1
|
Jones M, Bosacki S, Talwar V. Interrelations Among Adolescents' Family Connections, Solitude Preferences, Theory of Mind and Perceptions of Academic and Work Competence. J Genet Psychol 2025; 186:56-72. [PMID: 39126355 DOI: 10.1080/00221325.2024.2386016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/05/2023] [Accepted: 06/28/2024] [Indexed: 08/12/2024]
Abstract
This study explored the links among family connections, solitude preferences, perceptions of work (academic and job) competence, and Theory of Mind (ToM) in 73 Canadian adolescents aged 11-18 (M age = 13.1). Previous studies show significant connections among these factors, although little is known about how such associations may relate to one another, especially the role of young people's perceptions. To address these gaps in the literature, this study focused on adolescents' experiences and perceptions of their family relationships, solitude preferences, and competence in the school context and workplace. Participants completed a series of self-report measures, advanced ToM tasks and written explanation for perceived family emotional connections. Results revealed that adolescents with more positive family connections reported higher levels of self-perceived academic and job competence, embraced solitude positively, yet felt less desire to be alone. Girls showed a higher affinity for solitude than boys, and the presence of more siblings reduced the desire for solitude. Those youth who were proficient in ToM skills reported positive family connections and high levels of academic competence. Findings hold implications for future research and education in adolescent's social cognition and social and academic outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Megan Jones
- Department of Psychology, University of Exeter, Exeter, UK
| | - Sandra Bosacki
- Department of Educational Studies, Brock University, St. Catharines, Canada
| | - Victoria Talwar
- Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Symeonidou M, Mizokawa A, Kabaya S, Doherty MJ, Ross J. Contrasting one's share of the shared life space: Comparing the roles of metacognition and inhibitory control in the development of theory of mind among Scottish and Japanese children. Dev Sci 2024; 27:e13417. [PMID: 37408284 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13417] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2022] [Revised: 04/24/2023] [Accepted: 05/12/2023] [Indexed: 07/07/2023]
Abstract
Cultural comparisons suggest that an understanding of other minds may develop sooner in independent versus interdependent settings, and vice versa for inhibitory control. From a western lens, this pattern might be considered paradoxical, since there is a robust positive relationship between theory of mind (ToM) and inhibitory control in western samples. In independent cultures, an emphasis on one's own mind offers a clear route to 'simulate' other minds, and inhibitory control may be required to set aside one's own perspective to represent the perspective of others. However, in interdependent cultures, social norms are considered the key catalyst for behaviour, and metacognitive reflection and/or suppression of one's own perspective may not be necessary. The cross-cultural generalizability of the western developmental route to ToM is therefore questionable. The current study used an age-matched cross-sectional sample to contrast 56 Japanese and 56 Scottish 3-6-year-old's metacognition, ToM and inhibitory control skills. We replicated the expected cultural patterns for ToM (Scotland > Japan) and inhibitory control (Japan > Scotland). Supporting western developmental enrichment theories, we find that inhibitory control and metacognition predict theory of mind competence in Scotland. However, these variables cannot be used to predict Japanese ToM. This confirms that individualistic mechanisms do not capture the developmental mechanism underlying ToM in Japan, highlighting a bias in our understanding of ToM development. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: We replicate an independent cultural advantage for theory of mind (Scotland > Japan) and interdependent advantage for inhibitory control (Japan > Scotland). From a western lens, this pattern might be considered paradoxical, since there is a robust positive relationship between theory of mind and inhibitory control. Supporting western developmental enrichment theories, we find that the development of inhibitory control mediates the link between metacognition and theory of mind in Scotland. However, this model does not predict Japanese theory of mind, highlighting an individualistic bias in our mechanistic understanding of theory of mind development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mariel Symeonidou
- Psychology, School of Humanities, Social Sciences and Law, University of Dundee, Dundee, Scotland
| | - Ai Mizokawa
- Graduate School of Education and Human Development, Nagoya University, Nagoya, Japan
| | - Shinsuke Kabaya
- Department of Psychology, Aichi Shukutoku University, Nagakute, Aichi, Japan
| | | | - Josephine Ross
- Psychology, School of Humanities, Social Sciences and Law, University of Dundee, Dundee, Scotland
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Figueroa M, Bayés G, Darbra S, Silvestre N. Reading and Theory of Mind during the Primary-Secondary Educational Transition: A Multiple Case Study in Pupils with a Cochlear Implant. READING PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/02702711.2022.2156953] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Mario Figueroa
- Department of Basic, Developmental and Educational Psychology, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | | | - Sònia Darbra
- Department of Psychobiology and Methodology of Health Sciences, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
- Neurosciences Institute, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Nùria Silvestre
- Department of Basic, Developmental and Educational Psychology, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Hou XH, Liu Y, Li Y, Wang LY. Sequence of theory of mind acquisition in ethnic minority children in China: A comparison of Zhuang and Han samples. CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1007/s12144-022-03945-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
|
5
|
Pérez-Mata N, Moreno A, Diges M, Peláez M. How Chronological Age, Theory of Mind, and Yield are Interrelated to Memory and Suggestion in Young Children. THE SPANISH JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2022; 25:e26. [PMID: 36210368 DOI: 10.1017/sjp.2022.22] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
We investigated the interrelations between chronological age, theory of mind (ToM), Yield (as a measure of individual suggestibility), memory and acceptance of experimental suggestion in a sample of children between 3 and 7 years old (N = 106). One week after participants interacted with 'a Teacher', they were asked to recall activities carried out with the Teacher (direct experience) and the contents of a story read to them by the Teacher (indirect experience). Data were examined with an analysis of developmental trajectories, which allows establishing the predictor value of socio-cognitive developmental factors regardless of participants' chronological age. It also estimates predictor values in interaction with the age and determines whether age is the best predictor for performance. As in previous research, results showed that chronological age was the main predictor of memory performance, both for direct experience (i.e., activities performed) and indirect experience (i.e., contents of the story). However, ToM and Yield, together with participants' ages, modulated their acceptance of the external suggestions received (presented only once, one week after the event). A turning point was observed at age 4.6. Below this age, the greater the mentalist skills (higher ToM), the lower was the vulnerability to external suggestion. Still, children below this age characterized individually as being suggestible (Yield medium or high) were more vulnerable to suggestion the younger they were. Thus, developmental socio-cognitive factors might modulate young children's vulnerability to external suggestions, even if received only once.
Collapse
|
6
|
Takamatsu R. Empathy and group processes in Japanese preschool children: The odd one out among friends receives less empathic concern than out-groups. J Exp Child Psychol 2022; 221:105460. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105460] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/29/2021] [Revised: 04/16/2022] [Accepted: 04/19/2022] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
7
|
Taumoepeau M, Kata 'UF, Veikune 'AH, Lotulelei S, Vea PT, Fonua 'I. Could, would, should: Theory of mind and deontic reasoning in Tongan children. Child Dev 2022; 93:1511-1526. [PMID: 35616232 PMCID: PMC9545884 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13797] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2021] [Revised: 03/13/2022] [Accepted: 03/14/2022] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
This study examined the developmental profiles of children's social reasoning about individual agentive and deontic concerns. Tongan children (N = 140, 47.9% male), aged 4–8 years, were given a set of mentalistic (standard theory‐of‐mind) and deontic reasoning tasks. On average, children found diverse desires, knowledge access, hidden emotion, and belief emotion easier than the false‐belief and diverse belief tasks. Tongan children were sensitive to social norms governing behavior, and this information was recruited for predicting behavior in a false‐belief task when embedded in a socially normative context. We discuss the potential for cultural mandates to shape children's social understanding and the impact of culture on our theoretical framing of children's development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mele Taumoepeau
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | | | - 'Ana Heti Veikune
- School of Language, Arts and Media, University of the South Pacific, Tonga Campus, Tonga
| | - Susana Lotulelei
- School of Teaching Studies, Tupou Tertiary Institute, Nuku'alofa, Tonga
| | - Peseti Tupou'ila Vea
- School of Language, Arts and Media, University of the South Pacific, Tonga Campus, Tonga
| | - 'Ilaisaane Fonua
- School of Teaching Studies, Tupou Tertiary Institute, Nuku'alofa, Tonga
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Fujita N, Devine RT, Hughes C. Theory of mind and executive function in early childhood: A cross-cultural investigation. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101150] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
9
|
Miyamoto S, Harashima S, Yoshiuchi K. Validating the family coping questionnaire for eating disorders for caregivers of Japanese patients with eating disorders: association between coping strategies and psychological characteristics. J Eat Disord 2021; 9:163. [PMID: 34922632 PMCID: PMC8683820 DOI: 10.1186/s40337-021-00517-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/25/2021] [Accepted: 11/28/2021] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Eating disorders (ED) can adversely affect the psychological health of patients' caregivers. The present study aimed to validate a Japanese version of the Family Coping Questionnaire for Eating Disorders (FCQ-ED-J) and investigate the association between the coping strategies and psychological states of the caregivers of ED patients. METHODS The caregivers completed the FCQ-ED-J and the Profile of Mood States. The FCQ-ED measures the coping strategies of caregivers of ED patients to the ED symptom-related behaviors. As confirmatory factor analysis did not yield an adequate model fit, the factor structure of the FCQ-ED-J was analyzed using exploratory factor analysis. Subsequently, the reliability and validity of the FCQ-ED-J were examined using Cronbach's alpha and Pearson's correlation coefficients in relation to the Profile of Mood States. RESULTS Data from 150 caregivers, including 91 mothers and 34 fathers, was analyzed (mean age 51.1 years, SD = 12.0). The FCQ-ED-J, with 13 items grouped across four subscales ["response to binge-eating" (factor 1), "response to frequent weighing" (factor 2), "response to too much physical exercise" (factor 3), and "response to abusing laxatives and/or diuretics" (factor 4)] had Cronbach's alpha values representing acceptable to good internal consistency (0.71-0.85). Each subscale of the FCQ-ED-J was significantly correlated with the Profile of Mood States subscales. CONCLUSIONS The FCQ-ED-J had sufficient reliability and validity. The Japanese caregivers' responses to the patient's ED symptom-related behavior were associated with their psychological states. Thus, the FCQ-ED-J may offer insight into more effective and reasonable care by caregivers for ED patients. The Family Coping Questionnaire has been used by researchers to assess the coping strategies of the relatives of patients. The present study aimed to validate a Japanese version of the Family Coping Questionnaire for Eating Disorders (FCQ-ED-J) and investigate the association between the coping strategies and psychological states of the caregivers of ED patients. Data from 150 caregivers, including 91 mothers and 34 fathers, were analyzed. The FCQ-ED-J comprised 13 items grouped into four subscales, with acceptable to good internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha values between 0.71 and 0.85). All subscales of the FCQ-ED-J were found to be statistically significantly correlated with the profile of mood states (POMS) subscales. The Japanese caregivers' responses to the patient's ED symptom-related behavior were associated with their psychological states. Thus, the FCQ-ED-J can be utilized to help caregivers provide more effective and reasonable psychological care and support to ED patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Seraki Miyamoto
- Department of Stress Sciences and Psychosomatic Medicine, Graduate School of Medicine, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Saki Harashima
- Department of Stress Sciences and Psychosomatic Medicine, Graduate School of Medicine, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Kazuhiro Yoshiuchi
- Department of Stress Sciences and Psychosomatic Medicine, Graduate School of Medicine, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Read DW, Manrique HM, Walker MJ. On the Working Memory of Humans and Great Apes: Strikingly Similar or Remarkably Different? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 134:104496. [PMID: 34919985 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.12.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/22/2020] [Revised: 11/08/2021] [Accepted: 12/09/2021] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
In this article we review publications relevant to addressing widely reported claims in both the academic and popular press that chimpanzees working memory (WM) is comparable to, if not exceeding, that of humans. WM is a complex multidimensional construct with strong parallels in humans to prefrontal cortex and cognitive development. These parallels occur in chimpanzees, but to a lesser degree. We review empirical evidence and conclude that the size of WM in chimpanzees is 2 ± 1 versus Miller's famous 7 ± 2 in humans. Comparable differences occur in experiments on chimpanzees relating to strategic and attentional WM subsystems. Regardless of the domain, chimpanzee WM performance is comparable to that of humans around the age of 4 or 5. Next, we review evidence showing parallels among the evolution of WM capacity in hominins ancestral to Homo sapiens, the phylogenetic evolution of hominins leading to Homo sapiens, and evolution in the complexity of stone tool technology over this time period.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dwight W Read
- Department of Anthropology, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, 90095, USA; Department of Statistics, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, 90095, USA.
| | - Héctor M Manrique
- Departamento de Psicología y Sociología, Universidad de Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain
| | - Michael J Walker
- Department of Zoology and Physical Anthropology, Faculty of Biology, University of Murcia, Murcia, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Wang S, Andrews G, Pendergast D, Neumann D, Chen Y, Shum DHK. A Cross-Cultural Study of Theory of Mind Using Strange Stories in School-Aged Children from Australia and Mainland China. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2021. [DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2021.1974445] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Si Wang
- Griffith University, Australia
| | | | | | | | - Yulu Chen
- Beijing Union University Teachers’ College, China
| | - David H. K. Shum
- Griffith University, Australia
- The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Mirski R, Bickhard MH. Conventional minds: An interactivist perspective on social cognition and its enculturation. NEW IDEAS IN PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2021.100856] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
13
|
Rajhi O, Halayem S, Ghazzai M, Taamallah A, Moussa M, Abbes ZS, Hajri M, Ben Yahia H, Touati M, Fakhfakh R, Bouden A. Validation of the Tunisian Social Situation Instrument in the General Pediatric Population. Front Psychol 2020; 11:557173. [PMID: 33192812 PMCID: PMC7658408 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.557173] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2020] [Accepted: 09/29/2020] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Background In order to better understand the deployment of the theory of mind (ToM) in Tunisian neurotypical children, we have developed a new tool of assessment of the ToM called the “Tunisian Social Situations Instrument” (TSSI). We opted for the creation of this test in view of the intercultural differences in the development of social skills. Our purpose was to validate this tool in general pediatric population. Methods It was a cross-sectional evaluative study that aimed to validate the TSSI in the general pediatric population. We initially conducted a beta test and a pre-validation study before taking the initial version of the TSSI on 123 neurotypical children. Then, we followed the typical validation procedure: appearance validity, content validity, construct validity, and reliability study. Results Regarding the validity of appearance, the TSSI was comprehensible and adapted to the Tunisian pediatric population. About content validity, the exploratory factor analysis extracted 6 factors that explain 69.3% of the total variance. These factors were respectively social clumsiness types 1 and 2, intention attribution, emotional ToM, epistemic ToM, and simple comprehension questions. The subdomains of social clumsiness (types 1 and 2) and emotional ToM had a Cronbach alpha higher than 0.8. This factor structure as well as the significant inter-correlation between subdomains and the global score were in favor of a good construct validity. The internal consistency study showed good reliability of the final version of the TSSI (alpha of Cronbach at 0,809). Regarding the performance of children at the TSSI, we have noticed a significant association between the global score, age, and verbal intelligence. Conclusion This work offers valuable insights about ToM and provides clinicians with a reliable tool to assess social clumsiness and emotional ToM in typically developing children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Olfa Rajhi
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Razi Hospital, Manouba, Tunisia.,Faculty of Medicine of Tunis, University of Tunis El Manar, Tunis, Tunisia
| | - Soumeyya Halayem
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Razi Hospital, Manouba, Tunisia.,Faculty of Medicine of Tunis, University of Tunis El Manar, Tunis, Tunisia
| | - Malek Ghazzai
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Razi Hospital, Manouba, Tunisia.,Faculty of Medicine of Tunis, University of Tunis El Manar, Tunis, Tunisia
| | - Amal Taamallah
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Razi Hospital, Manouba, Tunisia
| | - Mohamed Moussa
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Razi Hospital, Manouba, Tunisia.,Faculty of Medicine of Tunis, University of Tunis El Manar, Tunis, Tunisia
| | - Zeineb Salma Abbes
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Razi Hospital, Manouba, Tunisia.,Faculty of Medicine of Tunis, University of Tunis El Manar, Tunis, Tunisia
| | - Malek Hajri
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Razi Hospital, Manouba, Tunisia.,Faculty of Medicine of Tunis, University of Tunis El Manar, Tunis, Tunisia
| | - Houda Ben Yahia
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Razi Hospital, Manouba, Tunisia
| | - Maissa Touati
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Razi Hospital, Manouba, Tunisia
| | - Radhouane Fakhfakh
- Faculty of Medicine of Tunis, University of Tunis El Manar, Tunis, Tunisia.,Department of Preventive Medicine, Abderrahmen Mami Hospital, Ariana, Tunis, Tunisia
| | - Asma Bouden
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Razi Hospital, Manouba, Tunisia.,Faculty of Medicine of Tunis, University of Tunis El Manar, Tunis, Tunisia
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Mizokawa A, Hamana M. The relationship of theory of mind and maternal emotional expressiveness with aggressive behaviours in young Japanese children: A
gender‐differentiated
effect. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2020. [DOI: 10.1002/icd.2196] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Ai Mizokawa
- Graduate School of Education and Human Development Nagoya University Nagoya Japan
| | - Mai Hamana
- Senzoku Junior College of Childhood Education Kawasaki Japan
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Xu C, Ellefson MR, Ng FFY, Wang Q, Hughes C. An East-West contrast in executive function: Measurement invariance of computerized tasks in school-aged children and adolescents. J Exp Child Psychol 2020; 199:104929. [PMID: 32711217 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104929] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2020] [Revised: 05/07/2020] [Accepted: 05/31/2020] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Existing cross-cultural findings related to school-aged children's executive function (EF) from studies using computerized tasks highlight both an East-West contrast (East > West) and potential methodological confounds (e.g., contrasting levels of computer fluency). Capitalizing on two recent data sets, this multisite study of 1,311 children living in mainland China (n = 453; Mage = 11.89 years, SD = 0.87), Hong Kong (n = 371; Mage = 12.21 years, SD = 0.99), and the United Kingdom (n = 487; Mage = 11.91 years, SD = 0.93) tested measurement invariance of a computerized EF-task battery prior to investigating cultural contrasts in mean levels of EF efficiency scores. Our models established partial scalar invariance across sites. Latent factor means were substantially lower for British children than for their counterparts from either mainland China or Hong Kong, with a significant but smaller contrast between the latter two groups. Within the Chinese sample, self-reported computer use was unrelated to variation in children's performance on online tests of EF, indicating that peripheral effects of task modality are unlikely to explain the between-culture differences in EF task performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chengyi Xu
- Centre for Family Research, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3RQ, UK.
| | | | - Florrie Fei-Yin Ng
- Department of Educational Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, NT, Hong Kong
| | - Qian Wang
- Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, NT, Hong Kong
| | - Claire Hughes
- Centre for Family Research, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3RQ, UK
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Fujita N, Hughes C. Mind‐mindedness and self–other distinction: Contrasts between Japanese and British mothers’ speech samples. SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 2020. [DOI: 10.1111/sode.12454] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Nao Fujita
- Centre for Family Research University of Cambridge Cambridge UK
| | - Claire Hughes
- Centre for Family Research University of Cambridge Cambridge UK
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Kabha L, Berger A. The sequence of acquisition for theory of mind concepts: The combined effect of both cultural and environmental factors. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100852] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
18
|
Perspective‐shifting discourse training to improve young Japanese children's understanding of theory of mind. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2020. [DOI: 10.1002/icd.2178] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
19
|
Bartoli G, Bulgarelli D, Molina P. Theory of Mind Development in Children with Visual Impairment: The Contribution of the Adapted Comprehensive Test ToM Storybooks. J Autism Dev Disord 2019; 49:3494-3503. [PMID: 31119510 DOI: 10.1007/s10803-019-04064-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
Abstract
Research that focused on Theory of Mind (ToM) development in blind children showed that they were delayed, but not permanently deficient, in various types of false belief tasks. More recent studies reported first evidence of typical ToM development in blind children and suggested that more comprehensive tools to evaluate ToM had to be used. The current paper analyzed ToM development in blind children, using the adapted version of the ToM Storybooks; this is a standardized comprehensive test developed to provide a reliable and stable measurement, in comparison with the false belief tasks. Results showed that blind children's ToM performances were very similar to the ones of matched typically developing children, matched on chronological age and gender. The current finding supported the importance of the use of a more comprehensive tool to assess ToM in atypical population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gloriana Bartoli
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università degli Studi di Pavia, Pavia, Italy.,Fondazione Robert Hollman, Cannero Riviera, VB, Italy.,CAMHS: Child and Adolescent Mental Health Service, ADHB, Auckland District Health Board, and Private Practice in Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Daniela Bulgarelli
- Università degli Studi di Torino, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Turin, Italy.
| | - Paola Molina
- Università degli Studi di Torino, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Turin, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Taumoepeau M, Sadeghi S, Nobilo A. Cross-cultural differences in children’s theory of mind in Iran and New Zealand: The role of caregiver mental state talk. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.05.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
21
|
Hughes C, Devine RT, Wang Z. Does Parental Mind-Mindedness Account for Cross-Cultural Differences in Preschoolers' Theory of Mind? Child Dev 2018; 89:1296-1310. [PMID: 28160284 PMCID: PMC6849528 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12746] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
This study of 241 parent-child dyads from the United Kingdom (N = 120, Mage = 3.92, SD = 0.53) and Hong Kong (N = 121, Mage = 3.99, SD = 0.50) breaks new ground by adopting a cross-cultural approach to investigate children's theory of mind and parental mind-mindedness. Relative to the Hong Kong sample, U.K. children showed superior theory-of-mind performance and U.K. parents showed greater levels of mind-mindedness. Within both cultures parental mind-mindedness was correlated with theory of mind. Mind-mindedness also accounted for cultural differences in preschoolers' theory of mind. We argue that children's family environments might shed light on how culture shapes children's theory of mind.
Collapse
|
22
|
Hinten AE, Labuschagne LG, Boden H, Scarf D. Preschool children and young adults' preferences and expectations for helpers and hinderers. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2018. [DOI: 10.1002/icd.2093] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Ashley E. Hinten
- Department of Psychology; University of Otago; Dunedin New Zealand
| | | | - Hannah Boden
- Department of Psychology; University of Otago; Dunedin New Zealand
| | - Damian Scarf
- Department of Psychology; University of Otago; Dunedin New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Aime H, Broesch T, Aknin LB, Warneken F. Evidence for proactive and reactive helping in two- to five-year-olds from a small-scale society. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0187787. [PMID: 29141009 PMCID: PMC5687751 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187787] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2016] [Accepted: 10/25/2017] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Humans are unique in their propensity for helping. Not only do we help others in need by reacting to their requests, we also help proactively by assisting in the absence of a request. Proactive helping requires the actor to detect the need for help, recognize the intention of the other, and remedy the situation. Very little is known about the development of this social phenomenon beyond an urban, industrialized setting. We examined helping in nineteen two- to five-year old children in small-scale rural villages of Vanuatu. In the experimental condition, the intentions of the experimenter were made salient, whereas in the control condition they were ambiguous. Children helped more often in the experimental compared to the control condition, suggesting that the propensity to monitor others' goals and act accordingly can be detected in different cultural contexts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hilary Aime
- Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Tanya Broesch
- Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Lara B. Aknin
- Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Felix Warneken
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Valanides C, Sheppard E, Mitchell P. How accurately can other people infer your thoughts-And does culture matter? PLoS One 2017; 12:e0187586. [PMID: 29112972 PMCID: PMC5675417 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187586] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2016] [Accepted: 10/23/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
This research investigated how accurately people infer what others are thinking after observing a brief sample of their behaviour and whether culture/similarity is a relevant factor. Target participants (14 British and 14 Mediterraneans) were cued to think about either positive or negative events they had experienced. Subsequently, perceiver participants (16 British and 16 Mediterraneans) watched videos of the targets thinking about these things. Perceivers (both groups) were significantly accurate in judging when targets had been cued to think of something positive versus something negative, indicating notable inferential ability. Additionally, Mediterranean perceivers were better than British perceivers in making such inferences, irrespective of nationality of the targets, something that was statistically accounted for by corresponding group differences in levels of independently measured collectivism. The results point to the need for further research to investigate the possibility that being reared in a collectivist culture fosters ability in interpreting others’ behaviour.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Constantinos Valanides
- University of Nottingham, School of Psychology, University Park, Nottingham, United Kingdom
| | - Elizabeth Sheppard
- University of Nottingham, School of Psychology, University Park, Nottingham, United Kingdom
| | - Peter Mitchell
- University of Nottingham, School of Psychology, University Park, Nottingham, United Kingdom
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Dixson HGW, Komugabe-Dixson AF, Dixson BJ, Low J. Scaling Theory of Mind in a Small-Scale Society: A Case Study From Vanuatu. Child Dev 2017; 89:2157-2175. [DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12919] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
26
|
Lecce S, Caputi M, Pagnin A, Banerjee R. Theory of mind and school achievement: The mediating role of social competence. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2017. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
|
27
|
The Association between Sleep and Theory of Mind in School Aged Children with ADHD. Med Sci (Basel) 2017; 5:medsci5030018. [PMID: 29099034 PMCID: PMC5635805 DOI: 10.3390/medsci5030018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/03/2017] [Revised: 08/14/2017] [Accepted: 08/16/2017] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Theory of Mind (ToM) is defined as the ability to infer a range of internal mental states of others, including beliefs, intentions, desires, and emotions. These abilities are associated with children’s ability to socialize effectively with peers. ToM impairments are associated with peer rejection and psychiatric disorders such as Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Previous studies have found poor sleep negatively impacts executive functioning (EF) and emotional information processing, which are essential for the effective use of ToM. Youth with ADHD have EF deficits and sleep problems. However, the relationship between sleep, executive functioning, and ToM in children with ADHD has not been studied. In this review, we propose that the poor social and interpersonal skills characterizing individuals with ADHD could be explained by the impact of poor sleep on the emotional and cognitive mechanisms underlying ToM.
Collapse
|
28
|
Wang Z, Wang XC, Chui WY. Young Children's Understanding of Teaching and Learning and Their Theory of Mind Development: A Causal Analysis from a Cross-Cultural Perspective. Front Psychol 2017; 8:725. [PMID: 28559863 PMCID: PMC5432649 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00725] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2017] [Accepted: 04/21/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Children's understanding of the concepts of teaching and learning is closely associated with their theory of mind (ToM) ability and vital for school readiness. This study aimed to develop and validate a Preschool Teaching and Learning Comprehension Index (PTLCI) across cultures and examine the causal relationship between children's comprehension of teaching and learning and their mental state understanding. Two hundred and twelve children from 3 to 6 years of age from Hong Kong and the United States participated in study. The results suggested strong construct validity of the PTLCI, and its measurement and structural equivalence within and across cultures. ToM and PTLCI were significantly correlated with a medium effect size, even after controlling for age, and language ability. Hong Kong children outperformed their American counterparts in both ToM and PTLCI. Competing structural equation models suggested that children's performance on the PTLCI causally predicted their ToM across countries.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhenlin Wang
- Department of Psychology, The Education University of Hong KongTai Po, Hong Kong
| | - X Christine Wang
- Department of Learning and Instruction, University at Buffalo, SUNYBuffalo, NY, USA
| | - Wai Yip Chui
- Department of Psychology, The Education University of Hong KongTai Po, Hong Kong.,Assessment Research Centre, The Education University of Hong KongTai Po, Hong Kong
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Ellefson MR, Ng FFY, Wang Q, Hughes C. Efficiency of Executive Function: A Two-Generation Cross-Cultural Comparison of Samples From Hong Kong and the United Kingdom. Psychol Sci 2017; 28:555-566. [PMID: 28384072 DOI: 10.1177/0956797616687812] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/12/2023] Open
Abstract
Although Asian preschoolers acquire executive functions (EFs) earlier than their Western counterparts, little is known about whether this advantage persists into later childhood and adulthood. To address this gap, in the current study we gave four computerized EF tasks (providing measures of inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility, and planning) to a large sample ( n = 1,427) of 9- to 16-year-olds and their parents. All participants lived in either the United Kingdom or Hong Kong. Our findings highlight the importance of combining developmental and cultural perspectives and show both similarities and contrasts across sites. Specifically, adults' EF performance did not differ between the two sites; age-related changes in executive function for both the children and the parents appeared to be culturally invariant, as did a modest intergenerational correlation. In contrast, school-age children and young adolescents in Hong Kong outperformed their United Kingdom counterparts on all four EF tasks, a difference consistent with previous findings from preschool children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Qian Wang
- 3 Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong
| | - Claire Hughes
- 4 Centre for Family Research, University of Cambridge
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Pragmatic development explains the Theory-of-Mind Scale. Cognition 2017; 158:165-176. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2016] [Revised: 10/18/2016] [Accepted: 10/29/2016] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
31
|
Devine RT, Hughes C. Measuring theory of mind across middle childhood: Reliability and validity of the Silent Films and Strange Stories tasks. J Exp Child Psychol 2016; 149:23-40. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2015] [Revised: 07/17/2015] [Accepted: 07/19/2015] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
|
32
|
Theory of mind and executive function during middle childhood across cultures. J Exp Child Psychol 2016; 149:6-22. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 77] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2015] [Revised: 09/26/2015] [Accepted: 09/26/2015] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
33
|
|
34
|
Abstract
Theory of mind (ToM) is the child’s representational understanding of mental states (e.g., true and false beliefs) and how these influence people’s overt behavior. Past research in numerous Western and a few non-Western cultures has suggested that children throughout the world master a key milestone in ToM development, false-belief understanding, by age 5 to 6 years. However, before drawing theoretical conclusions about such apparent cross-cultural synchrony in timing, investigation of a broader range of non-Western cultures is crucial. We selected the Philippines because there has been no known previous study of ToM development in this population. A sample of 78 Filipino children aged 3 through 6 years took three standard false-belief tests and a measure of language ability in their mother tongue. The results revealed strikingly poor ToM performance. Only 12% of the full sample ( Mage = 4.95 years) passed any false-belief test at all, and only 15% of those older than 5 years ( Mage = 5.54 years; n = 39) displayed ToM by passing two out of three tests. ToM was unrelated to parents’ educational background, family size, and child language ability. Nor could methodological factors (e.g., type of false-belief test used) readily explain Filipino children’s exceptionally slow false-belief mastery. Further study is clearly needed to confirm and extend these intriguing results. Based on past evidence from other cultures, possible influences of parental conversation and socialization styles warrant further exploration in the Filipino context.
Collapse
|
35
|
Mizokawa A, Lecce S. Sensitivity to criticism and theory of mind: A cross cultural study on Japanese and Italian children. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2016. [DOI: 10.1080/17405629.2016.1180970] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
|
36
|
Abstract
In a sample of Pacific Island families living in New Zealand ( N = 45), this study tested the relation between caregivers’ strength of ethnic identity and their use of desire, cognitive, and emotion language with their toddlers during a picture description task at 15, 20, 26, 33, and 39 months. Using multi-level growth modeling, caregivers’ strength of ethnic identity predicted the change trajectories of caregivers’ mental state talk over and above the effects of education levels, and these individual estimates were predictive of their children’s performance on an emotion situation and knowledge access tasks at 39 months. We discuss the results in the light of theories regarding the role of culture and mental state language socialization of young children.
Collapse
|
37
|
Cadamuro A, Versari A, Vezzali L, Trifiletti E. Preventing the detrimental effect of posttraumatic stress in young children: The role of theory of mind in the aftermath of a natural disaster. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.1080/17405629.2015.1055240] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
|
38
|
Hughes C, Devine RT. Individual Differences in Theory of Mind From Preschool to Adolescence: Achievements and Directions. CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES 2015. [DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12124] [Citation(s) in RCA: 63] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
|
39
|
Brooks R, Meltzoff AN. Connecting the dots from infancy to childhood: a longitudinal study connecting gaze following, language, and explicit theory of mind. J Exp Child Psychol 2014; 130:67-78. [PMID: 25462032 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 120] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2014] [Revised: 09/22/2014] [Accepted: 09/23/2014] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
This longitudinal study tested the same children at three time points: infancy (10.5 months of age), toddlerhood (2.5 years of age), and early childhood (4.5 years of age). At 10.5 months, infants were assessed experimentally with a gaze-following paradigm. At 2.5 years, children's language skills were measured using the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories. At 4.5 years, children's explicit theory of mind was assessed with a standard test battery. Analyses revealed that infants with higher gaze-following scores at 10.5 months produced significantly more mental-state words at 2.5 years and that children with more mental-state words at 2.5 years were more successful on the theory-of-mind battery at 4.5 years. These predictive longitudinal relationships remained significant after controlling for general language, maternal education, and nonsocial attention. The results illuminate the bridging role that language plays in connecting infants' social cognition to children's later understanding of others' mental states. The obtained specificity in the longitudinal relations informs theories concerning mechanisms of developmental change.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rechele Brooks
- Institute of Learning & Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA; Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Science, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA.
| | - Andrew N Meltzoff
- Institute of Learning & Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Lecce S, Bianco F, Devine RT, Hughes C, Banerjee R. Promoting theory of mind during middle childhood: A training program. J Exp Child Psychol 2014; 126:52-67. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.03.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/21/2012] [Revised: 03/06/2014] [Accepted: 03/06/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
41
|
Lecce S, Bianco F, Demicheli P, Cavallini E. Training preschoolers on first-order false belief understanding: transfer on advanced ToM skills and metamemory. Child Dev 2014; 85:2404-18. [PMID: 25040788 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12267] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
Abstract
This study investigated the relation between theory of mind (ToM) and metamemory knowledge using a training methodology. Sixty-two 4- to 5-year-old children were recruited and randomly assigned to one of two training conditions: A first-order false belief (ToM) and a control condition. Intervention and control groups were equivalent at pretest for age, parents' education, verbal ability, inhibition, and ToM. Results showed that after the intervention children in the ToM group improved in their first-order false belief understanding significantly more than children in the control condition. Crucially, the positive effect of the ToM intervention was stable over 2 months and generalized to more complex ToM tasks and metamemory.
Collapse
|