1
|
Bhandari A, Keglovits H, Buyukyazgan D, Badre D. Task structure tailors the geometry of neural representations in human lateral prefrontal cortex. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2025:2024.03.06.583429. [PMID: 38496680 PMCID: PMC10942429 DOI: 10.1101/2024.03.06.583429] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/19/2024]
Abstract
How do human brains represent tasks of varying structure? The lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC) flexibly represents task information. However, principles that shape lPFC representational geometry remain unsettled. We use fMRI and pattern analyses to reveal the structure of lPFC representational geometries as humans perform two distinct categorization tasks- one with flat, conjunctive categories and another with hierarchical, context-dependent categories. We show that lPFC encodes task-relevant information with task-tailored geometries of intermediate dimensionality. These geometries preferentially enhance the separability of task-relevant variables while encoding a subset in abstract form. Specifically, in the flat task, a global axis encodes response-relevant categories abstractly, while category-specific local geometries are high-dimensional. In the hierarchy task, a global axis abstractly encodes the higher-level context, while low-dimensional, context-specific local geometries compress irrelevant information and abstractly encode the relevant information. Comparing these task geometries exposes generalizable principles by which lPFC tailors representations to different tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Apoorva Bhandari
- Department of Cognitive and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, 190 Thayer St., Providence, RI 02912, USA
| | - Haley Keglovits
- Department of Cognitive and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, 190 Thayer St., Providence, RI 02912, USA
| | - Defne Buyukyazgan
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
| | - David Badre
- Department of Cognitive and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, 190 Thayer St., Providence, RI 02912, USA
- Robert J & Nancy D Carney Institute for Brain Science, Brown University, 164 Angell St, Providence, RI 02912, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Song H, Park J, Rosenberg MD. Understanding cognitive processes across spatial scales of the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 2025; 29:282-294. [PMID: 39500686 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2024.09.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/22/2024] [Revised: 09/23/2024] [Accepted: 09/24/2024] [Indexed: 03/08/2025]
Abstract
Cognition arises from neural operations at multiple spatial scales, from individual neurons to large-scale networks. Despite extensive research on coding principles and emergent cognitive processes across brain areas, investigation across scales has been limited. Here, we propose ways to test the idea that different cognitive processes emerge from distinct information coding principles at various scales, which collectively give rise to complex behavior. This approach involves comparing brain-behavior associations and the underlying neural geometry across scales, alongside an investigation of global and local scale interactions. Bridging findings across species and techniques through open science and collaborations is essential to comprehensively understand the multiscale brain and its functions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hayoung Song
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA; Department of Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, MO, USA.
| | - JeongJun Park
- Department of Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, MO, USA.
| | - Monica D Rosenberg
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA; Neuroscience Institute, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA; Institute for Mind and Biology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Park J, Holmes CD, Snyder LH. Compositional architecture: Orthogonal neural codes for task context and spatial memory in prefrontal cortex. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2025:2025.02.25.640211. [PMID: 40060470 PMCID: PMC11888474 DOI: 10.1101/2025.02.25.640211] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/15/2025]
Abstract
The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is crucial for maintaining working memory across diverse cognitive tasks, yet how it adapts to varying task demands remains unclear. Compositional theories propose that cognitive processes in neural network rely on shared components that can be reused to support different behaviors. However, previous studies have suggested that working memory components are task specific, challenging this framework. Here, we revisit this question using a population-based approach. We recorded neural activity in macaque monkeys performing two spatial working memory tasks with opposing goals: one requiring movement toward previously presented spatial locations (look task) and the other requiring avoidance of those locations (no-look task). Despite differences in task demands, we found that spatial memory representations were largely conserved at the population level, with a common low-dimensional neural subspace encoding memory across both tasks. In parallel, task identity was encoded in an orthogonal subspace, providing a stable and independent representation of contextual information. These results provide neural evidence for a compositional model of working memory, where representational geometry enables the efficient and flexible reuse of mnemonic codes across behavioral contexts while maintaining an independent representation of context.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- JeongJun Park
- Department of Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, United States
| | - Charles D Holmes
- Department of Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, United States
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California San Diego, San Diego, CA, United States
| | - Lawrence H Snyder
- Department of Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, United States
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Kerrén C, Reznik D, Doeller CF, Griffiths BJ. Exploring the role of dimensionality transformation in episodic memory. Trends Cogn Sci 2025:S1364-6613(25)00021-X. [PMID: 39952797 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2025.01.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2024] [Revised: 01/20/2025] [Accepted: 01/20/2025] [Indexed: 02/17/2025]
Abstract
Episodic memory must accomplish two adversarial goals: encoding and storing a multitude of experiences without exceeding the finite neuronal structure of the brain, and recalling memories in vivid detail. Dimensionality reduction and expansion ('dimensionality transformation') enable the brain to meet these demands. Reduction compresses sensory input into simplified, storable codes, while expansion reconstructs vivid details. Although these processes are essential to memory, their neural mechanisms for episodic memory remain unclear. Drawing on recent insights from cognitive psychology, systems neuroscience, and neuroanatomy, we propose two accounts of how dimensionality transformation occurs in the brain: structurally (via corticohippocampal pathways) and functionally (through neural oscillations). By examining cross-species evidence, we highlight neural mechanisms that may support episodic memory and identify crucial questions for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Casper Kerrén
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.
| | - Daniel Reznik
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Christian F Doeller
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany; Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, Centre for Neural Computation, Egil and Pauline Braathen and Fred Kavli Centre for Cortical Microcircuits, Jebsen Centre for Alzheimer's Disease, NTNU Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Chiou R, Duncan J, Jefferies E, Lambon Ralph MA. The Dimensionality of Neural Coding for Cognitive Control Is Gradually Transformed within the Lateral Prefrontal Cortex. J Neurosci 2025; 45:e0233242024. [PMID: 39663116 PMCID: PMC11800757 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0233-24.2024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/03/2024] [Revised: 10/04/2024] [Accepted: 10/11/2024] [Indexed: 12/13/2024] Open
Abstract
Cognitive control relies on neural representations that are inherently high-dimensional and distributed across multiple subregions in the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Traditional approaches tackle prefrontal representation by reducing it into a unidimensional measure (univariate amplitude) or using it to distinguish a limited number of alternatives (pattern classification). In contrast, representational similarity analysis (RSA) enables flexibly formulating various hypotheses about informational contents underlying the neural codes, explicitly comparing hypotheses, and examining the representational alignment between brain regions. Here, we used a multifaceted paradigm wherein the difficulty of cognitive control was manipulated separately for five cognitive tasks. We used RSA to unveil representational contents, measure the representational alignment between regions, and quantify representational generality versus specificity. We found a graded transition in the lateral PFC: The dorsocaudal PFC was tuned to task difficulty (indexed by reaction times), preferentially connected with the parietal cortex, and representationally generalizable across domains. The ventrorostral PFC was tuned to the abstract structure of tasks, preferentially connected with the temporal cortex, and representationally specific. The middle PFC (interposed between the dorsocaudal and ventrorostral PFC) was tuned to individual task sets and ranked in the middle in terms of connectivity and generalizability. Furthermore, whether a region was dimensionally rich or sparse covaried with its functional profile: Low dimensionality (only gist) in the dorsocaudal PFC dovetailed with better generality, whereas high dimensionality (gist plus details) in the ventrorostral PFC corresponded with better ability to encode subtleties. Our findings, collectively, demonstrate how cognitive control is decomposed into distinct facets that transition steadily along prefrontal subregions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rocco Chiou
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, Cambridgeshire CB2 7EF, United Kingdom
- Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, University of Oxford, Headington, Oxfordshire OX3 9DA, United Kingdom
- School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey GU2 7XH, United Kingdom
| | - John Duncan
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, Cambridgeshire CB2 7EF, United Kingdom
| | - Elizabeth Jefferies
- Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington, Yorkshire YO10 5DD, United Kingdom
| | - Matthew A Lambon Ralph
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, Cambridgeshire CB2 7EF, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Stroud JP, Wojcik M, Jensen KT, Kusunoki M, Kadohisa M, Buckley MJ, Duncan J, Stokes MG, Lengyel M. Effects of noise and metabolic cost on cortical task representations. eLife 2025; 13:RP94961. [PMID: 39836111 PMCID: PMC11750133 DOI: 10.7554/elife.94961] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/22/2025] Open
Abstract
Cognitive flexibility requires both the encoding of task-relevant and the ignoring of task-irrelevant stimuli. While the neural coding of task-relevant stimuli is increasingly well understood, the mechanisms for ignoring task-irrelevant stimuli remain poorly understood. Here, we study how task performance and biological constraints jointly determine the coding of relevant and irrelevant stimuli in neural circuits. Using mathematical analyses and task-optimized recurrent neural networks, we show that neural circuits can exhibit a range of representational geometries depending on the strength of neural noise and metabolic cost. By comparing these results with recordings from primate prefrontal cortex (PFC) over the course of learning, we show that neural activity in PFC changes in line with a minimal representational strategy. Specifically, our analyses reveal that the suppression of dynamically irrelevant stimuli is achieved by activity-silent, sub-threshold dynamics. Our results provide a normative explanation as to why PFC implements an adaptive, minimal representational strategy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jake Patrick Stroud
- Computational and Biological Learning Lab, Department of Engineering, University of CambridgeCambridgeUnited Kingdom
| | - Michal Wojcik
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of OxfordOxfordUnited Kingdom
| | - Kristopher Torp Jensen
- Computational and Biological Learning Lab, Department of Engineering, University of CambridgeCambridgeUnited Kingdom
| | - Makoto Kusunoki
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of OxfordOxfordUnited Kingdom
| | - Mikiko Kadohisa
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of OxfordOxfordUnited Kingdom
| | - Mark J Buckley
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of OxfordOxfordUnited Kingdom
| | - John Duncan
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of CambridgeCambridgeUnited Kingdom
| | - Mark G Stokes
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of OxfordOxfordUnited Kingdom
- Oxford Centre for Human Brain Activity, Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, Department of Psychiatry, University of OxfordOxfordUnited Kingdom
| | - Mate Lengyel
- Computational and Biological Learning Lab, Department of Engineering, University of CambridgeCambridgeUnited Kingdom
- Center for Cognitive Computation, Department of Cognitive Science, Central European UniversityBudapestHungary
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Zhang J, Li H, Qu J, Liu X, Feng X, Fu X, Mei L. Language proficiency is associated with neural representational dimensionality of semantic concepts. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 2024; 258:105485. [PMID: 39388908 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2024.105485] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2024] [Revised: 09/28/2024] [Accepted: 10/04/2024] [Indexed: 10/12/2024]
Abstract
Previous studies suggest that semantic concepts are characterized by high-dimensional neural representations and that language proficiency affects semantic processing. However, it is not clear whether language proficiency modulates the dimensional representations of semantic concepts at the neural level. To address this question, the present study adopted principal component analysis (PCA) and representational similarity analysis (RSA) to examine the differences in representational dimensionalities (RDs) and in semantic representations between words in highly proficient (Chinese) and less proficient (English) language. PCA results revealed that language proficiency increased the dimensions of lexical representations in the left inferior frontal gyrus, temporal pole, inferior temporal gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus, and fusiform gyrus. RSA results further showed that these regions represented semantic information and that higher semantic representations were observed in highly proficient language relative to less proficient language. These results suggest that language proficiency is associated with the neural representational dimensionality of semantic concepts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jingxian Zhang
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents, South China Normal University, Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, 510631, Guangzhou, China; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China; School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Huiling Li
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents, South China Normal University, Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, 510631, Guangzhou, China; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China; School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Jing Qu
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents, South China Normal University, Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, 510631, Guangzhou, China; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China; School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Xiaoyu Liu
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents, South China Normal University, Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, 510631, Guangzhou, China; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China; School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Xiaoxue Feng
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents, South China Normal University, Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, 510631, Guangzhou, China; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China; School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Xin Fu
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents, South China Normal University, Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, 510631, Guangzhou, China; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China; School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Leilei Mei
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents, South China Normal University, Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, 510631, Guangzhou, China; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China; School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Woo JH, Costa VD, Taswell CA, Rothenhoefer KM, Averbeck BB, Soltani A. Contribution of amygdala to dynamic model arbitration under uncertainty. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2024:2024.09.13.612869. [PMID: 39314420 PMCID: PMC11419134 DOI: 10.1101/2024.09.13.612869] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/25/2024]
Abstract
Intrinsic uncertainty in the reward environment requires the brain to run multiple models simultaneously to predict outcomes based on preceding cues or actions, commonly referred to as stimulus- and action-based learning. Ultimately, the brain also must adopt appropriate choice behavior using reliability of these models. Here, we combined multiple experimental and computational approaches to quantify concurrent learning in monkeys performing tasks with different levels of uncertainty about the model of the environment. By comparing behavior in control monkeys and monkeys with bilateral lesions to the amygdala or ventral striatum, we found evidence for dynamic, competitive interaction between stimulus-based and action-based learning, and for a distinct role of the amygdala. Specifically, we demonstrate that the amygdala adjusts the initial balance between the two learning systems, thereby altering the interaction between arbitration and learning that shapes the time course of both learning and choice behaviors. This novel role of the amygdala can account for existing contradictory observations and provides testable predictions for future studies into circuit-level mechanisms of flexible learning and choice under uncertainty.
Collapse
|
9
|
Wards Y, Ehrhardt SE, Garner KG, Mattingley JB, Filmer HL, Dux PE. Stimulating prefrontal cortex facilitates training transfer by increasing representational overlap. Cereb Cortex 2024; 34:bhae209. [PMID: 38771242 PMCID: PMC11654026 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae209] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/17/2024] [Revised: 04/29/2024] [Accepted: 04/30/2024] [Indexed: 05/22/2024] Open
Abstract
A recent hypothesis characterizes difficulties in multitasking as being the price humans pay for our ability to generalize learning across tasks. The mitigation of these costs through training has been associated with reduced overlap of constituent task representations within frontal, parietal, and subcortical regions. Transcranial direct current stimulation, which can modulate functional brain activity, has shown promise in generalizing performance gains when combined with multitasking training. However, the relationship between combined transcranial direct current stimulation and training protocols with task-associated representational overlap in the brain remains unexplored. Here, we paired prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation with multitasking training in 178 individuals and collected functional magnetic resonance imaging data pre- and post-training. We found that 1 mA transcranial direct current stimulation applied to the prefrontal cortex paired with multitasking training enhanced training transfer to spatial attention, as assessed via a visual search task. Using machine learning to assess the overlap of neural activity related to the training task in task-relevant brain regions, we found that visual search gains were predicted by changes in classification accuracy in frontal, parietal, and cerebellar regions for participants that received left prefrontal cortex stimulation. These findings demonstrate that prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation may interact with training-related changes to task representations, facilitating the generalization of learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yohan Wards
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, McElwain
Building, Campbell Road, St Lucia, Queensland
4072, Australia
| | - Shane E Ehrhardt
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, McElwain
Building, Campbell Road, St Lucia, Queensland
4072, Australia
| | - Kelly G Garner
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, McElwain
Building, Campbell Road, St Lucia, Queensland
4072, Australia
- Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland,
Building 79, Upland Road, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales,
Mathews Building, Gate 11, Botany Street, Randwick, New South Wales
2052, Australia
- School of Psychology, University of Birmingham,
Hills Building, Edgbaston Park Rd, Birmingham B15 2TT, United Kingdom
| | - Jason B Mattingley
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, McElwain
Building, Campbell Road, St Lucia, Queensland
4072, Australia
- Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland,
Building 79, Upland Road, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
- School of Psychology, University of Birmingham,
Hills Building, Edgbaston Park Rd, Birmingham B15 2TT, United Kingdom
| | - Hannah L Filmer
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, McElwain
Building, Campbell Road, St Lucia, Queensland
4072, Australia
| | - Paul E Dux
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, McElwain
Building, Campbell Road, St Lucia, Queensland
4072, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Losey DM, Hennig JA, Oby ER, Golub MD, Sadtler PT, Quick KM, Ryu SI, Tyler-Kabara EC, Batista AP, Yu BM, Chase SM. Learning leaves a memory trace in motor cortex. Curr Biol 2024; 34:1519-1531.e4. [PMID: 38531360 PMCID: PMC11097210 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2024.03.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/21/2023] [Revised: 12/06/2023] [Accepted: 03/04/2024] [Indexed: 03/28/2024]
Abstract
How are we able to learn new behaviors without disrupting previously learned ones? To understand how the brain achieves this, we used a brain-computer interface (BCI) learning paradigm, which enables us to detect the presence of a memory of one behavior while performing another. We found that learning to use a new BCI map altered the neural activity that monkeys produced when they returned to using a familiar BCI map in a way that was specific to the learning experience. That is, learning left a "memory trace" in the primary motor cortex. This memory trace coexisted with proficient performance under the familiar map, primarily by altering neural activity in dimensions that did not impact behavior. Forming memory traces might be how the brain is able to provide for the joint learning of multiple behaviors without interference.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Darby M Losey
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Machine Learning Department, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| | - Jay A Hennig
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Machine Learning Department, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| | - Emily R Oby
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| | - Matthew D Golub
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Electrical Engineering, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA; Paul G. Allen School of Computer Science & Engineering, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA
| | - Patrick T Sadtler
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| | - Kristin M Quick
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| | - Stephen I Ryu
- Department of Electrical Engineering, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA; Department of Neurosurgery, Palo Alto Medical Foundation, Palo Alto, CA 94301, USA
| | - Elizabeth C Tyler-Kabara
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Neurological Surgery, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Neurosurgery, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
| | - Aaron P Batista
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA.
| | - Byron M Yu
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Biomedical Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA.
| | - Steven M Chase
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Biomedical Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Takahashi T, Zhang H, Agetsuma M, Nabekura J, Otomo K, Okamura Y, Nemoto T. Large-scale cranial window for in vivo mouse brain imaging utilizing fluoropolymer nanosheet and light-curable resin. Commun Biol 2024; 7:232. [PMID: 38438546 PMCID: PMC10912766 DOI: 10.1038/s42003-024-05865-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2023] [Accepted: 01/26/2024] [Indexed: 03/06/2024] Open
Abstract
Two-photon microscopy enables in vivo imaging of neuronal activity in mammalian brains at high resolution. However, two-photon imaging tools for stable, long-term, and simultaneous study of multiple brain regions in same mice are lacking. Here, we propose a method to create large cranial windows covering such as the whole parietal cortex and cerebellum in mice using fluoropolymer nanosheets covered with light-curable resin (termed the 'Nanosheet Incorporated into light-curable REsin' or NIRE method). NIRE method can produce cranial windows conforming the curved cortical and cerebellar surfaces, without motion artifacts in awake mice, and maintain transparency for >5 months. In addition, we demonstrate that NIRE method can be used for in vivo two-photon imaging of neuronal ensembles, individual neurons and subcellular structures such as dendritic spines. The NIRE method can facilitate in vivo large-scale analysis of heretofore inaccessible neural processes, such as the neuroplastic changes associated with maturation, learning and neural pathogenesis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taiga Takahashi
- Division of Biophotonics, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Higashiyama 5-1, Myodaiji, Okazaki, Aichi, 444-8787, Japan
- Biophotonics Research Group, Exploratory Research Center on Life and Living Systems (ExCELLS), National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Higashiyama 5-1, Myodaiji, Okazaki, Aichi, 444-8787, Japan
- School of Life Science, The Graduate University for Advanced Studies (SOKENDAI), Higashiyama 5-1, Myodaiji, Okazaki, Aichi, 444-8787, Japan
- Department of Medical and Robotic Engineering Design, Faculty of Advanced Engineering, Tokyo University of Science, 6-3-1 Niijuku, Katsushika, Tokyo, 125-8585, Japan
| | - Hong Zhang
- Micro/Nano Technology Center, Tokai University, 4-1-1 Kitakaname, Hiratsuka, Kanagawa, 259-1292, Japan
- School of Chemical Engineering and Technology, Tianjin University, 135 Yaguan Road, Jinnan District, Tianjin, 300350, China
| | - Masakazu Agetsuma
- Division of Homeostatic Development, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Okazaki, 444-8585, Japan
- Quantum Regenerative and Biomedical Engineering Team, Institute for Quantum Life Science, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology (QST), Anagawa 4-9-1, Chiba Inage-ku, Chiba, 263-8555, Japan
| | - Junichi Nabekura
- School of Life Science, The Graduate University for Advanced Studies (SOKENDAI), Higashiyama 5-1, Myodaiji, Okazaki, Aichi, 444-8787, Japan
- Division of Homeostatic Development, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Okazaki, 444-8585, Japan
| | - Kohei Otomo
- Division of Biophotonics, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Higashiyama 5-1, Myodaiji, Okazaki, Aichi, 444-8787, Japan
- Biophotonics Research Group, Exploratory Research Center on Life and Living Systems (ExCELLS), National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Higashiyama 5-1, Myodaiji, Okazaki, Aichi, 444-8787, Japan
- School of Life Science, The Graduate University for Advanced Studies (SOKENDAI), Higashiyama 5-1, Myodaiji, Okazaki, Aichi, 444-8787, Japan
- Department of Biochemistry and Systems Biomedicine, Graduate School of Medicine, Juntendo University, 2-1-1 Hongo, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo, 113-8421, Japan
| | - Yosuke Okamura
- Micro/Nano Technology Center, Tokai University, 4-1-1 Kitakaname, Hiratsuka, Kanagawa, 259-1292, Japan
- Department of Applied Chemistry, School of Engineering, Tokai University, 4-1-1 Kitakaname, Hiratsuka, Kanagawa, 259-1292, Japan
- Course of Applied Science, Graduate School of Engineering, Tokai University, 4-1-1 Kitakaname, Hiratsuka, Kanagawa, 259-1292, Japan
| | - Tomomi Nemoto
- Division of Biophotonics, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Higashiyama 5-1, Myodaiji, Okazaki, Aichi, 444-8787, Japan.
- Biophotonics Research Group, Exploratory Research Center on Life and Living Systems (ExCELLS), National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Higashiyama 5-1, Myodaiji, Okazaki, Aichi, 444-8787, Japan.
- School of Life Science, The Graduate University for Advanced Studies (SOKENDAI), Higashiyama 5-1, Myodaiji, Okazaki, Aichi, 444-8787, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Wards Y, Ehrhardt SE, Filmer HL, Mattingley JB, Garner KG, Dux PE. Neural substrates of individual differences in learning generalization via combined brain stimulation and multitasking training. Cereb Cortex 2023; 33:11679-11694. [PMID: 37930735 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhad406] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/22/2023] [Revised: 10/05/2023] [Accepted: 10/11/2023] [Indexed: 11/07/2023] Open
Abstract
A pervasive limitation in cognition is reflected by the performance costs we experience when attempting to undertake two tasks simultaneously. While training can overcome these multitasking costs, the more elusive objective of training interventions is to induce persistent gains that transfer across tasks. Combined brain stimulation and cognitive training protocols have been employed to improve a range of psychological processes and facilitate such transfer, with consistent gains demonstrated in multitasking and decision-making. Neural activity in frontal, parietal, and subcortical regions has been implicated in multitasking training gains, but how the brain supports training transfer is poorly understood. To investigate this, we combined transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex and multitasking training, with functional magnetic resonance imaging in 178 participants. We observed transfer to a visual search task, following 1 mA left or right prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation and multitasking training. These gains persisted for 1-month post-training. Notably, improvements in visual search performance for the right hemisphere stimulation group were associated with activity changes in the right hemisphere dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, intraparietal sulcus, and cerebellum. Thus, functional dynamics in these task-general regions determine how individuals respond to paired stimulation and training, resulting in enhanced performance on an untrained task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yohan Wards
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, McElwain Building, Campbell Road, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
| | - Shane E Ehrhardt
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, McElwain Building, Campbell Road, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
| | - Hannah L Filmer
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, McElwain Building, Campbell Road, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
| | - Jason B Mattingley
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, McElwain Building, Campbell Road, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
- Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Building 79, Upland Road, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
- Canadian Institute for Advanced Research, MaRS Centre, West tower, 661 University Ave., Suite 505, Toronto, Ontario M5G 1M1, Canada
| | - Kelly G Garner
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, McElwain Building, Campbell Road, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
- Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Building 79, Upland Road, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Mathews Building, Gate 11, Botany Street, Randwick, New South Wales 2052, Australia
- School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Hills Building, Edgbaston Park Rd, Birmingham B15 2TT, United Kingdom
| | - Paul E Dux
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, McElwain Building, Campbell Road, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Yan P, Akhoundi A, Shah NP, Tandon P, Muratore DG, Chichilnisky EJ, Murmann B. Data Compression Versus Signal Fidelity Tradeoff in Wired-OR Analog-to-Digital Compressive Arrays for Neural Recording. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL CIRCUITS AND SYSTEMS 2023; 17:754-767. [PMID: 37402181 DOI: 10.1109/tbcas.2023.3292058] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/06/2023]
Abstract
Future high-density and high channel count neural interfaces that enable simultaneous recording of tens of thousands of neurons will provide a gateway to study, restore and augment neural functions. However, building such technology within the bit-rate limit and power budget of a fully implantable device is challenging. The wired-OR compressive readout architecture addresses the data deluge challenge of a high channel count neural interface using lossy compression at the analog-to-digital interface. In this article, we assess the suitability of wired-OR for several steps that are important for neuroengineering, including spike detection, spike assignment and waveform estimation. For various wiring configurations of wired-OR and assumptions about the quality of the underlying signal, we characterize the trade-off between compression ratio and task-specific signal fidelity metrics. Using data from 18 large-scale microelectrode array recordings in macaque retina ex vivo, we find that for an event SNR of 7-10, wired-OR correctly detects and assigns at least 80% of the spikes with at least 50× compression. The wired-OR approach also robustly encodes action potential waveform information, enabling downstream processing such as cell-type classification. Finally, we show that by applying an LZ77-based lossless compressor (gzip) to the output of the wired-OR architecture, 1000× compression can be achieved over the baseline recordings.
Collapse
|
14
|
Abstract
Humans are able to rapidly perform novel tasks, but show pervasive performance costs when attempting to do two things at once. Traditionally, empirical and theoretical investigations into the sources of such multitasking interference have largely focused on multitasking in isolation to other cognitive functions, characterizing the conditions that give rise to performance decrements. Here we instead ask whether multitasking costs are linked to the system's capacity for knowledge generalization, as is required to perform novel tasks. We show how interrogation of the neurophysiological circuitry underlying these two facets of cognition yields further insights for both. Specifically, we demonstrate how a system that rapidly generalizes knowledge may induce multitasking costs owing to sharing of task contingencies between contexts in neural representations encoded in frontoparietal and striatal brain regions. We discuss neurophysiological insights suggesting that prolonged learning segregates such representations by refining the brain's model of task-relevant contingencies, thereby reducing information sharing between contexts and improving multitasking performance while reducing flexibility and generalization. These proposed neural mechanisms explain why the brain shows rapid task understanding, multitasking limitations and practice effects. In short, multitasking limits are the price we pay for behavioural flexibility.
Collapse
|
15
|
Kaufman MT, Benna MK, Rigotti M, Stefanini F, Fusi S, Churchland AK. The implications of categorical and category-free mixed selectivity on representational geometries. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2022; 77:102644. [PMID: 36332415 DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2022.102644] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2022] [Revised: 08/29/2022] [Accepted: 09/26/2022] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
The firing rates of individual neurons displaying mixed selectivity are modulated by multiple task variables. When mixed selectivity is nonlinear, it confers an advantage by generating a high-dimensional neural representation that can be flexibly decoded by linear classifiers. Although the advantages of this coding scheme are well accepted, the means of designing an experiment and analyzing the data to test for and characterize mixed selectivity remain unclear. With the growing number of large datasets collected during complex tasks, the mixed selectivity is increasingly observed and is challenging to interpret correctly. We review recent approaches for analyzing and interpreting neural datasets and clarify the theoretical implications of mixed selectivity in the variety of forms that have been reported in the literature. We also aim to provide a practical guide for determining whether a neural population has linear or nonlinear mixed selectivity and whether this mixing leads to a categorical or category-free representation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew T Kaufman
- Department of Organismal Biology and Anatomy, Neuroscience Institute, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Marcus K Benna
- Department of Neurobiology, School of Biological Sciences, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
| | | | - Fabio Stefanini
- Mortimer B. Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Department of Neuroscience, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Stefano Fusi
- Mortimer B. Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Department of Neuroscience, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA.
| | - Anne K Churchland
- David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Christensen AJ, Ott T, Kepecs A. Cognition and the single neuron: How cell types construct the dynamic computations of frontal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2022; 77:102630. [PMID: 36209695 PMCID: PMC10375540 DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2022.102630] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2022] [Revised: 08/18/2022] [Accepted: 08/23/2022] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
Frontal cortex is thought to underlie many advanced cognitive capacities, from self-control to long term planning. Reflecting these diverse demands, frontal neural activity is notoriously idiosyncratic, with tuning properties that are correlated with endless numbers of behavioral and task features. This menagerie of tuning has made it difficult to extract organizing principles that govern frontal neural activity. Here, we contrast two successful yet seemingly incompatible approaches that have begun to address this challenge. Inspired by the indecipherability of single-neuron tuning, the first approach casts frontal computations as dynamical trajectories traversed by arbitrary mixtures of neurons. The second approach, by contrast, attempts to explain the functional diversity of frontal activity with the biological diversity of cortical cell-types. Motivated by the recent discovery of functional clusters in frontal neurons, we propose a consilience between these population and cell-type-specific approaches to neural computations, advancing the conjecture that evolutionarily inherited cell-type constraints create the scaffold within which frontal population dynamics must operate.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amelia J Christensen
- Department of Neuroscience and Department of Psychiatry, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA.
| | - Torben Ott
- Department of Neuroscience and Department of Psychiatry, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA; Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience Berlin, Humboldt University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany.
| | - Adam Kepecs
- Department of Neuroscience and Department of Psychiatry, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Khoury CF, Fala NG, Runyan CA. The spatial scale of somatostatin subnetworks increases from sensory to association cortex. Cell Rep 2022; 40:111319. [PMID: 36070697 DOI: 10.1016/j.celrep.2022.111319] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2022] [Revised: 07/01/2022] [Accepted: 08/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Incoming signals interact with rich, ongoing population activity dynamics in cortical circuits. These intrinsic dynamics are the consequence of interactions among local excitatory and inhibitory neurons and affect inter-region communication and information coding. It is unclear whether specializations in the patterns of interactions among excitatory and inhibitory neurons underlie systematic differences in activity dynamics across the cortex. Here, in mice, we compare the functional interactions among somatostatin (SOM)-expressing inhibitory interneurons and the rest of the neural population in auditory cortex (AC), a sensory region of the cortex, and posterior parietal cortex (PPC), an association region. The spatial structure of shared variability among SOM and non-SOM neurons differs across regions: correlations decay rapidly with distance in AC but not in PPC. However, in both regions, activity of SOM neurons is more highly correlated than non-SOM neurons' activity. Our results imply both generalization and specialization in the functional structure of inhibitory subnetworks across the cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christine F Khoury
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Neuroscience, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA
| | - Noelle G Fala
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA
| | - Caroline A Runyan
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Neuroscience, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Pruning recurrent neural networks replicates adolescent changes in working memory and reinforcement learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2022; 119:e2121331119. [PMID: 35622896 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2121331119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022] Open
Abstract
SignificanceAdolescence is a period during which there are important changes in behavior and the structure of the brain. In this manuscript, we use theoretical modeling to show how improvements in working memory and reinforcement learning that occur during adolescence can be explained by the reduction in synaptic connectivity in prefrontal cortex that occurs during a similar period. We train recurrent neural networks to solve working memory and reinforcement learning tasks and show that when we prune connectivity in these networks, they perform the tasks better. The improvement in task performance, however, can come at the cost of flexibility as the pruned networks are not able to learn some new tasks as well.
Collapse
|
19
|
Sheng J, Zhang L, Liu C, Liu J, Feng J, Zhou Y, Hu H, Xue G. Higher-dimensional neural representations predict better episodic memory. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2022; 8:eabm3829. [PMID: 35442734 PMCID: PMC9020666 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abm3829] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/14/2021] [Accepted: 03/03/2022] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
Episodic memory enables humans to encode and later vividly retrieve information about our rich experiences, yet the neural representations that support this mental capacity are poorly understood. Using a large fMRI dataset (n = 468) of face-name associative memory tasks and principal component analysis to examine neural representational dimensionality (RD), we found that the human brain maintained a high-dimensional representation of faces through hierarchical representation within and beyond the face-selective regions. Critically, greater RD was associated with better subsequent memory performance both within and across participants, and this association was specific to episodic memory but not general cognitive abilities. Furthermore, the frontoparietal activities could suppress the shared low-dimensional fluctuations and reduce the correlations of local neural responses, resulting in greater RD. RD was not associated with the degree of item-specific pattern similarity, and it made complementary contributions to episodic memory. These results provide a mechanistic understanding of the role of RD in supporting accurate episodic memory.
Collapse
|
20
|
Cognitive strategies shift information from single neurons to populations in prefrontal cortex. Neuron 2022; 110:709-721.e4. [PMID: 34932940 PMCID: PMC8857053 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.11.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/26/2021] [Revised: 09/27/2021] [Accepted: 11/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Neurons in primate lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) play a critical role in working memory (WM) and cognitive strategies. Consistent with adaptive coding models, responses of these neurons are not fixed but flexibly adjust on the basis of cognitive demands. However, little is known about how these adjustments affect population codes. Here, we investigated ensemble coding in LPFC while monkeys implemented different strategies in a WM task. Although single neurons were less tuned when monkeys used more stereotyped strategies, task information could still be accurately decoded from neural populations. This was due to changes in population codes that distributed information among a greater number of neurons, each contributing less to the overall population. Moreover, this shift occurred for task-relevant, but not irrelevant, information. These results demonstrate that cognitive strategies that impose structure on information held in mind rearrange population codes in LPFC, such that information becomes more distributed among neurons in an ensemble.
Collapse
|
21
|
Differential coding of goals and actions in ventral and dorsal corticostriatal circuits during goal-directed behavior. Cell Rep 2022; 38:110198. [PMID: 34986350 PMCID: PMC9608360 DOI: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.110198] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2021] [Revised: 07/08/2021] [Accepted: 12/10/2021] [Indexed: 02/04/2023] Open
Abstract
Goal-directed behavior requires identifying objects in the environment that can satisfy internal needs and executing actions to obtain those objects. The current study examines ventral and dorsal corticostriatal circuits that support complementary aspects of goal-directed behavior. We analyze activity from the amygdala, ventral striatum, orbitofrontal cortex, and lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) while monkeys perform a three-armed bandit task. Information about chosen stimuli and their value is primarily encoded in the amygdala, ventral striatum, and orbitofrontal cortex, while the spatial information is primarily encoded in the LPFC. Before the options are presented, information about the to-be-chosen stimulus is represented in the amygdala, ventral striatum, and orbitofrontal cortex; at the time of choice, the information is passed to the LPFC to direct a saccade. Thus, learned value information specifying behavioral goals is maintained throughout the ventral corticostriatal circuit, and it is routed through the dorsal circuit at the time actions are selected.
Collapse
|
22
|
Averbeck B, O'Doherty JP. Reinforcement-learning in fronto-striatal circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology 2022; 47:147-162. [PMID: 34354249 PMCID: PMC8616931 DOI: 10.1038/s41386-021-01108-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 60] [Impact Index Per Article: 20.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2021] [Revised: 07/06/2021] [Accepted: 07/09/2021] [Indexed: 01/03/2023]
Abstract
We review the current state of knowledge on the computational and neural mechanisms of reinforcement-learning with a particular focus on fronto-striatal circuits. We divide the literature in this area into five broad research themes: the target of the learning-whether it be learning about the value of stimuli or about the value of actions; the nature and complexity of the algorithm used to drive the learning and inference process; how learned values get converted into choices and associated actions; the nature of state representations, and of other cognitive machinery that support the implementation of various reinforcement-learning operations. An emerging fifth area focuses on how the brain allocates or arbitrates control over different reinforcement-learning sub-systems or "experts". We will outline what is known about the role of the prefrontal cortex and striatum in implementing each of these functions. We then conclude by arguing that it will be necessary to build bridges from algorithmic level descriptions of computational reinforcement-learning to implementational level models to better understand how reinforcement-learning emerges from multiple distributed neural networks in the brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - John P O'Doherty
- Division of Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Hennig JA, Oby ER, Losey DM, Batista AP, Yu BM, Chase SM. How learning unfolds in the brain: toward an optimization view. Neuron 2021; 109:3720-3735. [PMID: 34648749 PMCID: PMC8639641 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.09.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2021] [Revised: 08/25/2021] [Accepted: 09/02/2021] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
How do changes in the brain lead to learning? To answer this question, consider an artificial neural network (ANN), where learning proceeds by optimizing a given objective or cost function. This "optimization framework" may provide new insights into how the brain learns, as many idiosyncratic features of neural activity can be recapitulated by an ANN trained to perform the same task. Nevertheless, there are key features of how neural population activity changes throughout learning that cannot be readily explained in terms of optimization and are not typically features of ANNs. Here we detail three of these features: (1) the inflexibility of neural variability throughout learning, (2) the use of multiple learning processes even during simple tasks, and (3) the presence of large task-nonspecific activity changes. We propose that understanding the role of these features in the brain will be key to describing biological learning using an optimization framework.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jay A Hennig
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Machine Learning Department, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA.
| | - Emily R Oby
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - Darby M Losey
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Machine Learning Department, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - Aaron P Batista
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - Byron M Yu
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Department of Biomedical Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - Steven M Chase
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Department of Biomedical Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Ebitz RB, Hayden BY. The population doctrine in cognitive neuroscience. Neuron 2021; 109:3055-3068. [PMID: 34416170 PMCID: PMC8725976 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.07.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 103] [Impact Index Per Article: 25.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2021] [Revised: 07/02/2021] [Accepted: 07/13/2021] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
A major shift is happening within neurophysiology: a population doctrine is drawing level with the single-neuron doctrine that has long dominated the field. Population-level ideas have so far had their greatest impact in motor neuroscience, but they hold great promise for resolving open questions in cognition as well. Here, we codify the population doctrine and survey recent work that leverages this view to specifically probe cognition. Our discussion is organized around five core concepts that provide a foundation for population-level thinking: (1) state spaces, (2) manifolds, (3) coding dimensions, (4) subspaces, and (5) dynamics. The work we review illustrates the progress and promise that population-level thinking holds for cognitive neuroscience-for delivering new insight into attention, working memory, decision-making, executive function, learning, and reward processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R Becket Ebitz
- Department of Neurosciences, Faculté de médecine, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada.
| | - Benjamin Y Hayden
- Department of Neuroscience, Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, and Center for Neuroengineering, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Dekker MM, França ASC, Panja D, Cohen MX. Characterizing neural phase-space trajectories via Principal Louvain Clustering. J Neurosci Methods 2021; 362:109313. [PMID: 34384798 DOI: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2021.109313] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/08/2021] [Revised: 07/29/2021] [Accepted: 08/04/2021] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND With the growing size and richness of neuroscience datasets in terms of dimension, volume, and resolution, identifying spatiotemporal patterns in those datasets is increasingly important. Multivariate dimension-reduction methods are particularly adept at addressing these challenges. NEW METHOD In this paper, we propose a novel method, which we refer to as Principal Louvain Clustering (PLC), to identify clusters in a low-dimensional data subspace, based on time-varying trajectories of spectral dynamics across multisite local field potential (LFP) recordings in awake behaving mice. Data were recorded from prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and parietal cortex in eleven mice while they explored novel and familiar environments. RESULTS PLC-identified subspaces and clusters showed high consistency across animals, and were modulated by the animals' ongoing behavior. CONCLUSIONS PLC adds to an important growing literature on methods for characterizing dynamics in high-dimensional datasets, using a smaller number of parameters. The method is also applicable to other kinds of datasets, such as EEG or MEG.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark M Dekker
- Department of Information and Computing Sciences, Utrecht University, Princetonplein 5, 3584 CC Utrecht, The Netherlands; Centre for Complex Systems Studies, Utrecht University, Minnaertgebouw, Leuvenlaan 4, 3584 CE Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | - Arthur S C França
- Radboud University Medical Center, Donders Centre for Medical Neuroscience, The Netherlands
| | - Debabrata Panja
- Department of Information and Computing Sciences, Utrecht University, Princetonplein 5, 3584 CC Utrecht, The Netherlands; Centre for Complex Systems Studies, Utrecht University, Minnaertgebouw, Leuvenlaan 4, 3584 CE Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Michael X Cohen
- Radboud University Medical Center, Donders Centre for Medical Neuroscience, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Alizadeh M, Manmatharayan AR, Johnston T, Thalheimer S, Finley M, Detloff M, Sharan A, Harrop J, Newburg A, Krisa L, Mohamed FB. Graph theoretical structural connectome analysis of the brain in patients with chronic spinal cord injury: preliminary investigation. Spinal Cord Ser Cases 2021; 7:60. [PMID: 34274953 PMCID: PMC8286254 DOI: 10.1038/s41394-021-00424-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/23/2020] [Revised: 06/25/2021] [Accepted: 06/28/2021] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
STUDY DESIGN Retrospective study. OBJECTIVES We aimed to characterize the convergent disruptions of the structural connectivity based on network modeling technique (i.e., graph theory) to identify significant changes in network organization/reorganization between uninjured and chronic spinal cord injury (SCI) participants. SETTING USA. METHODS Ten adult participants including 4 with chronic SCI and 6 uninjured were scanned using a multi-shell diffusion imaging on a 3.0 T MR scanner. Whole brain structural connectivity matrix was estimated by performing the quantification of the number of white matter fibers (called edges) connecting each possible pair of brain region (called nodes). Brain regions were defined according to Desikan-Killiany cortical atlas. Using connectivity matrix, connectivity strength as well as six different graph theoretical measurements were computed for each participant. They include: (1) global efficiency; (2) local efficiency; (3) degree; (4) betweenness centrality; (5) average shortest length and (6) clustering coefficient. Finally network based statistics was applied to extract nodes/connections with significant differences between groups (uninjured vs SCI). RESULTS The SCI group showed significant decreases in betweenness centrality in the left precentral gyrus (T-score=2.98, p value=0.02), and the right caudal middle frontal gyrus (score = 2.35, p value=0.047). It also showed significant decrease in left transverse temporal gyrus (T-score=2.36, p value=0.046) in clustering coefficient. In addition, altered regions in the occipital and parietal lobe were also identified. CONCLUSION These results suggest that not only local but also global alterations of the white matter occur after SCI. The proposed modeling technique has the potential to serve as a screening tool to identify any areas of the brain affected after SCI.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mahdi Alizadeh
- Jefferson Integrated Magnetic Resonance Imaging Center, Department of Radiology, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
- Department of Neurosurgery, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
| | | | - Therese Johnston
- Department of Physical Therapy, Jefferson College of Rehabilitation Sciences, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Sara Thalheimer
- Jefferson Integrated Magnetic Resonance Imaging Center, Department of Radiology, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Margaret Finley
- Department of Physical Therapy & Rehabilitation Science, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Megan Detloff
- Department of Neurobiology & Anatomy, Marion Murray Spinal Cord Research Center, College of Medicine, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Ashwini Sharan
- Jefferson Integrated Magnetic Resonance Imaging Center, Department of Radiology, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - James Harrop
- Jefferson Integrated Magnetic Resonance Imaging Center, Department of Radiology, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Andrew Newburg
- Marcus Institute of Integrative Health-Myrna Brind Center, Marcus Institute, Thomas Jefferson University, Villanova, PA, USA
| | - Laura Krisa
- Department of Physical Therapy, Jefferson College of Rehabilitation Sciences, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Feroze B Mohamed
- Jefferson Integrated Magnetic Resonance Imaging Center, Department of Radiology, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Over-representation of fundamental decision variables in the prefrontal cortex underlies decision bias. Neurosci Res 2021; 173:1-13. [PMID: 34274406 DOI: 10.1016/j.neures.2021.07.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2021] [Revised: 06/15/2021] [Accepted: 07/13/2021] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
The brain is organized into anatomically distinct structures consisting of a variety of projection neurons. While such evolutionarily conserved neural circuit organization underlies the innate ability of animals to swiftly adapt to environments, they can cause biased cognition and behavior. Although recent studies have begun to address the causal importance of projection-neuron types as distinct computational units, it remains unclear how projection types are functionally organized in encoding variables during cognitive tasks. This review focuses on the neural computation of decision making in the prefrontal cortex and discusses what decision variables are encoded by single neurons, neuronal populations, and projection type, alongside how specific projection types constrain decision making. We focus particularly on "over-representations" of distinct decision variables in the prefrontal cortex that reflect the biological and subjective significance of the variables for the decision makers. We suggest that task-specific over-representation in the prefrontal cortex involves the refinement of the given decision making, while generalized over-representation of fundamental decision variables is associated with suboptimal decision biases, including pathological ones such as those in patients with psychiatric disorders. Such over-representation of the fundamental decision variables in the prefrontal cortex appear to be tightly constrained by afferent and efferent connections that can be optogenetically intervened on. These ideas may provide critical insights into potential therapeutic targets for psychiatric disorders, including addiction and depression.
Collapse
|
28
|
Pérez-Prieto N, Delgado-Restituto M. Recording Strategies for High Channel Count, Densely Spaced Microelectrode Arrays. Front Neurosci 2021; 15:681085. [PMID: 34326718 PMCID: PMC8313871 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2021.681085] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2021] [Accepted: 06/18/2021] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Neuroscience research into how complex brain functions are implemented at an extra-cellular level requires in vivo neural recording interfaces, including microelectrodes and read-out circuitry, with increased observability and spatial resolution. The trend in neural recording interfaces toward employing high-channel-count probes or 2D microelectrodes arrays with densely spaced recording sites for recording large neuronal populations makes it harder to save on resources. The low-noise, low-power requirement specifications of the analog front-end usually requires large silicon occupation, making the problem even more challenging. One common approach to alleviating this consumption area burden relies on time-division multiplexing techniques in which read-out electronics are shared, either partially or totally, between channels while preserving the spatial and temporal resolution of the recordings. In this approach, shared elements have to operate over a shorter time slot per channel and active area is thus traded off against larger operating frequencies and signal bandwidths. As a result, power consumption is only mildly affected, although other performance metrics such as in-band noise or crosstalk may be degraded, particularly if the whole read-out circuit is multiplexed at the analog front-end input. In this article, we review the different implementation alternatives reported for time-division multiplexing neural recording systems, analyze their advantages and drawbacks, and suggest strategies for improving performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Norberto Pérez-Prieto
- Institute of Microelectronics of Seville (IMSE-Centro Nacional de Microelectrónica), Spanish National Research Council, Seville, Spain
| | - Manuel Delgado-Restituto
- Institute of Microelectronics of Seville (IMSE-Centro Nacional de Microelectrónica), Spanish National Research Council, Seville, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Badre D, Bhandari A, Keglovits H, Kikumoto A. The dimensionality of neural representations for control. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2021; 38:20-28. [PMID: 32864401 PMCID: PMC7451207 DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.07.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 17.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
Cognitive control allows us to think and behave flexibly based on our context and goals. At the heart of theories of cognitive control is a control representation that enables the same input to produce different outputs contingent on contextual factors. In this review, we focus on an important property of the control representation's neural code: its representational dimensionality. Dimensionality of a neural representation balances a basic separability/generalizability trade-off in neural computation. We will discuss the implications of this trade-off for cognitive control. We will then briefly review current neuroscience findings regarding the dimensionality of control representations in the brain, particularly the prefrontal cortex. We conclude by highlighting open questions and crucial directions for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Badre
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Carney Institute for Brain Science, Brown University
| | - Apoorva Bhandari
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Carney Institute for Brain Science, Brown University
| | - Haley Keglovits
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Carney Institute for Brain Science, Brown University
| | - Atsushi Kikumoto
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Carney Institute for Brain Science, Brown University
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Reward-related choices determine information timing and flow across macaque lateral prefrontal cortex. Nat Commun 2021; 12:894. [PMID: 33563989 PMCID: PMC7873307 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-021-20943-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2020] [Accepted: 12/16/2020] [Indexed: 01/25/2023] Open
Abstract
Prefrontal cortex is critical for cognition. Although much is known about the representation of cognitive variables in the prefrontal cortex, much less is known about the spatio-temporal neural dynamics that underlie cognitive operations. In the present study, we examined information timing and flow across the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC), while monkeys carried out a two-armed bandit reinforcement learning task in which they had to learn to select rewarding actions or rewarding objects. When we analyzed signals independently within subregions of the LPFC, we found a task-specific, caudo-rostral gradient in the strength and timing of signals related to chosen objects and chosen actions. In addition, when we characterized information flow among subregions, we found that information flow from action to object representations was stronger from the dorsal to ventral LPFC, and information flow from object to action representations was stronger from the ventral to dorsal LPFC. The object to action effects were more pronounced in object blocks, and also reflected learning specifically in these blocks. These results suggest anatomical segregation followed by the rapid integration of information within the LPFC. Previous studies provided conflicting evidence on the functional organization of the lateral prefrontal cortex. The authors show task-specific information flows along the caudo-rostral and dorso-ventral axes, reflecting the cognitive process of identifying the location or identity of a valuable object.
Collapse
|
31
|
Divergent Strategies for Learning in Males and Females. Curr Biol 2021; 31:39-50.e4. [PMID: 33125868 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.09.075] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2020] [Revised: 07/08/2020] [Accepted: 09/24/2020] [Indexed: 02/08/2023]
Abstract
A frequent assumption in value-based decision-making tasks is that agents make decisions based on the feature dimension that reward probabilities vary on. However, in complex, multidimensional environments, stimuli can vary on multiple dimensions at once, meaning that the feature deserving the most credit for outcomes is not always obvious. As a result, individuals may vary in the strategies used to sample stimuli across dimensions, and these strategies may have an unrecognized influence on decision-making. Sex is a proxy for multiple genetic and endocrine influences on behavior, including how environments are sampled. In this study, we examined the strategies adopted by female and male mice as they learned the value of stimuli that varied in both image and location in a visually cued two-armed bandit, allowing two possible dimensions to learn about. Female mice acquired the correct image-value associations more quickly than male mice, preferring a fundamentally different strategy. Female mice were more likely to constrain their decision-space early in learning by preferentially sampling one location over which images varied. Conversely, male mice were more likely to be inconsistent, changing their choice frequently and responding to the immediate experience of stochastic rewards. Individual strategies were related to sex-biased changes in neuronal activation in early learning. Together, we find that in mice, sex is associated with divergent strategies for sampling and learning about the world, revealing substantial unrecognized variability in the approaches implemented during value-based decision making.
Collapse
|
32
|
Correlates of Auditory Decision-Making in Prefrontal, Auditory, and Basal Lateral Amygdala Cortical Areas. J Neurosci 2020; 41:1301-1316. [PMID: 33303679 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2217-20.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/20/2020] [Revised: 11/02/2020] [Accepted: 11/26/2020] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Spatial selective listening and auditory choice underlie important processes including attending to a speaker at a cocktail party and knowing how (or whether) to respond. To examine task encoding and the relative timing of potential neural substrates underlying these behaviors, we developed a spatial selective detection paradigm for monkeys, and recorded activity in primary auditory cortex (AC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), and the basolateral amygdala (BLA). A comparison of neural responses among these three areas showed that, as expected, AC encoded the side of the cue and target characteristics before dlPFC and BLA. Interestingly, AC also encoded the choice of the monkey before dlPFC and around the time of BLA. Generally, BLA showed weak responses to all task features except the choice. Decoding analyses suggested that errors followed from a failure to encode the target stimulus in both AC and dlPFC, but again, these differences arose earlier in AC. The similarities between AC and dlPFC responses were abolished during passive sensory stimulation with identical trial conditions, suggesting that the robust sensory encoding in dlPFC is contextually gated. Thus, counter to a strictly PFC-driven decision process, in this spatial selective listening task AC neural activity represents the sensory and decision information before dlPFC. Unlike in the visual domain, in this auditory task, the BLA does not appear to be robustly involved in selective spatial processing.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT We examined neural correlates of an auditory spatial selective listening task by recording single-neuron activity in behaving monkeys from the amygdala, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and auditory cortex. We found that auditory cortex coded spatial cues and choice-related activity before dorsolateral prefrontal cortex or the amygdala. Auditory cortex also had robust delay period activity. Therefore, we found that auditory cortex could support the neural computations that underlie the behavioral processes in the task.
Collapse
|
33
|
Averbeck BB, Murray EA. Hypothalamic Interactions with Large-Scale Neural Circuits Underlying Reinforcement Learning and Motivated Behavior. Trends Neurosci 2020; 43:681-694. [PMID: 32762959 PMCID: PMC7483858 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.06.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2020] [Revised: 06/02/2020] [Accepted: 06/19/2020] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
Abstract
Biological agents adapt behavior to support the survival needs of the individual and the species. In this review we outline the anatomical, physiological, and computational processes that support reinforcement learning (RL). We describe two circuits in the primate brain that are linked to specific aspects of learning and goal-directed behavior. The ventral circuit, that includes the amygdala, ventral medial prefrontal cortex, and ventral striatum, has substantial connectivity with the hypothalamus. The dorsal circuit, that includes inferior parietal cortex, dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex, and the dorsal striatum, has minimal connectivity with the hypothalamus. The hypothalamic connectivity suggests distinct roles for these circuits. We propose that the ventral circuit defines behavioral goals, and the dorsal circuit orchestrates behavior to achieve those goals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bruno B Averbeck
- Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD 20892-4415, USA.
| | - Elisabeth A Murray
- Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD 20892-4415, USA
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Kyriazi P, Headley DB, Paré D. Different Multidimensional Representations across the Amygdalo-Prefrontal Network during an Approach-Avoidance Task. Neuron 2020; 107:717-730.e5. [PMID: 32562662 PMCID: PMC7442738 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.05.039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2020] [Revised: 04/21/2020] [Accepted: 05/28/2020] [Indexed: 01/07/2023]
Abstract
The prelimbic (PL) area and basolateral amygdala (lateral [LA] and basolateral [BL] nuclei) have closely related functions and similar extrinsic connectivity. Reasoning that the computational advantage of such redundancy should be reflected in differences in how these structures represent information, we compared the coding properties of PL and amygdala neurons during a task that requires rats to produce different conditioned defensive or appetitive behaviors. Rather than unambiguous regional differences in the identities of the variables encoded, we found gradients in how the same variables are represented. Whereas PL and BL neurons represented many different parameters through minor variations in firing rates, LA cells coded fewer task features with stronger changes in activity. At the population level, whereas valence could be easily distinguished from amygdala activity, PL neurons could distinguish both valence and trial identity as well as or better than amygdala neurons. Thus, PL has greater representational capacity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pinelopi Kyriazi
- Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers State University, Newark, NJ 07102, USA; Behavioral and Neural Sciences Graduate Program, Rutgers State University, Newark, NJ 07102, USA
| | - Drew B Headley
- Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers State University, Newark, NJ 07102, USA.
| | - Denis Paré
- Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers State University, Newark, NJ 07102, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex Codes Information Relevant for Managing Explore-Exploit Tradeoffs. J Neurosci 2020; 40:2553-2561. [PMID: 32060169 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2355-19.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2019] [Revised: 01/26/2020] [Accepted: 02/09/2020] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Reinforcement learning (RL) refers to the behavioral process of learning to obtain reward and avoid punishment. An important component of RL is managing explore-exploit tradeoffs, which refers to the problem of choosing between exploiting options with known values and exploring unfamiliar options. We examined correlates of this tradeoff, as well as other RL related variables, in orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) while three male monkeys performed a three-armed bandit learning task. During the task, novel choice options periodically replaced familiar options. The values of the novel options were unknown, and the monkeys had to explore them to see if they were better than other currently available options. The identity of the chosen stimulus and the reward outcome were strongly encoded in the responses of single OFC neurons. These two variables define the states and state transitions in our model that are relevant to decision-making. The chosen value of the option and the relative value of exploring that option were encoded at intermediate levels. We also found that OFC value coding was stimulus specific, as opposed to coding value independent of the identity of the option. The location of the option and the value of the current environment were encoded at low levels. Therefore, we found encoding of the variables relevant to learning and managing explore-exploit tradeoffs in OFC. These results are consistent with findings in the ventral striatum and amygdala and show that this monosynaptically connected network plays an important role in learning based on the immediate and future consequences of choices.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) has been implicated in representing the expected values of choices. Here we extend these results and show that OFC also encodes information relevant to managing explore-exploit tradeoffs. Specifically, OFC encodes an exploration bonus, which characterizes the relative value of exploring novel choice options. OFC also strongly encodes the identity of the chosen stimulus, and reward outcomes, which are necessary for computing the value of novel and familiar options.
Collapse
|