethal aggression (n = 108 primate species) to provide the first quantitative, comparative test of three existing hypotheses. The 'self-domestication' and 'cooperative eye' explanations predict white sclerae to be associated with cooperative, rather than competitive, environments. The 'gaze camouflage' hypothesis predicts that dark scleral pigment functions as gaze direction camouflage in competitive social environments. Notably, the experimental evidence that non-human primates draw social information from conspecific eye movements is unclear, with the latter two hypotheses having recently been challenged. Here, we show that white sclerae in primates are associated with increased cooperative behaviours whereas dark sclerae are associated with reduced cooperative behaviours and increased conspecific lethal violence. These results are consistent with all three hypotheses of scleral evolution, suggesting that primate scleral morphologies evolve in relation to variation in social environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alex S Mearing
- Department of Archaeology, Fitzwilliam Street, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 1QH, UK.
| | - Judith M Burkart
- Department of Anthropology, University of Zurich, 8057, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Jacob Dunn
- Department of Archaeology, Fitzwilliam Street, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 1QH, UK.,School of Life Sciences, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge, CB1 1PT, UK.,Department of Cognitive Biology, University of Vienna, 1090, Vienna, Austria
| | - Sally E Street
- Department of Anthropology, University of Durham, Durham, DH1 3LE, UK
| | - Kathelijne Koops
- Department of Archaeology, Fitzwilliam Street, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 1QH, UK.,Department of Anthropology, University of Zurich, 8057, Zurich, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Whitham W, Schapiro SJ, Troscianko J, Yorzinski JL. The gaze of a social monkey is perceptible to conspecifics and predators but not prey. Proc Biol Sci 2022; 289:20220194. [PMID: 35642370 PMCID: PMC9156918 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2022.0194] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Eye gaze is an important source of information for animals, implicated in communication, cooperation, hunting and antipredator behaviour. Gaze perception and its cognitive underpinnings are much studied in primates, but the specific features that are used to estimate gaze can be difficult to isolate behaviourally. We photographed 13 laboratory-housed tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] apella) to quantify chromatic and achromatic contrasts between their iris, pupil, sclera and skin. We used colour vision models to quantify the degree to which capuchin eye gaze is discriminable to capuchins, their predators and their prey. We found that capuchins, regardless of their colour vision phenotype, as well as their predators, were capable of effectively discriminating capuchin gaze across ecologically relevant distances. Their prey, in contrast, were not capable of discriminating capuchin gaze, even under relatively ideal conditions. These results suggest that specific features of primate eyes can influence gaze perception, both within and across species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Will Whitham
- Department of Ecology and Conservation Biology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA,Department of Comparative Medicine, UT MD Anderson Cancer Center, Bastrop, TX, USA
| | - Steven J. Schapiro
- Department of Comparative Medicine, UT MD Anderson Cancer Center, Bastrop, TX, USA
| | - Jolyon Troscianko
- Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, Exeter, UK
| | - Jessica L. Yorzinski
- Department of Ecology and Conservation Biology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Whitham W, Schapiro SJ, Troscianko J, Yorzinski JL. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) gaze is conspicuous at ecologically-relevant distances. Sci Rep 2022; 12:9249. [PMID: 35661127 PMCID: PMC9166731 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-13273-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2022] [Accepted: 05/23/2022] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) sclera appear much darker than the white sclera of human eyes, to such a degree that the direction of chimpanzee gaze may be concealed from conspecifics. Recent debate surrounding this topic has produced mixed results, with some evidence suggesting that (1) primate gaze is indeed concealed from their conspecifics, and (2) gaze colouration is among the suite of traits that distinguish uniquely social and cooperative humans from other primates (the cooperative eye hypothesis). Using a visual modelling approach that properly accounts for specific-specific vision, we reexamined this topic to estimate the extent to which chimpanzee eye coloration is discriminable. We photographed the faces of captive chimpanzees and quantified the discriminability of their pupil, iris, sclera, and surrounding skin. We considered biases of cameras, lighting conditions, and commercial photography software along with primate visual acuity, colour sensitivity, and discrimination ability. Our visual modeling of chimpanzee eye coloration suggests that chimpanzee gaze is visible to conspecifics at a range of distances (within approximately 10 m) appropriate for many species-typical behaviours. We also found that chimpanzee gaze is discriminable to the visual system of primates that chimpanzees prey upon, Colobus monkeys. Chimpanzee sclera colour does not effectively conceal gaze, and we discuss this result with regard to the cooperative eye hypothesis, the evolution of primate eye colouration, and methodological best practices for future primate visual ecology research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Will Whitham
- Department of Ecology and Conservation Biology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA. .,Department of Comparative Medicine, UT MD Anderson Cancer Center, Bastrop, TX, USA.
| | - Steven J Schapiro
- Department of Comparative Medicine, UT MD Anderson Cancer Center, Bastrop, TX, USA
| | - Jolyon Troscianko
- Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, Exeter, UK
| | - Jessica L Yorzinski
- Department of Ecology and Conservation Biology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Kano F, Kawaguchi Y, Hanling Y. Experimental evidence that uniformly white sclera enhances the visibility of eye-gaze direction in humans and chimpanzees. eLife 2022; 11:74086. [PMID: 35256053 PMCID: PMC8903827 DOI: 10.7554/elife.74086] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2021] [Accepted: 01/24/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Hallmark social activities of humans, such as cooperation and cultural learning, involve eye-gaze signaling through joint attentional interaction and ostensive communication. The gaze-signaling and related cooperative-eye hypotheses posit that humans evolved unique external eye morphologies, including uniformly white sclera (the whites of the eye), to enhance the visibility of eye-gaze for conspecifics. However, experimental evidence is still lacking. This study tested the ability of human and chimpanzee participants to discriminate the eye-gaze directions of human and chimpanzee images in computerized tasks. We varied the level of brightness and size in the stimulus images to examine the robustness of the eye-gaze directional signal against simulated shading and distancing. We found that both humans and chimpanzees discriminated eye-gaze directions of humans better than those of chimpanzees, particularly in visually challenging conditions. Also, participants of both species discriminated the eye-gaze directions of chimpanzees better when the contrast polarity of the chimpanzee eye was reversed compared to when it was normal; namely, when the chimpanzee eye has human-like white sclera and a darker iris. Uniform whiteness in the sclera thus facilitates the visibility of eye-gaze direction even across species. Our findings thus support but also critically update the central premises of the gaze-signaling hypothesis. From an early age, we are able to detect the direction others are looking in (known as eye-gaze) and make eye contact with each other to communicate. The front of the human eye has a large white area known as the sclera that surrounds the darker colored iris and pupil in the center. Compared to us, chimpanzees and other nonhuman great apes have sclerae that are much darker in color or at least not as uniformly white as human eyes. Some researchers believe that the white sclera of the human eye may have evolved to make it easier for other individuals to detect the direction of our gaze. However, there is a lack of experimental evidence as to whether white sclerae actually helps humans to distinguish the direction of eye-gaze. Here, Kano, Kawaguchi and Yeow presented human and chimpanzee participants with images of other humans and chimpanzees on a computer screen and asked them to indicate the direction of eye-gaze in each image. The experiments found that both humans and chimpanzees were better able to discriminate the directions of eye-gaze from the images of humans than those of chimpanzees, particularly when the images were smaller or more shaded. Moreover, artificially altering the eyes in the chimpanzee images so that they were more human-like – that is, had a light-colored sclera and a darker iris – enabled both humans and chimpanzees to better discriminate the eye-gaze directions of the chimpanzees. Kano, Kawaguchi and Yeow’s findings indicate that white sclerae do indeed help both humans and chimpanzees to discriminate the direction of eye-gaze, even though only humans have white sclerae. This is likely because humans use eye-gaze in key social activities (including learning languages, coordinating to complete complex tasks and transmitting cultural information), indicating that white sclerae may have evolved to enhance human-specific communication. To learn more about this type of communication, future research could focus on finding out when white sclerae first evolved.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fumihiro Kano
- Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany.,Kumamoto Sanctuary, Kyoto University, Kumamoto, Japan.,Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Radolfzell, Germany
| | - Yuri Kawaguchi
- Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Vienna, Austria.,Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS), Tokyo, Japan.,Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Japan
| | - Yeow Hanling
- Kumamoto Sanctuary, Kyoto University, Kumamoto, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Kano F, Furuichi T, Hashimoto C, Krupenye C, Leinwand JG, Hopper LM, Martin CF, Otsuka R, Tajima T. What is unique about the human eye? Comparative image analysis on the external eye morphology of human and nonhuman great apes. EVOL HUM BEHAV 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2021.12.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
22
|
Rodrigues ED, Fröhlich M. Operationalizing Intentionality in Primate Communication: Social and Ecological Considerations. INT J PRIMATOL 2021. [DOI: 10.1007/s10764-021-00248-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/05/2023]
Abstract
AbstractAn intentional transfer of information is central to human communication. When comparing nonhuman primate communication systems to language, a critical challenge is to determine whether a signal is used in intentional, goal-oriented ways. As it is not possible to directly observe psychological states in any species, comparative researchers have inferred intentionality via behavioral markers derived from studies on prelinguistic human children. Recent efforts to increase consistency in nonhuman primate communication studies undervalue the effect of possible sources of bias: some behavioral markers are not generalizable across certain signal types (gestures, vocalizations, and facial expressions), contexts, settings, and species. Despite laudable attempts to operationalize first-order intentionality across signal types, a true “multimodal” approach requires integration across their sensory components (visual-silent, contact, audible), as a signal from a certain type can comprise more than one sensory component. Here we discuss how the study of intentional communication in nonlinguistic systems is hampered by issues of reliability, validity, consistency, and generalizability. We then highlight future research avenues that may help to understand the use of goal-oriented communication by opting, whenever possible, for reliable, valid, and consistent behavioral markers, but also taking into account sampling biases and integrating detailed observations of intraspecific communicative interactions.
Collapse
|
23
|
Leroux M, Monday G, Chandia B, Akankwasa JW, Zuberbühler K, Hobaiter C, Crockford C, Townsend SW, Asiimwe C, Fedurek P. First observation of a chimpanzee with albinism in the wild: Social interactions and subsequent infanticide. Am J Primatol 2021; 84:e23305. [PMID: 34270104 PMCID: PMC9541794 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23305] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2021] [Revised: 06/28/2021] [Accepted: 07/05/2021] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Albinism—the congenital absence of pigmentation—is a very rare phenomenon in animals due to the significant costs to fitness of this condition. Both humans and non‐human individuals with albinism face a number of challenges, such as reduced vision, increased exposure to ultraviolet radiation, or compromised crypticity resulting in an elevated vulnerability to predation. However, while observations of social interactions involving individuals with albinism have been observed in wild non‐primate animals, such interactions have not been described in detail in non‐human primates (hereafter, primates). Here, we report, to our knowledge, the first sighting of an infant with albinism in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii), including social interactions between the infant, its mother, and group members. We also describe the subsequent killing of the infant by conspecifics as well as their behavior towards the corpse following the infanticide. Finally, we discuss our observations in relation to our understanding of chimpanzee behavior or attitudes towards individuals with very conspicuous appearances. Observations of wild non‐human primates with albinism are extremely rare We report the first observation of a chimpanzee with albinism in the wild We describe interactions between the infant with albinism and other group members We describe the subsequent infanticide of the individual with albinism We discuss these observations in light of our understanding of chimpanzee behavior
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maël Leroux
- Department of Comparative Language Science, University of Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland.,Budongo Conservation Field Station, Masindi, Uganda.,Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Language Evolution (ISLE), University of Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland
| | | | | | | | - Klaus Zuberbühler
- Budongo Conservation Field Station, Masindi, Uganda.,Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Language Evolution (ISLE), University of Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland.,Department of Comparative Cognition, Institute of Biology, University of Neuchâtel, Neuchâtel, Switzerland.,Center for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, UK
| | - Catherine Hobaiter
- Budongo Conservation Field Station, Masindi, Uganda.,Center for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, UK
| | | | - Simon W Townsend
- Department of Comparative Language Science, University of Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland.,Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Language Evolution (ISLE), University of Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland.,Department of Psychology, University of Warwick, Warwick, UK
| | | | - Pawel Fedurek
- Budongo Conservation Field Station, Masindi, Uganda.,Division of Psychology, Faculty of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling, UK
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Diversity in Primate External Eye Morphology: Previously Undescribed Traits and Their Potential Adaptive Value. Symmetry (Basel) 2021. [DOI: 10.3390/sym13071270] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023] Open
Abstract
Comparative examinations of external eye morphology in primates initially focused on communicative functions of the eye. Subsequent work has failed to find consistent associations between specific eye morphologies and communicative functions. In this article, we review the field of primate external eye morphology and inspect publicly available and unpublished photographs. We identify and describe five commonly occurring traits that have not received attention so far. We cross-examined the clinical and psychological literature to propose potential adaptive functions. These potential adaptive functions include communicative functions, but also photoregulatory functions and photoprotective functions.
Collapse
|
25
|
Quantifying gaze conspicuousness: Are humans distinct from chimpanzees and bonobos? J Hum Evol 2021; 157:103043. [PMID: 34246864 DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2021.103043] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/11/2021] [Revised: 06/07/2021] [Accepted: 06/07/2021] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
26
|
Birch J, Heyes C. The cultural evolution of cultural evolution. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2021; 376:20200051. [PMID: 33993760 PMCID: PMC8126465 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0051] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/15/2020] [Accepted: 09/03/2020] [Indexed: 12/25/2022] Open
Abstract
What makes fast, cumulative cultural evolution work? Where did it come from? Why is it the sole preserve of humans? We set out a self-assembly hypothesis: cultural evolution evolved culturally. We present an evolutionary account that shows this hypothesis to be coherent, plausible, and worthy of further investigation. It has the following steps: (0) in common with other animals, early hominins had significant capacity for social learning; (1) knowledge and skills learned by offspring from their parents began to spread because bearers had more offspring, a process we call CS1 (or Cultural Selection 1); (2) CS1 shaped attentional learning biases; (3) these attentional biases were augmented by explicit learning biases (judgements about what should be copied from whom). Explicit learning biases enabled (4) the high-fidelity, exclusive copying required for fast cultural accumulation of knowledge and skills by a process we call CS2 (or Cultural Selection 2) and (5) the emergence of cognitive processes such as imitation, mindreading and metacognition-'cognitive gadgets' specialized for cultural learning. This self-assembly hypothesis is consistent with archaeological evidence that the stone tools used by early hominins were not dependent on fast, cumulative cultural evolution, and suggests new priorities for research on 'animal culture'. This article is part of the theme issue 'Foundations of cultural evolution'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan Birch
- Department of Philosophy, Logic and Scientific Method, London School of Economics and Political Science, Houghton Street, London WC2A 2AE, UK
| | - Cecilia Heyes
- All Souls College and Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 4AL, UK
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Caspar KR, Biggemann M, Geissmann T, Begall S. Ocular pigmentation in humans, great apes, and gibbons is not suggestive of communicative functions. Sci Rep 2021; 11:12994. [PMID: 34155285 PMCID: PMC8217224 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-92348-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2021] [Accepted: 06/09/2021] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Pigmentation patterns of the visible part of the eyeball, encompassing the iris and portions of the sclera, have been discussed to be linked to social cognition in primates. The cooperative eye hypothesis suggests the white sclera of humans to be a derived adaptive trait that enhances eye-mediated communication. Here, we provide a comparative analysis of ocular pigmentation patterns in 15 species of hominoids (humans, great apes & gibbons) that show marked differences in social cognition and quantify scleral exposure at the genus level. Our data reveals a continuum of eye pigmentation traits in hominoids which does not align with the complexity of gaze-mediated communication in the studied taxa. Gibbons display darker eyes than great apes and expose less sclera. Iridoscleral contrasts in orangutans and gorillas approach the human condition but differ between congeneric species. Contrary to recent discussions, we found chimpanzee eyes to exhibit a cryptic coloration scheme that resembles gibbons more than other apes. We reevaluate the evidence for links between social cognition and eye pigmentation in primates, concluding that the cooperative eye hypothesis cannot explain the patterns observed. Differences in scleral pigmentation between great apes and humans are gradual and might have arisen via genetic drift and sexual selection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kai R Caspar
- Department of General Zoology, University of Duisburg-Essen, Universitaetsstraße 5, 45141, Essen, Germany.
| | - Marco Biggemann
- Department of General Zoology, University of Duisburg-Essen, Universitaetsstraße 5, 45141, Essen, Germany
| | - Thomas Geissmann
- Anthropological Institute, University Zurich-Irchel, Winterthurerstraße 190, 8057, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Sabine Begall
- Department of General Zoology, University of Duisburg-Essen, Universitaetsstraße 5, 45141, Essen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Yorzinski JL, Harbourne A, Thompson W. Sclera color in humans facilitates gaze perception during daytime and nighttime. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0249137. [PMID: 33780503 PMCID: PMC8006985 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249137] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/25/2020] [Accepted: 03/11/2021] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Species vary widely in the conspicuousness of their eye morphology and this could influence gaze perception. Eyes with conspicuous morphology can enhance gaze perception while eyes with camouflaged morphology may hinder gaze perception. While evidence suggests that conspicuous eye morphology enhances gaze perception, little is known about how environmental conditions affect this interaction. Thus, we investigated whether environmental light conditions affect gaze perception. Human subjects (Homo sapiens) were instructed to find direct-gaze faces within arrays of averted-gaze faces or to find averted-gaze faces within arrays of directed-gaze faces. The faces were displayed under conditions simulating nighttime or daytime conditions. Furthermore, the faces had naturally-colored sclera (white) or modified sclera (same color as the iris). Participants were fastest and most accurate in detecting faces during the daytime and nighttime conditions when the sclera were naturally-colored. Participants were worst at detecting faces with modified sclera during the nighttime conditions. These results suggest that eyes with conspicuous morphology enhance gaze perception during both daytime and nighttime conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica L. Yorzinski
- Department of Ecology and Conservation Biology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Texas, United States of America
| | - Amy Harbourne
- Department of English, Texas A&M University, College Station, Texas, United States of America
| | - William Thompson
- School of Computing, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Facial expressions can be categorized along the upper-lower facial axis, from a perceptual perspective. Atten Percept Psychophys 2021; 83:2159-2173. [PMID: 33759116 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-021-02281-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/09/2021] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
A critical question, fundamental for building models of emotion, is how to categorize emotions. Previous studies have typically taken one of two approaches: (a) they focused on the pre-perceptual visual cues, how salient facial features or configurations were displayed; or (b) they focused on the post-perceptual affective experiences, how emotions affected behavior. In this study, we attempted to group emotions at a peri-perceptual processing level: it is well known that humans perceive different facial expressions differently, therefore, can we classify facial expressions into distinct categories in terms of their perceptual similarities? Here, using a novel non-lexical paradigm, we assessed the perceptual dissimilarities between 20 facial expressions using reaction times. Multidimensional-scaling analysis revealed that facial expressions were organized predominantly along the upper-lower face axis. Cluster analysis of behavioral data delineated three superordinate categories, and eye-tracking measurements validated these clustering results. Interestingly, these superordinate categories can be conceptualized according to how facial displays interact with acoustic communications: One group comprises expressions that have salient mouth features. They likely link to species-specific vocalization, for example, crying, laughing. The second group comprises visual displays with diagnosing features in both the mouth and the eye regions. They are not directly articulable but can be expressed prosodically, for example, sad, angry. Expressions in the third group are also whole-face expressions but are completely independent of vocalization, and likely being blends of two or more elementary expressions. We propose a theoretical framework to interpret the tripartite division in which distinct expression subsets are interpreted as successive phases in an evolutionary chain.
Collapse
|
30
|
Yorzinski JL, Thorstenson CA, Nguyen TP. Sclera and Iris Color Interact to Influence Gaze Perception. Front Psychol 2021; 12:632616. [PMID: 33776853 PMCID: PMC7989964 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.632616] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/24/2020] [Accepted: 02/16/2021] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The white sclera is important in facilitating gaze perception in humans. Iris color may likewise influence gaze perception but no previous studies have directly assessed its effect. We therefore examined how the interaction between sclera and iris color influences human gaze perception. We recorded the eye movements of human participants as they performed a visual search task with human faces exhibiting directed or averted gaze. The faces either exhibited light or dark irises. In addition, the faces had sclera that were depigmented (white) or pigmented (matched the color of the iris). We found that participants were quick and accurate in evaluating gaze regardless of iris color in faces with depigmented sclera. When the sclera were pigmented, participants were slower to evaluate the gaze of faces with both light and dark irises but these effects were most pronounced in the faces with dark irises. Furthermore, participants were generally less accurate in assessing faces with pigmented sclera when the irises were dark rather than light. Our results suggest that depigmented sclera are especially important for gaze perception in faces with dark irises. Because depigmented sclera likely evolved at a time when ancestral humans exhibited dark irises, the depigmented sclera may have been crucial for efficient and accurate gaze perception in ancestral humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica L Yorzinski
- Department of Ecology and Conservation Biology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, United States
| | - Christopher A Thorstenson
- Department of Psychology and Wisconsin Institute for Discovery, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, United States
| | - Trezze P Nguyen
- Department of Wildlife and Fisheries Sciences, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, United States
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Sex differences in ocular morphology in Caucasian people: a dubious role of sexual selection in the evolution of sexual dimorphism of the human eye. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2020. [DOI: 10.1007/s00265-020-02894-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
The horizontal size of the exposed depigmented sclera in Caucasians has been previously suggested to be sexually dimorphic, and the significance of this phenomenon remains unclear. Here we build on a previous study and extend it by (i) examining sex differences in other measures of ocular morphology and (ii) exploring the link between eye morphology and biometric markers of facial attractiveness. We used facial photographs of 100 Caucasians (50 men) from Eastern-Central Europe and digitally measured four ocular features. Eye measurements were tested for sex differences and associations with morphometric data on facial averageness and sexual shape dimorphism. We found that sclera surface is more horizontally exposed in men, even though the total surface area is similar in both sexes. We also found that eye fissures are rounder (less rectangular) in women than in men and that irises are brighter in women. We did not find any relationship between the examined eye features and two aspects of facial attractiveness: facial averageness and sexual dimorphism in facial shape. Despite being sexually dimorphic, eye features may be loosely linked with the development of facial sexual ornamentation. The role of sexual selection in the evolution of the observed phenomena is disputable.
Significance statement
It is often argued that because of their physical appearance, human eyes are crucial to interpersonal and social interactions. In many aspects, however, the significance of the human eye architecture is unclear. In this study, we examine sex differences in eye morphology and explore the link between ocular features and biometric measures of facial attractiveness in Caucasian men and women. We found that despite being sexually dimorphic, eye features may be loosely linked with biometric markers of facial attractiveness. We argue that the role of sexual selection in the evolution of the observed sex differences is disputable.
Collapse
|
32
|
Emotional expressions in human and non-human great apes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2020; 115:378-395. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2019] [Revised: 01/17/2020] [Accepted: 01/22/2020] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
33
|
|
34
|
Dalmaso M, Alessi G, Castelli L, Galfano G. Eye contact boosts the reflexive component of overt gaze following. Sci Rep 2020; 10:4777. [PMID: 32179802 PMCID: PMC7075930 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-61619-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2019] [Accepted: 02/25/2020] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Establishing eye contact with an individual can subsequently lead to a stronger gaze-mediated orienting effect. However, studies exploring this phenomenon have, so far, only assessed manual responses and focused on covert attention - namely, without eye movements. Here, in two experiments, we explored for the first time whether eye contact can also impact on overt attention in an oculomotor task. This approach has two main advantages, in that it relies on more sensitive, online measures of attention allocation and it better mimics real life settings. Participants performed leftwards and rightwards eye movements in response to a central cue. Furthermore, a task-irrelevant central face established - or not - eye contact with the participant, and then averted its gaze either leftwards or rightwards. Hence, eye movement direction was either congruent or incongruent with that of the gaze stimulus. In both experiments, a gaze following behaviour emerged - specifically, smaller saccadic latencies and a greater accuracy emerged on congruent than on incongruent trials - but its magnitude was not modulated by eye contact. However, in Experiment 2 - in which the different eye contact conditions were presented intermixed rather than blocked, thus making eye contact contextually salient - eye contact led to an overall decrement of saccadic latencies and enhanced the reflexive component of gaze following. Taken together, these results provide novel evidence indicating that eye contact can impact on both eye movements programming and overt gaze following mechanisms, at least when eye contact is made contextually salient.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mario Dalmaso
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology University of Padova, Padova, Italy.
| | - Giada Alessi
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | - Luigi Castelli
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | - Giovanni Galfano
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| |
Collapse
|