1
|
Duchesne L, Gingras MP, Gagnon M. Communicative participation of school-aged children with cochlear implants: parental perceptions. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2025:enaf021. [PMID: 40304708 DOI: 10.1093/jdsade/enaf021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/19/2024] [Revised: 03/07/2025] [Accepted: 03/31/2025] [Indexed: 05/02/2025]
Abstract
This qualitative study aimed to explore parents' perceptions regarding the communication of their school-aged child with cochlear implants (CIs) in various social contexts. To this end, the construct of "communicative participation" (World Health Organization (2001), International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health, https://www.who.int/standards/classifications/international-classification-of-functioning-disability-and-health) was broadly applied to the population of children with CIs. Nineteen parents participated in semi-structured interviews and described their perceptions and experiences regarding the communication of their child in social contexts. Data were analyzed using a thematic analysis approach. The results showed that overall, despite describing very positive outcomes with CIs, parents reported that their children do experience participation restrictions and activity limitations because of communication difficulties and/or environmental and social barriers. The findings from this study unveiled the communication difficulties of school-aged children with CIs, as experienced by their parents. These difficulties-which may seem subtle-can significantly impact the participation in communication. Language interventions could be improved to better support pupils who are experienced CI users.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Louise Duchesne
- Département d'orthophonie, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières, Trois-Rivières, Canada
| | - Marie-Pier Gingras
- Département d'orthophonie, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières, Trois-Rivières, Canada
| | - Maude Gagnon
- Département d'orthophonie, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières, Trois-Rivières, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Obuchi C, Kawase T, Sasame Y, Yamamoto Y, Sasaki K, Iwasaki J, Okamoto H, Kaga K. Traits of Developmental Disorders in Adults With Listening Difficulties Without Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder And/or Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Clin Med 2024; 13:6281. [PMID: 39458230 PMCID: PMC11508553 DOI: 10.3390/jcm13206281] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2024] [Revised: 10/17/2024] [Accepted: 10/18/2024] [Indexed: 10/28/2024] Open
Abstract
Background: Some individuals have a normal audiogram but have listening difficulties (LiD). As many studies have investigated the relationship between listening and developmental disorders, the traits of developmental disorders might explain the symptoms of LiD. In this study, we examined the traits of developmental disorders of adults with LiD to help clarify the cause of LiD symptoms. Methods: In total, 60 adults with LiD and 57 adults without LiD were included. Participants completed a questionnaire for the autism spectrum quotient (AQ) test, the Adult Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Self-Rating Scale (A-ADHD), the Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile (SP), and the severity of subjective LiD in daily life. Results: Before analysis, we excluded participants with LiD who were already diagnosed or met the criteria for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or ADHD, and the results of the remaining 30 participants (50.0%) with LiD were analyzed. Adults with LiD showed higher scores than those without LiD in the AQ. Attention switching in the AQ and attention ability in the A-ADHD scale were correlated with the severity of LiD symptoms in everyday life. The AQ scores were also significantly correlated with subscales of the SP. Conclusions: Adults with LiD showed greater autistic traits than those without LiD; therefore, LiD symptoms are possibly related to autistic symptoms. Furthermore, adults with LiD might have attention disorder traits of both ASD and ADHD and sensory processing problems. These findings suggest that the attention problems in adults with LiD noted in previous studies might be related to these traits of developmental disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chie Obuchi
- Institute of Human Sciences, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba 305-8572, Japan
| | | | - Yuka Sasame
- Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, International University of Health and Welfare, Chiba 286-8686, Japan; (Y.S.); (Y.Y.); (K.S.); (J.I.)
| | - Yayoi Yamamoto
- Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, International University of Health and Welfare, Chiba 286-8686, Japan; (Y.S.); (Y.Y.); (K.S.); (J.I.)
| | - Kaori Sasaki
- Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, International University of Health and Welfare, Chiba 286-8686, Japan; (Y.S.); (Y.Y.); (K.S.); (J.I.)
| | - Junya Iwasaki
- Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, International University of Health and Welfare, Chiba 286-8686, Japan; (Y.S.); (Y.Y.); (K.S.); (J.I.)
| | - Hidehiko Okamoto
- Department of Physiology, International University of Health and Welfare, Chiba 286-8686, Japan;
| | - Kimitaka Kaga
- National Hospital Organization Tokyo Medical Center, National Institute of Sensory Organs, Tokyo 152-8902, Japan;
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Bongioletti J, Doble M, Purcell A. Conversation and pragmatics in children who are hard-of-hearing: a scoping review. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2024; 29:456-466. [PMID: 38757199 PMCID: PMC11413802 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/enae011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2023] [Revised: 02/05/2024] [Accepted: 02/21/2024] [Indexed: 05/18/2024]
Abstract
Technological and therapeutic advances have allowed many children who are born hard-of-hearing (HoH) to start school with age-appropriate spoken language skills, yet many of these children continue to find everyday conversations challenging. This scoping review maps the evidence related to development of conversation and pragmatic skills in children who are HoH and learning spoken language. The review followed Arksey and O'Malley's methodological framework and the PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews guidelines. Quality appraisal, data extraction, and thematic analysis were used to describe the data. Systematic searches identified 36 articles for inclusion. Sample sizes were small and heterogenous. Most studies focused on school-aged children with severe hearing loss or greater. Methodological rigor varied. Thematic analysis revealed two global themes. First, children who are HoH continue to find conversation and pragmatics difficult to master, and second, there are a set of audiological, communication, environmental, and demographic characteristics that are associated with better conversation and pragmatic outcomes, some of which are fixed, whereas others are malleable. Focused attention on designing valid and reliable assessments for conversation and pragmatic skills, and on developing therapeutic approaches targeting early conversation and pragmatic skill development, is needed to reduce the impact conversation and pragmatic differences across the lifespan.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jenna Bongioletti
- Faculty of Medicine and Health, Discipline of Speech Pathology, The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia
| | - Maree Doble
- Faculty of Medicine and Health, Discipline of Speech Pathology, The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia
| | - Alison Purcell
- School of Health Sciences, Speech Pathology, Western Sydney University, Campbelltown, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Nikrah P, Ghareh Chahie R, Ghazvini A, Hajizadeh A. Evaluating the effect of cochlear implantation age on pragmatic abilities before and after age of 3. APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY. CHILD 2024:1-7. [PMID: 39269920 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2024.2403100] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/15/2024]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION The cochlear implant (CI) is crucial in developing hearing, speech, language, and communication skills in children with profound hearing loss (HL). The study aimed to assess how the age at which children receive a CI affects the development of pragmatic abilities between the ages of 5 and 8 for those who received a CI before or after the age of 3. METHODS Forty children with CI were evaluated. The children between the ages of 5 and 8 were divided into two groups who received CIs before or after age 3. The Persian version of the Children's Communication Checklist (CCC) was used to assess pragmatic abilities with a parent rating scale. RESULTS Children implanted under 3 got higher scores in social relationships, using context, conversational rapport, syntax, and speech. Also, their performance in inappropriate and stereotyped conversations was better than those over 3 years. They have a significant difference in the pragmatic composite than those who received CI over 3 except for coherence and interest (p < 0.05). CONCLUSION According to the results, children with CI under 3 performed better in pragmatic composites, which means they have better pragmatic abilities. These findings suggest that early CI is important. Of course, other individual and environmental factors must also be considered.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pegah Nikrah
- Communicative Disorders, University of Louisiana at Lafayette, Lafayette, USA
| | - Rasool Ghareh Chahie
- Electrical & Computer Engineering, University of Louisiana at Lafayette, Lafayette, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Sadati S, Mohamadi R. Communication breakdown repair skills: Comparison between deaf and hard-of-hearing, and normal-hearing adolescents. Cochlear Implants Int 2024; 25:99-108. [PMID: 39028756 DOI: 10.1080/14670100.2024.2379122] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/21/2024]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Communication breakdowns and their repair by deaf and hard-of-hearing (DHH) and hearing adolescents were examined in conversation with an unfamiliar communication partner. METHOD This study compared the number and type of clarification requests and responses to those requests of 16 DHH and 16 normal-hearing adolescents aged 11-16 years, in a 10-minute conversation with an unfamiliar adult. It also analyzed the relationship between speech intelligibility, communication breakdowns, and clarification requests by an unfamiliar adult. the Children's Communication Checklist (CCC) was completed by parents. RESULTS DHH adolescents demonstrated significantly higher usage of nonverbal clarification requests and verbal and nonverbal responses to clarification requests compared to normal-hearing adolescents in conversations with an unfamiliar adult. Furthermore, the subscale scores of the CCC and the speech intelligibility of DHH adolescents were significantly lower than those of normal-hearing adolescents. There were correlations between speech intelligibility and the speech subscale score of the CCC, as well as correlations between the pragmatic composite score of the CCC, the number of communication breakdowns, and the number of clarification requests by an unfamiliar adult. DISCUSSION The adolescents with DHH experienced more communication breakdowns in conversation with an unfamiliar adult and the number of clarification requests made by adults was higher.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sahar Sadati
- Rehabilitation Research Centre, Department of Speech Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Science, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Reyhane Mohamadi
- Rehabilitation Research Centre, Department of Speech Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Science, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Kawar K, Walters J, Fichman S. Evaluation devices in the narratives of deaf/hard of hearing and hearing Arabic-speaking adolescents. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2024; 59:180-194. [PMID: 37501380 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12938] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/18/2022] [Accepted: 07/11/2023] [Indexed: 07/29/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Pragmatic language use in general and the use of evaluation devices (EDs) are very important components in everyday communication and expressing thoughts and feelings in narrative production. However, very little is known about the use of evaluative devices in the narratives of Arabic-speaking adolescents, especially those who are deaf and hard of hearing (DHH). AIMS The present study investigated the use of EDs in personal oral narratives elicited from 124 hearing and DHH Arabic-speaking adolescents. METHODS & PROCEDURES Each participant was asked to tell a personal story about a time he or she was in a dangerous situation. Narratives were transcribed and coded for nine different types of evaluation. OUTCOMES & RESULTS A three-way interaction of group (hearing/DHH) by gender (Female/Male) by evaluation type emerged such that female hearing adolescents' narratives contained significantly more frames of mind than the narratives of DHH females and hearing males and more hedges than DHH females. In addition, hearing males' narratives contained more hedges than those of DHH males and more negative comments than the narratives of both hearing females and DHH males. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS The present research identifies factors that facilitate narrative abilities by DHH. Discussion centres on explanations for group and gender differences in terms of exposure and language socialization. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS What is already known on this subject Although narratives have been researched extensively in comparisons of hearing and deaf and hard of hearing (DHH) children, only a few studies have examined pragmatic language use in general and evaluation devices in particular. These studies show that the narratives of DHH children and adolescents contain fewer figurative expressions, frames of mind and hedges and that children who are DHH have difficulty comprehending and producing metaphors. Very little is known about the use of evaluative devices in the narratives of Arabic-speaking adolescents, especially those who are DHH. What this study adds The present study found that hearing participants produced significantly more evaluation devices (Eds) than their DHH peers, and female participants produced significantly more EDs than their male peers. Moreover, hearing females' narratives contained more frames of mind and more hedges than did the narratives of DHH females; also, hearing females' narratives contained more frames of mind than those of hearing males. In addition, hearing males' narratives contained more hedges than those of DHH males. Finally, hearing males' narratives had more negative comments than both the narratives of hearing females and those of DHH males. Clinical implications The one evaluation device produced in large numbers (hedges) by adolescents who are DHH was expressed in only one of two ways. One important clinical implication of this finding is that pragmatic language in general and use of EDs in particular should be part of any speech and language evaluation. Another takeaway is that intervention should attempt to provide rich exposure to social communicative language, providing many ways to convey each function, enabling adolescents to communicate in different contexts.
Collapse
|
7
|
Tuohimaa K, Loukusa S, Löppönen H, Välimaa T, Kunnari S. Development of Social-Pragmatic Understanding in Children With Congenital Hearing Loss and Typical Hearing Between the Ages of 4 and 6 Years. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2023:1-18. [PMID: 37227801 DOI: 10.1044/2023_jslhr-22-00700] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/27/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE This prospective longitudinal study aimed to explore (a) the development of social-pragmatic understanding of children with bilateral hearing aids (BiHAs), bilateral cochlear implants (BiCIs), and typical hearing (TH) between the ages of 4 and 6 years and (b) group differences between children with BiHAs, BiCIs, and TH. METHOD The Pragma test was used for a comprehensive assessment of social-pragmatic understanding of a total of 86 children: 19 children with BiHAs, 22 children with BiCIs, and 45 children with TH. The Pragma test requires answering socially and contextually demanding questions and explaining the right answers. The explanation tasks are targeted at studying the participant's own awareness of the inferencing process. The children in this study were assessed yearly at the ages of 4, 5, and 6 years. RESULTS The participants with BiHAs, BiCIs, and TH showed significant development in their social-pragmatic understanding between the ages of 4 and 6 years, but most children with hearing loss (HL) still did not meet age expectations at the age of 6 years. Children with BiHAs and BiCIs both showed large-scale inferential difficulties, including utilizing theory of mind, utilizing verbal and visual information, and understanding conversational norms and emotions in context. CONCLUSIONS Children with BiHAs and BiCIs are at risk of delays in social-pragmatic understanding despite early detection of HL, early amplification, and cochlear implantation. Therefore, the social-pragmatic abilities of children with HL should be assessed regularly, and the children with HL should have early access to social-pragmatic interventions where utilizing contextual information is practiced comprehensively.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Krista Tuohimaa
- Research Unit of Logopedics, University of Oulu, Finland
- Child Language Research Center, University of Oulu, Finland
| | - Soile Loukusa
- Research Unit of Logopedics, University of Oulu, Finland
- Child Language Research Center, University of Oulu, Finland
| | - Heikki Löppönen
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Eastern Finland, Kuopio
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery, Kuopio University Hospital, Finland
| | - Taina Välimaa
- Research Unit of Logopedics, University of Oulu, Finland
- Child Language Research Center, University of Oulu, Finland
| | - Sari Kunnari
- Research Unit of Logopedics, University of Oulu, Finland
- Child Language Research Center, University of Oulu, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Skrbic R, Bugarski-Ignjatovic V, Komazec Z, Veselinovic M. Verbal, Figural, and Arithmetic Fluency of Children with Cochlear Implants. Behav Sci (Basel) 2023; 13:bs13050349. [PMID: 37232588 DOI: 10.3390/bs13050349] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2023] [Revised: 04/18/2023] [Accepted: 04/19/2023] [Indexed: 05/27/2023] Open
Abstract
Cochlear implantation gives children with prelingual severe hearing loss and deafness the opportunity to develop their hearing abilities, speech, language, cognitive abilities and academic skills with adequate rehabilitation. The aim of the research was to analyze verbal, figural and arithmetic fluency and their interrelationship in children with a cochlear implant (CI) and children with normal hearing (NH). A total of 46 children with CI and 110 children with NH, aged 9 to 16, participated in the research. Verbal fluency was assessed using phonemic and semantic fluency, and non-verbal fluency using figural fluency. Arithmetic fluency was assessed using simple arithmetic tasks within the number range up to 100. The results showed that children with CI achieved poorer results in phonemic fluency (z = -4.92; p < 0.001), semantic fluency (z = -3.89; p < 0.001), figural fluency (z = -3.07; p = 0.002), and arithmetic fluency (z = -4.27; p < 0.001). In both groups, a positive correlation was obtained between the measured modalities and types of fluency. In the group of children with CI, a sex difference was obtained on the phonemic fluency test, in favor of girls. The age of children with CI was correlated with arithmetic fluency. Verbal, figural and arithmetic fluency of children with CI speak in favor of the importance of early auditory and language experiences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Renata Skrbic
- Faculty of Medicine, University of Novi Sad, 21 137 Novi Sad, Serbia
| | - Vojislava Bugarski-Ignjatovic
- Faculty of Medicine, University of Novi Sad, 21 137 Novi Sad, Serbia
- Clinic for Neurology, University Clinical Center of Vojvodina, 21 137 Novi Sad, Serbia
| | - Zoran Komazec
- Faculty of Medicine, University of Novi Sad, 21 137 Novi Sad, Serbia
- Clinic for Otorhinolaryngology and Head and Neck Surgery, University Clinical Center of Vojvodina, 21 137 Novi Sad, Serbia
| | - Mila Veselinovic
- Faculty of Medicine, University of Novi Sad, 21 137 Novi Sad, Serbia
- Clinic for Otorhinolaryngology and Head and Neck Surgery, University Clinical Center of Vojvodina, 21 137 Novi Sad, Serbia
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Cardillo R, Crisci G, Seregni S, Mammarella IC. Social perception in children and adolescents with ADHD: The role of higher-order cognitive skills. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2023; 135:104440. [PMID: 36764097 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2023.104440] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/19/2022] [Revised: 01/13/2023] [Accepted: 01/23/2023] [Indexed: 06/18/2023]
Abstract
Despite children with ADHD frequently experiencing difficulties in social perception, the mechanisms underlying this impairment have been poorly explored. In this study, we examined social perception in children with ADHD, comparing them with typically-developing (TD) children on semi-naturalistic tasks, and considering the effect of nonverbal signal recognition. Our aim was to ascertain whether the two groups' social perception related to different types of stimulus (video, audio or combined/multimodal). The role of three higher-order cognitive skills (theory of mind, attention and pragmatic language) was also investigated. Thirty-six children with ADHD, and 36 TD controls were tested. Social perception was significantly associated with participants' ability to recognize nonverbal signals, and with the stimulus presentation modality. Children with ADHD only performed less well than TD children with combined stimuli. As concerns the higher-order cognitive skills, theory of mind had a significant role in both groups, but only with the video and combined stimuli, while attention explained most of the variance in social perception for all types of stimulus. Better pragmatic language skills were only associated with a better social perception in TD children, whatever the type of stimulus presented. Semi-naturalistic tasks should be included when assessing social perception in ADHD, and both theory of mind and attention should be the object of efforts to enhance social perception in the ADHD population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ramona Cardillo
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35131 Padova, Italy
| | - Giulia Crisci
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35131 Padova, Italy.
| | - Stefano Seregni
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35131 Padova, Italy
| | - Irene C Mammarella
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35131 Padova, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Socher M, Löfkvist U, Wass M. Comparing the semantic networks of children with cochlear implants and children with typical hearing: Effects of length of language access. JOURNAL OF COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2022; 99:106247. [PMID: 35843069 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2022.106247] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/29/2021] [Revised: 07/06/2022] [Accepted: 07/06/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Kenett et al. (2013) report that the sematic networks, measured by using an oral semantic fluency task, of children with cochlear implants (CI) are less structured compared to the sematic networks of children with typical hearing (TH). This study aims to evaluate if such differences are only evident if children with CI are compared to children with TH matched on chronological age, or also if they are compared to children with TH matched on hearing age. METHOD The performance of a group of children with CI on a verbal fluency task was compared to the performance of a group of chronological-age matched children with TH. Subsequently, computational network analysis was used to compare the semantic network structure of the groups. The same procedure was applied to compare a group of children with CI to a group of hearing-age matched children with TH. RESULTS The children with CI perform on the same level on an oral semantic verbal fluency task as the children with TH matched on hearing age. There are significant differences in terms of the structure of the semantic network between the groups. The magnitude of these differences is very small and they are non-significant for a proportion of nodes included in the bootstrap analysis. This indicates that there is no true difference between the networks. Hearing age, but not age at implantation was found to be significantly positively correlated with semantic verbal fluency performance for the children with CI. CONCLUSIONS The results from the current study indicate that length of exposure to the tested language is an important factor for the structure of the semantic network and the performance on a semantic verbal fluency task for children with CI. Further studies are needed to explore the role of the accessibility of the language input for the development of semantic networks of children with CI.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michaela Socher
- Department of Behavioural Sciences and Learning, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden; Linnaeus Centre HEAD, Swedish Institute for Disability Research, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden; Fraunhofer Institute for Building Physics IBP, Institute Stuttgart, Stuttgart, Germany.
| | - Ulrika Löfkvist
- Department of Public Health and Caring Sciences, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden; Department of Clinical Science, Intervention and Technology, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Malin Wass
- Department of Health, Learning and Technology, Luleå University of Technology, Luleå, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Tuohimaa K, Loukusa S, Löppönen H, Välimaa T, Kunnari S. Communication abilities in children with hearing loss - views of parents and daycare professionals. JOURNAL OF COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2022; 99:106256. [PMID: 36029613 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2022.106256] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2021] [Revised: 08/12/2022] [Accepted: 08/18/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Today, children with hearing loss (HL) are diagnosed and fitted with hearing devices at an early age. However, HL may still influence their communicative development. Thus, we need up-to-date research on how children perform in their everyday environments, such as at home or daycare. This study provides an overview of the communication abilities of early-diagnosed children with HL reported by parents and daycare professionals. The first aim of the study is to compare the results of children with bilateral hearing aids (BiHAs) or bilateral cochlear implants (BiCIs) with those of children with normal hearing (NH) and with each other. The second aim of the study is to compare the views of the two respondents, parents and the daycare professionals. In addition, the effects of gender and nonverbal intelligence quotient (IQ) on the responses are explored. METHODS The participants, aged 4;0-6;9, were 25 children with BiHAs, 29 children with BiCIs, and 64 children with NH. The Finnish version of the Children's Communication Checklist-2 (CCC-2) was used to assess the communication skills of the participants. RESULTS Group and nonverbal IQ had a significant effect on the General Communication Composite (GCC) score. Both groups of children with HL had poorer GCC scores than the children with NH, apart from the respondent. The BiHA-group had significantly lower scores than the NH-group on Speech, Syntax, Semantics, and Coherence subscales. The BiCI-group had significantly lower scores than the NH-group across all subscales of the CCC-2. The parents rated the participants significantly higher than the daycare professionals in Speech and Social Relations. In contrast, the daycare professionals rated the participants higher than the parents in Coherence, Inappropriate Initiation, Stereotyped Language, and Use of Context. Furthermore, gender influenced Coherence, Nonverbal Communication, Social Relations, and Interests, for which the girls performed better than the boys. The nonverbal IQ had an effect on Syntax, Semantics, and Use of Context, for which higher nonverbal IQ was associated with better performance. CONCLUSIONS On average the children with HL had poorer communication skills than the children with NH. Pragmatic difficulties were more common in the BiCI-group than in the BiHA-group. The respondents were not completely unanimous, which may be because of the different demands of different environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Krista Tuohimaa
- Research Unit of Logopedics, University of Oulu, Finland; Child Language Research Center, University of Oulu, Finland.
| | - Soile Loukusa
- Research Unit of Logopedics, University of Oulu, Finland; Child Language Research Center, University of Oulu, Finland
| | - Heikki Löppönen
- Institute of Clinical Medicine, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University of Eastern Finland, Finland; Department of Otorhinolaryngology - Head and Neck Surgery, Kuopio University Hospital, Finland
| | - Taina Välimaa
- Research Unit of Logopedics, University of Oulu, Finland; Child Language Research Center, University of Oulu, Finland
| | - Sari Kunnari
- Research Unit of Logopedics, University of Oulu, Finland; Child Language Research Center, University of Oulu, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Rezaei M, Rashedi V, Borhaninejad V, Nurian ZS. Pragmatic Skills in Children with Hearing Loss: Comparison Between Cochlear Implants and Hearing Aids Users. Indian J Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2022; 74:434-438. [PMID: 36213476 PMCID: PMC9535044 DOI: 10.1007/s12070-021-02529-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2020] [Accepted: 03/22/2021] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Pragmatic language ability denotes the ability to use language in a social context. There is a lack of research that has compared children's pragmatic skills with hearing loss with those of hearing peers. This study questioned whether children with a cochlear implant would show better pragmatic skills than children with hearing aids. 52 children were included in three groups: cochlear implant (n = 16), hearing aid (n = 16), and normal hearing (n = 20). The participants' pragmatic skills were evaluated using the Persian version of the children's communication checklist. Of the 52 participants recruited, 22 (42.3%) were males, and 30 (57.7%) were females. The mean age of the CI, HA, and NH group participants was 75.19 ± 10.80, 72.19 ± 8.68, and 68.90 ± 6.78 months, respectively (P > 0.05). There was a significant difference between the mean scores of Speech Output and Syntax between the groups (CI, HA and NH) (P < 0.001). The hearing-impaired children show acceptable pragmatic skills in comparison with NH children. Specialists, such as teachers and clinicians, should be alert of the abilities and difficulties that the hearing-impaired children might be facing in the regular classroom.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mohammad Rezaei
- Autism Spectrum Disorder Research Center, Hamadan University of Medical Sciences, Hamadan, Iran
| | - Vahid Rashedi
- School of Behavioral Sciences and Mental Health (Tehran Institute of Psychiatry), Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Vahidreza Borhaninejad
- Social Determinants of Health Research Center, Institute for Futures Studies in Health, Kerman University of Medical Sciences, Kerman, Iran
| | - Zohreh Sadat Nurian
- Student Research Committee, Hamadan University of Medical Sciences, Hamadan, Iran
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Hilviu D, Parola A, Vivaldo S, Di Lisi D, Consolino P, Bosco F. Children with hearing impairment and early cochlear implant: A pragmatic assessment. Heliyon 2021; 7:e07428. [PMID: 34286120 PMCID: PMC8273221 DOI: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2021.e07428] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2021] [Revised: 05/27/2021] [Accepted: 06/24/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Extensive research has demonstrated the benefits of cochlear implants (CI) in contributing to improve the linguistic skills of children with hearing impairment; however, few studies have focused on the development of pragmatic ability and its relationship with age of implantation. Pragmatics is the ability to use language in different contexts and its development has crucial implications, e.g., social inclusion and professional attainments. In this study, we conducted a comprehensive assessment of pragmatic ability using the Language Pragmatic Abilities (APL Medea), a battery composed by five different tasks: Comprehension of Metaphors, Implicit meaning, Comics, Situations and Colors Game (a perspective taking task). Eighteen children with early CI, belonging to 3 different age groups (6; 11-7; 11, 8; 0-8; 11 and 9; 0-9; 11 years-old), and twenty-four children with typical development (Control Group) participated to the study. We also investigated how the precocity of CI, i.e., age of first implantation, may affect the pragmatic development. Globally, children with CI obtained lower scores in the APL Medea battery than typically hearing children. However, focusing on the Medea tasks separately, children with CIs differed from their hearing peers only in Comics and Colors Game tasks. Finally, age of implantation was a moderate but significant predictor of pragmatic performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D. Hilviu
- University of Turin, Department of Psychology, Group on Inferential Processes in Social Interaction (GIPSI), Turin, Italy
| | - A. Parola
- University of Turin, Department of Psychology, Group on Inferential Processes in Social Interaction (GIPSI), Turin, Italy
- Aarhus University, Department of Linguistics, Semiotics and Cognitive Science, Aarhus, Denmark
| | - S. Vivaldo
- Martini Hospital, ENT Department, Turin, Italy
| | - D. Di Lisi
- Martini Hospital, ENT Department, Turin, Italy
| | | | - F.M. Bosco
- University of Turin, Department of Psychology, Group on Inferential Processes in Social Interaction (GIPSI), Turin, Italy
- University of Turin, Neuroscience Institute of Turin, Turin, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Crowe K, Dammeyer J. A Review of the Conversational Pragmatic Skills of Children With Cochlear Implants. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2021; 26:171-186. [PMID: 33740059 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/enab001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2020] [Revised: 12/03/2020] [Accepted: 01/11/2021] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
Many children who use cochlear implants (CI) have strong skills in many aspects of spoken language; however, limited information is available about their mastery of the pragmatic skills required to participate in conversation. This study reviewed published literature describing the pragmatic skills of children who use CIs in conversational contexts. Twenty-five studies met the inclusion criteria, and data were extracted describing participant characteristics, methodology, data type, outcomes, and factors associated with outcomes. Pragmatic skills were described in three broad categories: speech acts, turns, and breakdowns and repairs. Participants showed heterogeneity in age, age at implantation, duration of implant use, and languages used. Studies employed a variety of methodologies, used a range of different sample types and coding strategies, and considered different factors associated that might be associated with children's pragmatic skills. Across studies, children with CIs were reported to have a range of pragmatic skills in conversational contexts, from few to severe difficulties. The body of literature on this topic is small and considered heterogeneous children with CIs with a wide range of skills. Further research is needed to understand the pragmatics language skills of children with CIs and the factors influencing the diversity in skills observed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathryn Crowe
- Schools of Health Sciences and Education, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
- School of Teacher Education, Charles Sturt University, Bathurst, Australia
| | - Jesper Dammeyer
- Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Socher M, Ellis RJ, Wass M, Lyxell B. Comparison of Expressive Spoken Language Skills in Children With Cochlear Implants and Children With Typical Hearing. Front Psychol 2020; 11:1405. [PMID: 32765338 PMCID: PMC7378954 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01405] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2020] [Accepted: 05/26/2020] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
When children start formal education, they are expected to be able to express complex thoughts. However, in order to do so, they need to be able to use both complex grammatical structures and a variety of words. One group that is at risk of having a delay in terms of their expressive language ability is children with cochlear implants (CI). In this study, we evaluated whether children with CI perform comparably to children with typical hearing (TH) on a standard expressive spoken grammar and a standard expressive spoken vocabulary task when the groups were matched on non-verbal intelligence and working memory capacity. It was found that the children with CI in this study performed more poorly on a standard expressive spoken vocabulary task but not on a standard expressive spoken grammar task when compared to the children with TH. Differences in terms of expressive spoken vocabulary do not seem to be explained by differences in cognitive ability. In addition, the variation in terms of expressive spoken language ability was larger in the children with CI compared to the children with TH. This might be explained by additional confounding factors, like the time of language deprivation or by a greater influence of cognitive differences for the acquisition of spoken language for children with CI.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michaela Socher
- Swedish Institute of Disability Research, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
| | - Rachel Jane Ellis
- Swedish Institute of Disability Research, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
| | - Malin Wass
- Special Needs Education, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| | - Björn Lyxell
- Swedish Institute of Disability Research, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
- Special Needs Education, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| |
Collapse
|