51
|
Nokes-Malach TJ, Fraundorf SH, Caddick ZA, Rottman BM. Cognitive perspectives on maintaining physicians' medical expertise: V. Using a motivational framework to understand the benefits and costs of testing. Cogn Res Princ Implic 2023; 8:64. [PMID: 37817025 PMCID: PMC10564705 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-023-00518-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2022] [Accepted: 09/27/2023] [Indexed: 10/12/2023] Open
Abstract
We apply a motivational perspective to understand the implications of physicians' longitudinal assessment. We review the literature on situated expectancy-value theory, achievement goals, mindsets, anxiety, and stereotype threat in relation to testing and assessment. This review suggests several motivational benefits of testing as well as some potential challenges and costs posed by high-stakes, standardized tests. Many of the motivational benefits for testing can be understood from the equation of having the perceived benefits of the test outweigh the perceived costs of preparing for and taking the assessment. Attention to instructional framing, test purposes and values, and longitudinal assessment frameworks provide vehicles to further enhance motivational benefits and reduce potential costs of assessment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Timothy J Nokes-Malach
- Learning Research and Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, 3420 Forbes Ave., Pittsburgh, PA, 15260, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, 3420 Forbes Ave., Pittsburgh, PA, 15260, USA
| | - Scott H Fraundorf
- Learning Research and Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, 3420 Forbes Ave., Pittsburgh, PA, 15260, USA.
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, 3420 Forbes Ave., Pittsburgh, PA, 15260, USA.
| | - Zachary A Caddick
- Learning Research and Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, 3420 Forbes Ave., Pittsburgh, PA, 15260, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, 3420 Forbes Ave., Pittsburgh, PA, 15260, USA
| | - Benjamin M Rottman
- Learning Research and Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, 3420 Forbes Ave., Pittsburgh, PA, 15260, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, 3420 Forbes Ave., Pittsburgh, PA, 15260, USA
| |
Collapse
|
52
|
Parenteau AM, Boyer CJ, Campos LJ, Carranza AF, Deer LK, Hartman DT, Bidwell JT, Hostinar CE. A review of mental health disparities during COVID-19: Evidence, mechanisms, and policy recommendations for promoting societal resilience. Dev Psychopathol 2023; 35:1821-1842. [PMID: 36097815 PMCID: PMC10008755 DOI: 10.1017/s0954579422000499] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/27/2023]
Abstract
Social and economic inequality are chronic stressors that continually erode the mental and physical health of marginalized groups, undermining overall societal resilience. In this comprehensive review, we synthesize evidence of greater increases in mental health symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic among socially or economically marginalized groups in the United States, including (a) people who are low income or experiencing homelessness, (b) racial and ethnic minorities, (c) women and lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, queer, and questioning (LGBTQ+) communities, (d) immigrants and migrants, (e) children and people with a history of childhood adversity, and (f) the socially isolated and lonely. Based on this evidence, we propose that reducing social and economic inequality would promote population mental health and societal resilience to future crises. Specifically, we propose concrete, actionable recommendations for policy, intervention, and practice that would bolster five "pillars" of societal resilience: (1) economic safety and equity, (2) accessible healthcare, including mental health services, (3) combating racial injustice and promoting respect for diversity, equity, and inclusion, (4) child and family protection services, and (5) social cohesion. Although the recent pandemic exposed and accentuated steep inequalities within our society, efforts to rebuild offer the opportunity to re-envision societal resilience and policy to reduce multiple forms of inequality for our collective benefit.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna M. Parenteau
- Psychology Department, University of California-Davis
- Center for Poverty and Inequality Research, University of California-Davis
| | - Chase J. Boyer
- Department of Human Ecology, University of California-Davis
| | | | | | - LillyBelle K. Deer
- Psychology Department, University of California-Davis
- Center for Poverty and Inequality Research, University of California-Davis
| | | | - Julie T. Bidwell
- Betty Irene Moore School of Nursing, University of California-Davis
| | - Camelia E. Hostinar
- Psychology Department, University of California-Davis
- Center for Poverty and Inequality Research, University of California-Davis
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Zola A, Engeln R. Brains over beauty: A preregistered test of the effects of objectification on women's cognitive performance. PLoS One 2023; 18:e0291853. [PMID: 37733711 PMCID: PMC10513279 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0291853] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2021] [Accepted: 09/07/2023] [Indexed: 09/23/2023] Open
Abstract
The present study was a preregistered, well-powered experimental test of findings related to the effect of state self-objectification and anticipation of the sexualized male gaze on women's cognitive performance. College women (n = 407) performed a working memory task in one of three randomly assigned conditions. In the experimental conditions (self-objectification and male gaze), women completed the task while being video recorded from the neck down. In the male gaze condition, participants were told their videos would later be evaluated by men as part of a separate dating study. Women in the control condition were not video recorded. Results indicated women experienced a moderate increase in state self-objectification in both experimental conditions. However, compared to the control condition, women in the experimental conditions did not show reduced performance on the working memory task (in either latency or accuracy), decreases in body satisfaction, or increases in negative mood. Across conditions, state self-objectification was not associated with accuracy or latency on the working memory task. Mixed findings concerning objectification's effect on cognitive performance may be attributed to variability in experimental manipulations and dependent variables employed in this area of research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anne Zola
- Department of Medical Social Sciences, Psychometrics and Statistics Core, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Renee Engeln
- Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Widge AS, Jordan A, Kraguljac NV, Sullivan CRP, Wilson S, Benton TD, Alpert JE, Carpenter LL, Krystal JH, Nemeroff CB, Dzirasa K. Structural Racism in Psychiatric Research Careers: Eradicating Barriers to a More Diverse Workforce. Am J Psychiatry 2023; 180:645-659. [PMID: 37073513 PMCID: PMC11227892 DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp.20220685] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/20/2023]
Abstract
Investigators from minoritized backgrounds are underrepresented in psychiatric research. That underrepresentation contributes to disparities in outcomes of access to mental health care. Drawing on lived experience, scholarly qualitative reports, and empirical data, the authors review how the underrepresentation of minoritized researchers arises from interlocking, self-reinforcing effects of structural biases in our research training and funding institutions. Minoritized researchers experience diminished early access to advanced training and opportunities, stereotype threats and microaggressions, isolation due to lack of peers and senior mentors, decreased access to early funding, and unique community and personal financial pressures. These represent structural racism-a system of institutional assumptions and practices that perpetuates race-based disparities, in spite of those institutions' efforts to increase diversity and in contradiction to the values that academic leaders outwardly espouse. The authors further review potential approaches to reversing these structural biases, including undergraduate-focused research experiences, financial support for faculty who lead training/mentoring programs, targeted mentoring through scholarly societies, better use of federal diversity supplement funding, support for scientific reentry, cohort building, diversity efforts targeting senior leadership, and rigorous examination of hiring, compensation, and promotion practices. Several of these approaches have empirically proven best practices and models for dissemination. If implemented alongside outcome measurement, they have the potential to reverse decades of structural bias in psychiatry and psychiatric research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alik S Widge
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (Widge, Sullivan, Wilson); Department of Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York (Jordan); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham (Kraguljac); Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia (Benton); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Montefiore Medical Center and Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York (Alpert); Butler Hospital and Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, R.I. (Carpenter); Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn. (Krystal); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin (Nemeroff); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, N.C., and Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Chevy Chase, Md. (Dzirasa)
| | - Ayana Jordan
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (Widge, Sullivan, Wilson); Department of Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York (Jordan); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham (Kraguljac); Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia (Benton); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Montefiore Medical Center and Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York (Alpert); Butler Hospital and Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, R.I. (Carpenter); Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn. (Krystal); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin (Nemeroff); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, N.C., and Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Chevy Chase, Md. (Dzirasa)
| | - Nina V Kraguljac
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (Widge, Sullivan, Wilson); Department of Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York (Jordan); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham (Kraguljac); Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia (Benton); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Montefiore Medical Center and Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York (Alpert); Butler Hospital and Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, R.I. (Carpenter); Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn. (Krystal); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin (Nemeroff); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, N.C., and Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Chevy Chase, Md. (Dzirasa)
| | - Christi R P Sullivan
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (Widge, Sullivan, Wilson); Department of Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York (Jordan); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham (Kraguljac); Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia (Benton); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Montefiore Medical Center and Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York (Alpert); Butler Hospital and Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, R.I. (Carpenter); Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn. (Krystal); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin (Nemeroff); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, N.C., and Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Chevy Chase, Md. (Dzirasa)
| | - Saydra Wilson
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (Widge, Sullivan, Wilson); Department of Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York (Jordan); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham (Kraguljac); Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia (Benton); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Montefiore Medical Center and Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York (Alpert); Butler Hospital and Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, R.I. (Carpenter); Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn. (Krystal); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin (Nemeroff); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, N.C., and Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Chevy Chase, Md. (Dzirasa)
| | - Tami D Benton
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (Widge, Sullivan, Wilson); Department of Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York (Jordan); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham (Kraguljac); Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia (Benton); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Montefiore Medical Center and Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York (Alpert); Butler Hospital and Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, R.I. (Carpenter); Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn. (Krystal); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin (Nemeroff); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, N.C., and Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Chevy Chase, Md. (Dzirasa)
| | - Jonathan E Alpert
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (Widge, Sullivan, Wilson); Department of Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York (Jordan); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham (Kraguljac); Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia (Benton); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Montefiore Medical Center and Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York (Alpert); Butler Hospital and Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, R.I. (Carpenter); Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn. (Krystal); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin (Nemeroff); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, N.C., and Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Chevy Chase, Md. (Dzirasa)
| | - Linda L Carpenter
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (Widge, Sullivan, Wilson); Department of Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York (Jordan); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham (Kraguljac); Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia (Benton); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Montefiore Medical Center and Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York (Alpert); Butler Hospital and Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, R.I. (Carpenter); Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn. (Krystal); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin (Nemeroff); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, N.C., and Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Chevy Chase, Md. (Dzirasa)
| | - John H Krystal
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (Widge, Sullivan, Wilson); Department of Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York (Jordan); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham (Kraguljac); Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia (Benton); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Montefiore Medical Center and Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York (Alpert); Butler Hospital and Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, R.I. (Carpenter); Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn. (Krystal); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin (Nemeroff); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, N.C., and Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Chevy Chase, Md. (Dzirasa)
| | - Charles B Nemeroff
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (Widge, Sullivan, Wilson); Department of Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York (Jordan); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham (Kraguljac); Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia (Benton); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Montefiore Medical Center and Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York (Alpert); Butler Hospital and Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, R.I. (Carpenter); Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn. (Krystal); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin (Nemeroff); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, N.C., and Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Chevy Chase, Md. (Dzirasa)
| | - Kafui Dzirasa
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (Widge, Sullivan, Wilson); Department of Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York (Jordan); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham (Kraguljac); Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia (Benton); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Montefiore Medical Center and Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York (Alpert); Butler Hospital and Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, R.I. (Carpenter); Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn. (Krystal); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin (Nemeroff); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, N.C., and Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Chevy Chase, Md. (Dzirasa)
| |
Collapse
|
55
|
Obenauf C, Mekawi Y, Lathan EC, Hinojosa CA, Thomas JG, Stevens JS, Powers A, Michopoulos V, Carter S. Indirect effect of race-related stress on traumatic stress and depression symptoms via subjective social status in a Black community sample. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF COMMUNITY PSYCHOLOGY 2023; 72:116-126. [PMID: 37434412 DOI: 10.1002/ajcp.12693] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2022] [Revised: 05/25/2023] [Accepted: 06/19/2023] [Indexed: 07/13/2023]
Abstract
Experiencing racism is linked to lower subjective social status (SSS), defined as one's perception of their position in society. SSS is influenced by power, prestige, and objective socioeconomic status (SES). Previous findings suggest that race-related stress may be related to adverse mental health outcomes through SSS in Black Americans, a population that has been deeply affected by continuing legacies of oppression. The current study examines the indirect association between race-related stress and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and depression symptoms through SSS in a community sample of largely trauma-exposed Black Americans (N = 173). Hierarchical regression analyses indicated that overall race-related stress significantly predicted lower SSS, higher PTSD symptoms, and higher depression symptoms. Analyses also revealed indirect effects of cultural race-related stress on PTSD and depression symptoms through SSS after controlling for SES. Results suggest that the experience of race-related stress, particularly cultural race-related stress, which involves the degradation and disparagement of one's culture and worldview, is associated with more severe PTSD and depression symptoms potentially due to these experiences decreasing Black Americans' SSS. Findings support the need for systemic intervention strategies to disrupt the cultural oppression of Black Americans and improve the societal value and mental health of this population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caterina Obenauf
- Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee, USA
| | - Yara Mekawi
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky, USA
| | - Emma C Lathan
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, Georgia, USA
| | - Cecilia A Hinojosa
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, Georgia, USA
| | - Joel G Thomas
- Department of Psychology, Agnes Scott College, Decatur, Georgia, USA
| | - Jennifer S Stevens
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, Georgia, USA
| | - Abigail Powers
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, Georgia, USA
| | - Vasiliki Michopoulos
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, Georgia, USA
- Emory National Primate Research Center, Atlanta, Georgia, USA
| | - Sierra Carter
- Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, Georgia, USA
| |
Collapse
|
56
|
MICHAELS ELIK, LAM‐HINE TRACY, NGUYEN THUT, GEE GILBERTC, ALLEN AMANIM. The Water Surrounding the Iceberg: Cultural Racism and Health Inequities. Milbank Q 2023; 101:768-814. [PMID: 37435779 PMCID: PMC10509530 DOI: 10.1111/1468-0009.12662] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2022] [Revised: 04/03/2023] [Accepted: 05/08/2023] [Indexed: 07/13/2023] Open
Abstract
Policy Points Cultural racism-or the widespread values that privilege and protect Whiteness and White social and economic power-permeates all levels of society, uplifts other dimensions of racism, and contributes to health inequities. Overt forms of racism, such as racial hate crimes, represent only the "tip of the iceberg," whereas structural and institutional racism represent its base. This paper advances cultural racism as the "water surrounding the iceberg," allowing it to float while obscuring its base. Considering the fundamental role of cultural racism is needed to advance health equity. CONTEXT Cultural racism is a pervasive social toxin that surrounds all other dimensions of racism to produce and maintain racial health inequities. Yet, cultural racism has received relatively little attention in the public health literature. The purpose of this paper is to 1) provide public health researchers and policymakers with a clearer understanding of what cultural racism is, 2) provide an understanding of how it operates in conjunction with the other dimensions of racism to produce health inequities, and 3) offer directions for future research and interventions on cultural racism. METHODS We conducted a nonsystematic, multidisciplinary review of theory and empirical evidence that conceptualizes, measures, and documents the consequences of cultural racism for social and health inequities. FINDINGS Cultural racism can be defined as a culture of White supremacy, which values, protects, and normalizes Whiteness and White social and economic power. This ideological system operates at the level of our shared social consciousness and is expressed in the language, symbols, and media representations of dominant society. Cultural racism surrounds and bolsters structural, institutional, personally mediated, and internalized racism, undermining health through material, cognitive/affective, biologic, and behavioral mechanisms across the life course. CONCLUSIONS More time, research, and funding is needed to advance measurement, elucidate mechanisms, and develop evidence-based policy interventions to reduce cultural racism and promote health equity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- ELI K. MICHAELS
- Division of Epidemiology, School of Public HealthUniversity of California
| | - TRACY LAM‐HINE
- Division of Epidemiology & Population HealthStanford University School of Medicine
| | | | - GILBERT C. GEE
- Jonathan and Karin Fielding School of Public HealthUniversity of California
| | - AMANI M. ALLEN
- Division of Epidemiology, School of Public HealthUniversity of California
- Division of Community Health Sciences, School of Public HealthUniversity of California
| |
Collapse
|
57
|
Hecht CA, Murphy MC, Dweck CS, Bryan CJ, Trzesniewski KH, Medrano FN, Giani M, Mhatre P, Yeager DS. Shifting the mindset culture to address global educational disparities. NPJ SCIENCE OF LEARNING 2023; 8:29. [PMID: 37644082 PMCID: PMC10465593 DOI: 10.1038/s41539-023-00181-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2023] [Accepted: 08/17/2023] [Indexed: 08/31/2023]
Abstract
Educational outcomes remain highly unequal within and across nations. Students' mindsets-their beliefs about whether intellectual abilities can be developed-have been identified as a potential lever for making adolescents' academic outcomes more equitable. Recent research, however, suggests that intervention programs aimed at changing students' mindsets should be supplemented by programs aimed at the changing the mindset culture, which is defined as the shared set of beliefs about learning in a school or classroom. This paper reviews the theoretical and empirical origin of the mindset culture and examines its potential to reduce group-based inequalities in education. In particular, experiments have identified two broad ways the mindset culture is communicated by teachers: via informal messages about growth (e.g., that all students will be helped to learn and succeed), and formal opportunities to improve (e.g., learning-focused grading policies and opportunities to revise and earn credit). New field experiments, applying techniques from behavioral science, have also revealed effective ways to influence teachers' culture-creating behaviors. This paper describes recent breakthroughs in the U.S. educational context and discusses how lessons from these studies might be applied in future, global collaborations with researchers and practitioners.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | | | | | - Matt Giani
- The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
58
|
Soares De Almeida A, Gillath O, Kahalon R, Shnabel N. Effects of attachment security priming on women's math performance. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1124308. [PMID: 37691797 PMCID: PMC10484519 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1124308] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2022] [Accepted: 08/01/2023] [Indexed: 09/12/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction Activating people's sense of attachment security can buffer against psychological threats. Here we tested whether security priming can also buffer the adverse effects of stereotype threat among women. Method Three studies (a pilot study (N = 79 women, 72 men), a laboratory study; N = 474 women, and an online study; N = 827 women) compared security priming to neutral and positive affect priming. Results The pilot study revealed that women exposed to attachment security primes (e.g., the word "love") had better math performance than women exposed to neutral primes (e.g., "boat"). Men's math performance did not differ across priming conditions. Study 1 revealed that women showed better math performance in the attachment security priming condition than in the neutral or positive (e.g., "luck") priming conditions. The effect was observed among women high on math identification. In Study 2, despite an effect of security priming on the manipulation check [higher State Adult Attachment Measure (SAAM) security score], security did not buffer stereotype threat effects. Discussion Our findings provide partial support to the idea that security priming (an interpersonal process) can buffer stereotype threat (an intergroup process). Theoretical and practical implications related to attachment security priming and stereotype threat are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Omri Gillath
- Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States
| | - Rotem Kahalon
- The Azrieli Faculty of Medicine, Bar-Ilan University, Safed, Israel
| | - Nurit Shnabel
- The School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
59
|
Sebastián-Tirado A, Félix-Esbrí S, Forn C, Sanchis-Segura C. Are gender-science stereotypes barriers for women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics? Exploring when, how, and to whom in an experimentally-controlled setting. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1219012. [PMID: 37621930 PMCID: PMC10445161 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1219012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/08/2023] [Accepted: 07/25/2023] [Indexed: 08/26/2023] Open
Abstract
Based on Social Cognitive Career Theory principles, the present study sought to investigate whether stereotype threat experiences could act as a barrier and reduce the persistence of women in math-intensive activities. More specifically, we assessed whether the experimental activation of stereotypes about women's lower math capabilities affected the performance, persistence, and self-selected difficulty of engineering students in a math task which required sustained effort. We also evaluated the relationships between these effects and the participants' pre-testing gender-science stereotypes and math self-concept. A sample of 340 engineering students (175 and 165 self-identified as males and females, respectively) were recruited and randomly assigned to a control (Con) or stereotype threat (StA) condition to form four similarly sized groups. All participants rated their self-concept in four academic domains (math, chemistry, physics, and coding), completed the gender-science Implicit Association Test, and a "reading comprehension task" that served to promote the stereotype threat manipulation immediately before facing a modified version of the Math Effort Task (M-MET). We observed that, in the control condition, M-MET performance, self-selected difficulty, and persistence were similar in female and male participants, independent of their gender-science implicit stereotypes but correlated to their math self-concept scores. In contrast, the StA condition triggered opposite effects in female and male students, so they showed decreased/enhanced M-MET performance and self-selected difficulty, respectively. This experimental condition also resulted in enhanced persistence of the male students without affecting the number of trials completed by female students. These effects were correlated with the strength of the participants' gender-science implicit stereotypes but not with their math self-concept scores. In fact, as revealed by finer-grain analyses, stereotype threat only had a significant impact on individuals harboring stereotypical gender-science implicit associations. Therefore, it is concluded that: (1) stereotypes can promote differences between male and female engineering students in their performance, self-confidence, and persistence in math-related activities; (2) These effects seem to be more prominent in individuals implicitly perceiving science as a masculine domain. The relevance of these findings to explain women's enhanced abandonment rates of math-intensive studies is discussed.
Collapse
|
60
|
Lillqvist M, Claeson AS, Zakrzewska M, Andersson L. Comparable responses to a wide range of olfactory stimulation in women and men. Sci Rep 2023; 13:9059. [PMID: 37270617 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-35936-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/20/2023] [Accepted: 05/25/2023] [Indexed: 06/05/2023] Open
Abstract
The evidence for differences between women and men in terms of olfactory abilities is contradictory. We analyzed women and men's performance and reactions to a wider range of odour exposure outcomes than usually studied, to assess possible differences and similarities between sexes. Measures of sensitivity and sensory decision rule were established in 37 women and 39 men. Perceptual, cognitive, symptom-related and autonomic nervous system (skin conductance level and heart-rate variability) reactions were also assessed during extended ambient odour exposure, as well as participants' self-rated chemical intolerance. Bayesian analyses consistently revealed greater support for sex-related similarities than differences, suggesting that women and men perform and react comparably not only in terms of basic olfactory measures, but also to environmental odour exposure mimicking everyday situations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Moa Lillqvist
- Department of Psychology, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden.
| | | | - Marta Zakrzewska
- Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute, Solna, Sweden
| | | |
Collapse
|
61
|
O'Sullivan L, Kagabo W, Prasad N, Laporte D, Aiyer A. Racial and Ethnic Bias in Medical School Clinical Grading: A Review. JOURNAL OF SURGICAL EDUCATION 2023; 80:806-816. [PMID: 37019709 DOI: 10.1016/j.jsurg.2023.03.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2022] [Revised: 02/26/2023] [Accepted: 03/17/2023] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Improving diversity in healthcare is a widely recognized national goal. The diversity of medical student matriculants has increased, yet this trend is not seen in the composition of competitive residency programs. In this review, we examine racial and ethnic disparities in medical student grading during clinical years and explore the consequences of how this may exclude minority students from accessing competitive residency positions. DESIGN Following PRISMA guidelines, we searched PubMed, Embase, Scopus, and ERIC databases using variations of the terms "race," "ethnicity," "clerkship," "rotation," "grade," "evaluation", or "shelf exam." Of 391 references found using the criteria, 29 were related to clinical grading and race/ethnicity and included in the review. The GRADE criteria were used to determine the quality of evidence. SETTING Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore MD. RESULTS Five studies examining a total of 107,687 students from up to 113 different schools found racial minority students receive significantly fewer Honors grades in core clerkships compared to White students. Three studies examining 94,814 medical student evaluations from up to 130 different schools found significant disparities in the wording of written clerkship evaluations based on race and/or ethnicity. CONCLUSIONS A large body of evidence suggests the presence of racial bias in subjective clinical grading and written clerkship evaluations of medical students. Grading disparities can disadvantage minority students when applying to competitive residency programs and may contribute to a lack of diversity in these fields. As low minority representation has a negative impact on patient care and research advancement, strategies to resolve this issue must be further explored.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lucy O'Sullivan
- Johns Hopkins Orthopedics, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland.
| | - Whitney Kagabo
- Department of Orthopedics, Johns Hopkins Orthopedics, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland
| | - Niyathi Prasad
- Department of Orthopedics, Johns Hopkins Orthopedics, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland
| | - Dawn Laporte
- Department of Orthopedics, Johns Hopkins Orthopedics, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland
| | - Amiethab Aiyer
- Department of Orthopedics, Johns Hopkins Orthopedics, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland
| |
Collapse
|
62
|
Haft SL, Greiner de Magalhães C, Hoeft F. A Systematic Review of the Consequences of Stigma and Stereotype Threat for Individuals With Specific Learning Disabilities. JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES 2023; 56:193-209. [PMID: 35499115 PMCID: PMC10090527 DOI: 10.1177/00222194221087383] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
Exposure to stigma and stereotype threat is detrimental for numerous marginalized groups. Research has demonstrated that individuals with specific learning disabilities (SLDs) are vulnerable to stigmatization. The purpose of this systematic review was to summarize the studies investigating associations between SLD-related stigma and stereotype threat and psychological and academic outcomes in individuals with SLDs, as well as examine the overall effect size of these associations across studies. A total of 16 studies met inclusion criteria-12 studies (52 effect sizes) examined SLD stigma with psychological adjustment or academic outcomes, and six studies (eight effect sizes) examined SLD stereotype threat. Greater SLD stigma scores had a medium-sized and significant correlation with less self-esteem across nine effect sizes (r = -.39, p = .002). Other outcomes were not powered enough for meta-analyses, although studies generally showed that greater SLD stigma and stereotype threat was related to less optimal psychological adjustment. These results suggest that these negative experiences should be a target of intervention and support efforts for individuals with SLDs. The associations between SLD stigma and stereotype threat and academic performance outcomes were comparatively more heterogeneous and less robust. These findings highlight the need for more research on SLD-related stigma and stereotype threat.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Fumiko Hoeft
- University of Connecticut, Storrs, USA
- University of California San Francisco,
USA
- Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT,
USA
- Keio University School of Medicine,
Japan
| |
Collapse
|
63
|
Rippon G. Mind the gender gap: The social neuroscience of belonging. Front Hum Neurosci 2023; 17:1094830. [PMID: 37091814 PMCID: PMC10116861 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1094830] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/10/2022] [Accepted: 03/20/2023] [Indexed: 04/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Gender gaps persist in the 21st century, in many aspects of society and in many types of organisation. There are earnings gaps in almost all domains, reports of glass ceilings and the “missing middle” in business, finance, law and politics, and dramatic under-representation of women in many branches of science, even in the most “gender equal” countries. This is despite decades of effort to address them, including targeted legislation and many Diversity and Inclusion initiatives. Early essentialist, competence-based explanations for the existence of gender gaps have been largely discredited at the research level, although their persistence in the public consciousness and at the level of education and training can still negatively bias both individual self-belief and organisational processes. Contemporary essentialist explanations are now emerging, with claims that such gaps are the manifestations of the presence or absence of endogenous, brain-based characteristics underpinning career progression or career preferences. The focus remains on the individual as the source of gender imbalances. Less attention has been paid to the contextual aspects of organisations where gender gaps are evident, to inclusion (or the lack of it), or the availability of unbiased reward and progression pathways. Advances in 21st century social cognitive neuroscience are revealing the importance of external organisational processes as powerful brain-changing forces, with their potentially negative impact on self-belief and a sense of belonging. Key research is demonstrating the cortical and behavioural consequences of negative social experiences, with the activation of core inhibitory pathways associated with low self-esteem, lack of engagement, and eventual withdrawal. This paper will argue that reference to such research will provide better explanations for the persistence of gender gaps, and offer evidence-based insights into addressing gender gap issues. Importantly, this is not a rejection of an endogenous, brain-based explanation for gender gaps but the elaboration of a better-informed 21st century model, flagging up the need to take factors such as cultural stereotyping and organisational bias into account in any drive toward true gender equity, or genuinely levelled playing fields.
Collapse
|
64
|
Guglielmi RS. Probing gaps in educational outcomes within the U.S.: A dual moderation multiple mediator latent growth model. J Sch Psychol 2023; 97:123-151. [PMID: 36914362 DOI: 10.1016/j.jsp.2023.01.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/23/2021] [Revised: 10/06/2022] [Accepted: 01/10/2023] [Indexed: 02/17/2023]
Abstract
Racial/ethnic disparities in math achievement are especially troubling because math proficiency predicts long-term educational outcomes, but the mechanisms underlying these disparities remain unclear. Previous research has demonstrated that across diverse samples, both within and outside the United States, the relation between students' academic aspirations and later postsecondary attainment is mediated by initial levels of math ability and by growth in that ability across time. The key issue examined in this investigation is the extent to which students' underestimation or overestimation of their math ability (i.e., calibration bias) moderates those mediated effects and whether this moderation varies as a function of race/ethnicity. Using data from two longitudinal national surveys (i.e., NELS:88 and HSLS:09), these hypotheses were tested in samples of East Asian American, Mexican American, and Non-Hispanic White American high school students. In both studies and in all groups, the model explained large portions of the variance in postsecondary attainment. In East Asian Americans and non-Hispanic White Americans, calibration bias moderated the effect mediated by 9th grade math achievement. The strength of this effect was greatest at high levels of underconfidence and steadily weakened as self-confidence grew, suggesting that some degree of underconfidence may be achievement-promoting. Indeed, in the East Asian American sample, this effect became negative at high levels of overconfidence (i.e., academic aspirations actually predicted the lowest postsecondary attainment levels). Educational implications of these findings are discussed and possible reasons for the failure to find moderation effects in the Mexican American sample are explored.
Collapse
|
65
|
Tenney K, Stringer BP, LaTona-Tequida T, White I. Conceptualizations and Limitations of STEM Literacy across Learning Theories. JOURNAL OF MICROBIOLOGY & BIOLOGY EDUCATION 2023; 24:00168-22. [PMID: 37089236 PMCID: PMC10117085 DOI: 10.1128/jmbe.00168-22] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/08/2022] [Accepted: 02/09/2023] [Indexed: 05/03/2023]
Abstract
The concept of literacy is relevant in many areas of life, both inside and out of the classroom. With science, technology, engineering, and math (STEM) being fast-growing fields, STEM literacy in particular holds an important place in today's education system and beyond. While there is not a single definition for STEM literacy, it is impacted by possible assumptions educators make about students and the learning process. These assumptions are derived from many sources, including personal beliefs, past experiences, and learning theories that individuals utilize. This paper discusses the conceptualizations of STEM literacy and related limitations within three different learning theories: information processing, constructivism, and sociocultural theory. A theoretical framework is then discussed that addresses these limitations and incorporates a sociopolitical lens on scientific literacy, for the sake of helping educators identify and adjust their own perceptions for the sake of better supporting students.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristin Tenney
- University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California, USA
| | | | | | - Isabel White
- University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
66
|
Tellhed U, Björklund F, Kallio Strand K. Tech-Savvy Men and Caring Women: Middle School Students’ Gender Stereotypes Predict Interest in Tech-Education. SEX ROLES 2023. [DOI: 10.1007/s11199-023-01353-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/13/2023]
Abstract
AbstractThe labor market is strongly gender segregated with few women working in the tech sector (e.g., IT) and few men working in the care sector (e.g., nursing). We tested the hypothesis that middle school students strongly associate technology with men and caregiving with women in a Swedish context (i.e., a country that scores high in gender equality indices), and that these gender stereotypes for tech relate to girls’ lower interest in tech-focused education. We measured technology/caregiving gender stereotypes with implicit (the Implicit Association Test) and explicit (self-report) measures in a sample of middle school students (n = 873). The results supported the main hypotheses, and corroborate Eccles’s expectancy value theory, indicating that the endorsement of implicit gender stereotypes may serve as barriers to pursuing masculine-typed career paths for women. Further, a sample of middle school teachers (n = 86) showed stronger implicit gender stereotypes than the students. Unexpectedly, middle school girls with a foreign background showed no implicit gender stereotypes, which we discuss in relation to the gender-equality paradox. These findings suggest that to fulfill the recruitment needs of an increasingly digitalized world, the tech-industry and other stakeholders should put effort into counteracting the stereotype that technology is for men.
Collapse
|
67
|
Jantz MK, Mak J, Dalrymple AN, Farooqui J, Grigsby EM, Herrera AJ, Pirondini E, Collinger JL. Lifting as we climb: Experiences and recommendations from women in neural engineering. Front Neurosci 2023; 17:1104419. [PMID: 36968482 PMCID: PMC10033556 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1104419] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/21/2022] [Accepted: 02/22/2023] [Indexed: 03/11/2023] Open
Abstract
Neural engineering is an emerging and multidisciplinary field in which engineering approaches are applied to neuroscience problems. Women are underrepresented in engineering fields, and indeed in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) fields generally. Underrepresentation of women is particularly notable at later academic career stages, suggesting that even though women are interested in the field, barriers exist that ultimately cause them to leave. Here, we investigate many of the obstacles to women's success in the field of neural engineering and provide recommendations and materials to overcome them. We conducted a review of the literature from the past 15 years regarding the experiences of women in academic careers, as well as reports on the number of women in fields closely related to neural engineering from the National Science Foundation (NSF) and the American Society for Engineering Education (ASEE). Additionally, we interviewed six women in neural engineering who are involved in initiatives and outreach concerning the inclusion and experiences of women in engineering. Throughout the literature and interviews, we identified common themes spanning the role of identity and confidence, professional relationships, career-related hurdles, and personal and professional expectations. We explore each of these themes in detail and provide resources to support the growth of women as they climb within the field of neural engineering.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria K. Jantz
- Rehab Neural Engineering Labs, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Center for Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Jennifer Mak
- Rehab Neural Engineering Labs, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Center for Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Ashley N. Dalrymple
- Rehab Neural Engineering Labs, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Department of Mechanical Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Juhi Farooqui
- Rehab Neural Engineering Labs, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Center for Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Erinn M. Grigsby
- Rehab Neural Engineering Labs, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Angelica J. Herrera
- Rehab Neural Engineering Labs, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Center for Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Elvira Pirondini
- Rehab Neural Engineering Labs, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Center for Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Jennifer L. Collinger
- Rehab Neural Engineering Labs, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Center for Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
68
|
Metzger KJ, Dingel M, Brown E. "No matter what your story is, there is a place for you in science": Students' Ability to Relate to Scientists Positively Shifts after Scientist Spotlight Assignments, Especially for First-Generation Students and Women. CBE LIFE SCIENCES EDUCATION 2023; 22:ar12. [PMID: 36696139 PMCID: PMC10074274 DOI: 10.1187/cbe.22-06-0103] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2022] [Revised: 12/07/2022] [Accepted: 12/23/2022] [Indexed: 06/17/2023]
Abstract
We evaluate the impact of a low-stakes easy-to-implement course-level intervention, Scientist Spotlight assignments, which feature personal and professional stories of diverse scientists. This work extends previous studies by examining whether shifts in relatability differ across student identities, particularly students who identify as first-generation students, a population that has not been the focus of previous investigations of this intervention. Using paired pre- and postcourse data from four implementations in an introductory biology course, we report a significant, positive shift in undergraduate students' self-reported ability to relate to scientists, and concomitant shifts in how students describe scientists after completing four or six Scientist Spotlight assignments.Importantly, our data demonstrate a disproportionate, positive shift for first-generation college students and for students who identify as female, a novel contribution to the body of literature investigating the Scientist Spotlight intervention. This study, along with previous reports of similar shifts in varying institutional contexts across different populations of learners, provides a strong argument that instructors interested in diversifying their course content to include representations of diverse scientists to enhance students' ability to identify a range of "types of people" who do science can do so successfully through incorporation of a small number of Spotlight assignments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kelsey J. Metzger
- Center for Learning Innovation, University of Minnesota Rochester, Rochester, MN 55904
| | - Molly Dingel
- Center for Learning Innovation, University of Minnesota Rochester, Rochester, MN 55904
| | - Ethan Brown
- Research Methodology Consulting Center, College of Education and Human Development, University of Minnesota, St. Paul, MN 55108
| |
Collapse
|
69
|
French AM, Else-Quest NM, Asher M, Thoman DB, Smith JL, Hyde JS, Harackiewicz JM. An Intersectional Application of Expectancy-Value Theory in an Undergraduate Chemistry Course. PSYCHOLOGY OF WOMEN QUARTERLY 2023. [DOI: 10.1177/03616843231153390] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/22/2023]
Abstract
The underrepresentation of women and Black, Latinx, and Native Americans within the United States scientific workforce is a persistent and multifaceted problem warranting an intersectional approach. Applying intersectionality to the expectancy-value theory of motivation, we examined initial motivation and subsequent achievement among a sample of undergraduate students ( N = 687) enrolled in the science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) gateway course of introductory chemistry at a diverse 4-year university. We found no racial/ethnic group differences in initial motivation, but small ( d = .30) group differences in achievement. Results revealed a pattern of gender differences across both underrepresented (i.e., Black, Latinx, and Native American) and well-represented (i.e., White, Asian American) racial/ethnic groups such that, relative to men, women began the class with lower levels of confidence about their performance, but greater utility value and attainment value in learning chemistry. Consistent with expectancy-value theory, motivation at the beginning of the semester positively predicted final exam scores across gender and racial/ethnic intersectional groups. For Black, Latinx, and Native American students, attainment value was an especially strong predictor of subsequent achievement. Our findings point to the need to cultivate social contexts within undergraduate STEM education that promotes all aspects of science motivation among students from underrepresented groups. Additional online materials for this article are available on PWQ's website at http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/suppl/10.1177/03616843231153390 .
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Allison M. French
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, Baltimore County, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Nicole M. Else-Quest
- Department of Women's and Gender Studies, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
| | - Michael Asher
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, USA
| | - Dustin B. Thoman
- Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Jessi L. Smith
- Department of Psychology, University of Colorado, Colorado Springs, CO, USA
| | - Janet S. Hyde
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, USA
- Department of Gender and Women's Studies, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
70
|
Snowden LR, Wallace N, Graaf G. Subsidized Marketplace Purchases Reduced Racial Disparities in Private Coverage Under the Affordable Care Act. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities 2023; 10:141-148. [PMID: 35032008 PMCID: PMC8760123 DOI: 10.1007/s40615-021-01204-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/02/2021] [Revised: 11/11/2021] [Accepted: 12/03/2021] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
The Affordable Care Act's Marketplaces, by allowing subsidized purchase of insurance coverage by persons with incomes from the poverty line to middle income, and through active outreach and enrollment assistance efforts, are well situated to reduce large African American-white private coverage disparities. Using data from the National Health Interview Survey for multiyear periods before and after Affordable Care Act implementation, from 2011-2013 to 2015-2018, this study assessed how much disparity reduction occurred when Marketplaces were implemented. Analysis compared private coverage take-up by African Americans and whites for persons with incomes between 100 and 400% of the Federal Poverty Line (FPL), controlling for African American-white income differences and other covariates. African Americans' gains were significantly greater than whites' and disparities did close. However, both groups gained considerably less coverage than they might have, and some disparity remained. To make ongoing operations more effective and to guide future subsidy extensions and increases as enacted in the American Rescue Plan, more research is needed into the incentive value of subsidies and to discover which Marketplace outreach and enrollment assistance efforts were most effective. In advancing these aims, high priority should be given to identifying strategies that were particularly successful in reaching and engaging uninsured African Americans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lonnie R Snowden
- School of Public Health, University of California, Berkeley, 2121 Berkeley Way, Room 5302, Berkeley, CA, 94720-7360, USA
| | - Neal Wallace
- OHSU-PSU School of Public Health, Portland State University, 1805 SW 4th Ave 523E, Portland, OR, 97201, USA
| | - Genevieve Graaf
- School of Social Work, University of Texas, Arlington, 211 S. Cooper St., Arlington, TX, 76017, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
71
|
Bastos BDP, Chiviacowsky S, Drews R, Cardozo P. Gender Stereotype Threat Undermines Dance Performance and Learning in Boys. J Mot Behav 2023; 55:256-261. [PMID: 36693652 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2023.2166454] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/26/2023]
Abstract
The objective of the present study was to examine the effects of gender stereotype on the performance and learning of a classical ballet pirouette in 11-year-old boys. Participants in the stereotype threat (ST) group were informed that, in general, boys tend to show worse results when completing the pirouette task in comparison to girls. Participants in the stereotype lift (SL) group were told that girls tend to show worse results in comparison to boys. The control group did not receive stereotype instructions. The results demonstrated worse performance during practice and retention (next day) for the ST group relative to the SL and control groups; the SL and control groups did not differ. The findings indicate that gender stereotype threat can undermine motor performance and learning in boys.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Ricardo Drews
- Federal University of Uberlândia, Uberlândia, Brazil
| | | |
Collapse
|
72
|
Abstract
Despite progress made toward increasing women's interest and involvement in science, technology, engineering, and math (STEM), women continue to be underrepresented and experience less equity and inclusion in some STEM fields. In this article, I review the psychological literature relevant to understanding and mitigating women's lower fit and inclusion in STEM. Person-level explanations concerning women's abilities, interests, and self-efficacy are insufficient for explaining these persistent gaps. Rather, women's relatively lower interest in male-dominated STEM careers such as computer science and engineering is likely to be constrained by gender stereotypes. These gender stereotypes erode women's ability to experience self-concept fit, goal fit, and/or social fit. Such effects occur independently of intentional interpersonal biases and discrimination, and yet they create systemic barriers to women's attraction to, integration in, and advancement in STEM. Dismantling these systemic barriers requires a multifaceted approach to changing organizational and educational cultures at the institutional, interpersonal, and individual level.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Toni Schmader
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada;
| |
Collapse
|
73
|
A Psychometric Network Analysis of CHC Intelligence Measures: Implications for Research, Theory, and Interpretation of Broad CHC Scores "Beyond g". J Intell 2023; 11:jintelligence11010019. [PMID: 36662149 PMCID: PMC9865475 DOI: 10.3390/jintelligence11010019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/18/2022] [Revised: 01/07/2023] [Accepted: 01/10/2023] [Indexed: 01/18/2023] Open
Abstract
For over a century, the structure of intelligence has been dominated by factor analytic methods that presume tests are indicators of latent entities (e.g., general intelligence or g). Recently, psychometric network methods and theories (e.g., process overlap theory; dynamic mutualism) have provided alternatives to g-centric factor models. However, few studies have investigated contemporary cognitive measures using network methods. We apply a Gaussian graphical network model to the age 9-19 standardization sample of the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability-Fourth Edition. Results support the primary broad abilities from the Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) theory and suggest that the working memory-attentional control complex may be central to understanding a CHC network model of intelligence. Supplementary multidimensional scaling analyses indicate the existence of possible higher-order dimensions (PPIK; triadic theory; System I-II cognitive processing) as well as separate learning and retrieval aspects of long-term memory. Overall, the network approach offers a viable alternative to factor models with a g-centric bias (i.e., bifactor models) that have led to erroneous conclusions regarding the utility of broad CHC scores in test interpretation beyond the full-scale IQ, g.
Collapse
|
74
|
Reggiani M, Gagnon JD, Lunn RJ. LGBT + academics' and PhD students' experiences of visibility in STEM: more than raising the rainbow flag. HIGHER EDUCATION 2023; 87:1-19. [PMID: 36684612 PMCID: PMC9839222 DOI: 10.1007/s10734-023-00993-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 01/06/2023] [Indexed: 06/17/2023]
Abstract
The experiences of lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender (LGBT +) individuals in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM) are still understudied and, despite some improvements, are still characterised by patterns of exclusion, disadvantage, and discrimination. In this article, we explore how visibility is perceived and navigated by LGBT + academics and PhD students in STEM, with a focus on the ways that interlocking systems of oppression impact people and groups who are marginalised and historically excluded. This article draws on a broader research project about the experiences of women and LGBT + people in STEM that was conducted between 2019 and 2020 at a UK university and is framed by intersectionality theory. Based on the thematic analysis of interviews and focus groups with 24 LGBT + participants, findings suggest that visibility is still a risk for LGBT + academics and PhD students in STEM. We found that the labour of navigating visibility was perceived as an unfair disadvantage and that the focus on individuals' visibility in the absence of meaningful and transformative inclusion initiatives by higher education institutions was regarded as tokenistic. The article argues that addressing LGBT + visibility should firstly be an institutional responsibility and not an individual burden and that this work is essential to set the conditions for personal visibility to happen by choice, safely and without retribution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marco Reggiani
- Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, UK
| | | | - Rebecca Jane Lunn
- Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, UK
| |
Collapse
|
75
|
Thiem KC, Clark JK. Gender stereotypes, intellectual performance, and stereotype validation: The role of lay theories of intelligence. SELF AND IDENTITY 2023. [DOI: 10.1080/15298868.2022.2164609] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Kelsey C. Thiem
- Counseling Psychology, Social Psychology, and Counseling, Ball State University, Muncie, U S
| | - Jason K. Clark
- Consumer Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, U S
| |
Collapse
|
76
|
Singh MK, Nimarko A, Bruno J, Anand KJS, Singh SP. Can Translational Social Neuroscience Research Offer Insights to Mitigate Structural Racism in the United States? BIOLOGICAL PSYCHIATRY. COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROIMAGING 2022; 7:1258-1267. [PMID: 35609781 PMCID: PMC11611498 DOI: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2022.05.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/15/2022] [Revised: 04/15/2022] [Accepted: 05/05/2022] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
Social isolation and conflict due to structural racism may result in human suffering and loneliness across the life span. Given the rising prevalence of these problems in the United States, combined with disruptions experienced during the COVID-19 pandemic, the neurobiology of affiliative behaviors may offer practical solutions to the pressing challenges associated with structural racism. Controlled experiments across species demonstrate that social connections are critical to survival, although strengthening individual resilience is insufficient to address the magnitude and impact of structural racism. In contrast, the multilevel construct of social resilience, defined by the power of groups to cultivate, engage in, and sustain positive relationships that endure and recuperate from social adversities, offers unique insights that may have greater impact, reach, and durability than individual-level interventions. Here, we review putative social resilience-enhancing interventions and, when available, their biological mediators, with the hope to stimulate discovery of novel approaches to mitigate structural racism. We first explore the social neuroscience principles underlying psychotherapy and other psychiatric interventions. Then, we explore translational efforts across species to tailor treatments that increase social resilience, with context and cultural sensitivity in mind. Finally, we conclude with some practical future directions for understudied areas that may be essential for progress in biological psychiatry, including ethical ways to increase representation in research and developing social paradigms that inform dynamics toward or away from socially resilient outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Manpreet K Singh
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, California.
| | - Akua Nimarko
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, California
| | - Jennifer Bruno
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, California
| | - Kanwaljeet J S Anand
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, California
| | - Swaran P Singh
- Centre of Mental Health and Wellbeing Research, Warwick Medical School, University of Warwick, Coventry, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
77
|
Piroelle M, Abadie M, Régner I. Toward a New Approach to Investigate the Role of Working Memory in Stereotype Threat Effects. Brain Sci 2022; 12:brainsci12121647. [PMID: 36552105 PMCID: PMC9775410 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci12121647] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2022] [Revised: 11/24/2022] [Accepted: 11/28/2022] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Stereotype threat arises when the activation of negative stereotypes about a group impairs performance of stigmatized individuals on stereotype relevant tasks. There is ample evidence that stereotype threat leads to performance detriments by consuming executive resources. Several studies indeed showed that working memory (WM) mediates stereotype threat effects among young adults. More recently, researchers have sought to understand whether the same mechanisms underlie age-based stereotype threat, but findings are mixed regarding the role of WM and some authors rather favor a motivational explanation based on regulatory fit. The present review critically appraises the empirical support for distinct forms of stereotype threat effects mediated by distinct mechanisms. We propose a novel approach based on one of the most recent WM models, the time-based resource sharing model, to evaluate the impact of stereotype threat on attentional resources in WM among both young and older adults.
Collapse
|
78
|
Guzmán B, Rodríguez C, Ferreira RA. Moderated-moderation effect of parents’ math anxiety and home numeracy activities on young children’s math performance-anxiety relationship. CONTEMPORARY EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2022.102140] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
|
79
|
Qu Y, Devakonda V, Shi Z, Yang B, Wang Q. The role of classroom- and individual-level teen stereotypes in Chinese adolescents' academic adjustment: A multilevel analysis. Front Psychol 2022; 13:933485. [PMID: 36506987 PMCID: PMC9732083 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.933485] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2022] [Accepted: 11/07/2022] [Indexed: 11/27/2022] Open
Abstract
Adolescence is often portrayed in a negative light in Western culture, with teens being viewed as rebellious and irresponsible. Yet, there is substantial cultural and individual variability in views of teens. The empirical research to date is limited in that it mainly examines whether teen stereotypes are influential at the individual level. Teen stereotypes might also be perpetuated at the classroom level, which may have important implications for adolescent adjustment over time. Focusing on adolescents in Chinese culture where the teen years are often viewed in a positive light, this two-wave longitudinal study employed multi-level analyses to investigate whether stereotypes of adolescence at the classroom level play a role in Chinese adolescents' academic adjustment over time (N = 785; 55% girls; mean age = 12.96 years). Consistent with prior research on views of teens, the present analyses suggested that teen stereotypes regarding family obligation and school engagement at the individual level predicted adolescents' value of school and self-regulated learning strategies over the seventh grade. More importantly, classroom-level teen stereotypes were also largely predictive of adolescents' value of school and self-regulated learning strategies over time, controlling for their earlier academic adjustment, individual-level teen stereotypes, and classroom-level adjustment. Taken together, these findings indicate that stereotypes of adolescence in classroom or peer settings may contribute to adolescents' academic adjustment during this phase. The findings also provide a potential foundation for interventions aimed at promoting adolescents' positive development via changing teen stereotypes in the classroom.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yang Qu
- School of Education and Social Policy, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, United States,*Correspondence: Yang Qu
| | - Varun Devakonda
- School of Education and Social Policy, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, United States
| | - Zeyi Shi
- Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China
| | - Beiming Yang
- School of Education and Social Policy, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, United States
| | - Qian Wang
- Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China,Qian Wang
| |
Collapse
|
80
|
Styk W, Wojtowicz E, Zmorzynski S. Reliable Knowledge about Obesity Risk, Rather Than Personality, Is Associated with Positive Beliefs towards Obese People: Investigating Attitudes and Beliefs about Obesity, and Validating the Polish Versions of ATOP, BAOP and ORK-10 Scales. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL RESEARCH AND PUBLIC HEALTH 2022; 19:14977. [PMID: 36429694 PMCID: PMC9690218 DOI: 10.3390/ijerph192214977] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2022] [Revised: 11/06/2022] [Accepted: 11/10/2022] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
UNLABELLED Obesity has reached epidemic proportions. With the increase in the number of obese people, we have also witnessed a rise in the stigmatisation of this population. The aim of our study was to: (I) validate Polish versions of the attitude toward obese people (ATOP) scale, the beliefs about obese persons (BAOP) scale, and translate the obesity risk knowledge scale (ORK-10); (II) analyse the relationship between personality and the knowledge about obesity, as well as attitudes and beliefs towards obese people. METHODS The translation procedure was based on the principles of intercultural validation scales. The study was conducted on a group of 306 individuals, including 189 females and 117 males. RESULTS The original three-factor structure of the ATOP scale was confirmed in the Polish version. Factor analysis confirmed the one-factor structure of the BAOP scale in the Polish version. A very strong correlation was found between ATOP/BAOP and ORK-10. The correlation of personality with ATOP/BAOP scales was at a low level. Regression analysis indicated that knowledge of obesity risk predicted ATOP and BAOP by more than 58% and 50%, in turn, personality only 20% and 3.7%, respectively. CONCLUSION The polish versions of ATOP, BAOP and ORK-10 scales are fully useful measurement tools. The knowledge about obesity risk is associated with beliefs and attitudes about obese people.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wojciech Styk
- Department of Psychology, Medical University of Lublin, 20-059 Lublin, Poland
| | - Ewa Wojtowicz
- Department of Psychology, Institute of Pedagogy and Psychology, Warsaw Management University, 03-772 Warszawa, Poland
| | - Szymon Zmorzynski
- Department of Cancer Genetics with Cytogenetic Laboratory, Medical University of Lublin, 20-059 Lublin, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
81
|
Wang Z, Guan J, Zhao L. Priming Science Identity Elicits Stereotype Boost for Chinese Science Female Students: Evidence From Math Performance. BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2022.2144315] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
|
82
|
Gender Stereotypes and Peer Selection in STEM Domains Among Children and Adolescents. SEX ROLES 2022; 87:455-470. [DOI: 10.1007/s11199-022-01327-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/16/2022] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
AbstractGender stereotypes are harmful for girls’ enrollment and performance in science and mathematics. So far, less is known about children’s and adolescents’ stereotypes regarding technology and engineering. In the current study, participants’ (N = 1,206, girls n = 623; 5–17-years-old, M = 8.63, SD = 2.81) gender stereotypes for each of the STEM (science, technology, engineering, and mathematics) domains were assessed along with the relation between these stereotypes and a peer selection task in a STEM context. Participants reported beliefs that boys are usually more skilled than are girls in the domains of engineering and technology; however, participants did not report gender differences in ability/performance in science and mathematics. Responses to the stereotype measures in favor of one’s in-group were greater for younger participants than older participants for both boys and girls. Perceptions that boys are usually better than girls at science were related to a greater likelihood of selecting a boy for help with a science question. These findings document the importance of domain specificity, even within STEM, in attempts to measure and challenge gender stereotypes in childhood and adolescence.
Collapse
|
83
|
Holmes O, Smith AN, Loyd DL, Gutiérrez AS. Scholars of color explore bias in academe: Calling in allies and sharing affirmations for us by us. ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR AND HUMAN DECISION PROCESSES 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2022.104204] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
84
|
Lei RF, Frazer‐Klotz Z, Szanton EC. Black‐Asian
solidarity through collective racial socialization. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2022. [DOI: 10.1002/icd.2380] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Ryan F. Lei
- Department of Psychology Haverford College Haverford Pennsylvania USA
| | - Zoe Frazer‐Klotz
- Department of Psychology Haverford College Haverford Pennsylvania USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
85
|
Social Evaluation at a Distance – Facets of Stereotype Content about Student Groups in Higher Distance Education. INTERNATIONAL REVIEW OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.5334/irsp.686] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
|
86
|
Vago DR, Farb N, Spreng RN. Clarifying Internally-Directed Cognition: A Commentary on the Attention to Thoughts Model. PSYCHOLOGICAL INQUIRY 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/1047840x.2022.2141005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/10/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- David R. Vago
- Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee
- Contemplative Sciences Center, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia
| | - Norman Farb
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga, Canada
| | - R. Nathan Spreng
- Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, Montreal Neurological Institute, Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
- Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
- Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Verdun, Canada
- McConnell Brain Imaging Centre, Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
87
|
Sure I can code (but do I want to?). Why boys' and girls’ programming beliefs differ and the effects of mandatory programming education. COMPUTERS IN HUMAN BEHAVIOR 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2022.107370] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
88
|
Nicolaou N, Kilduff M. Empowerment Mitigates Gender Differences in Tertius Iungens Brokering. ORGANIZATION SCIENCE 2022. [DOI: 10.1287/orsc.2022.1628] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Tertius iungens brokering that brings together people who might not otherwise meet is crucial for organizational effectiveness. But we know little about whether and why women and men differ in their propensity to engage in this brokering. Our paper focuses on the origins and mitigation of gender differences in the propensity to bring people together. In study 1, we showed that the Totterdell et al. [Totterdell P, Holman D, Hukin A (2008) Social networkers: Measuring and examining individual differences in propensity to connect with others. Soc. Networks 30(4):283–296] propensity-to-join-others scale that we used in study 2 and the Obstfeld [Obstfeld D (2005) Social networks, the tertius iungens orientation, and involvement in innovation. Admin. Sci. Quart. 50(1):100–130] tertius iungens scale overlapped not only conceptually, but also empirically, and that these measures of tertius iungens were distinct from mediation- and separation-brokering propensities [Grosser TJ, Obstfeld D, Labianca G, Borgatti SP (2019) Measuring mediation and separation brokerage orientations: A further step toward studying the social network brokerage process. Acad. Management Discoveries 5(2):114–136]. In study 2, we used a natural experiment to examine the tertius iungens brokering propensities of 876 identical and 625 fraternal same-sex twins. We found that brokering propensity was lower for women than for men, although the propensity toward sociability in terms of making friends and acquaintances was lower for men. We also found that for women, relative to men, tertius iungens brokering propensity was largely affected by environmental influences, such as the experience of stereotyping and discrimination, rather than representing an inherited disposition. Moreover, the differences between men and women with respect to brokering were mitigated for empowered samples, such as well-educated or entrepreneurial individuals. Our research asks new questions about how environmental pressures and empowerment affect social networking. Gender differences in brokering may be amenable to mitigation through empowering practices that include education and entrepreneurial experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicos Nicolaou
- Warwick Business School, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, United Kingdom
| | - Martin Kilduff
- UCL School of Management, University College London, London E14 5AA, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
89
|
Premo J, Wyatt BN, Horn M, Wilson-Ashworth H. Which Group Dynamics Matter: Social Predictors of Student Achievement in Team-Based Undergraduate Science Classrooms. CBE LIFE SCIENCES EDUCATION 2022; 21:ar51. [PMID: 35900893 PMCID: PMC9582812 DOI: 10.1187/cbe.21-06-0164] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/09/2023]
Abstract
While group work in undergraduate science education tends to have overall benefit, less is known about the specific peer-peer dynamics that optimize learning during group interaction. The current study used peer ratings and self-reported data from 436 students enrolled in team-based undergraduate science courses (biology or chemistry) to determine group dynamics that predicted both willingness to work with peers in the future and individual achievement in the course. Results show that greater personal connection and contributions predicted willingness to work with a group member (R2biology = 0.75; R2chemistry = 0.59). While active contribution to a group predicted greater achievement, more noncontent interactions (e.g., encouragement, listening to feedback, being polite) predicted lower achievement, despite these being on-task and relevant. Additionally, having group members who were willing to continue working with a student was a positive predictor of that student's achievement regardless of course. Strikingly, students in chemistry were significantly less willing to work with women in their groups compared with men. Finally, not all forms of group conflict predict decreased achievement. These findings highlight group factors such as student behavior within the group, aspects of the group social environment, and peer support that can be targeted for optimizing undergraduate science learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joshua Premo
- Department of Biology, Utah Valley University, Orem, UT 84058
- *Address correspondence to: Joshua Premo ()
| | | | - Matthew Horn
- Department of Chemistry, Utah Valley University, Orem, UT 84058
| | | |
Collapse
|
90
|
Muirhead L, Cimiotti JP, Hayes R, Haynes-Ferere A, Martyn K, Owen M, McCauley L. Diversity in nursing and challenges with the NCLEX-RN. Nurs Outlook 2022; 70:762-771. [PMID: 35933180 DOI: 10.1016/j.outlook.2022.06.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2022] [Revised: 06/06/2022] [Accepted: 06/14/2022] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
High-stakes, standardized testing has historically impeded education/career attainment for members of underrepresented minority groups and people needing testing accommodations. This study was to understand how high-stakes, standardized testing, particularly the NCLEX-RN, impacts diversity, equity, and inclusion (DEI) in nursing. This study explored the history, context, perspectives surrounding standardized testing, with a focus on the NCLEX-RN. The authors consider content, form, and delivery of testing, including accommodations. They identify available data and data collection gaps relevant to DEI and the NCLEX-RN. No nursing organization published the national data necessary to evaluate/refine the NCLEX-RN from a DEI perspective. Preliminary nursing studies and data from other professions indicated disparities in testing outcomes. Nursing must determine if prospective nurses are experiencing disparities in testing outcomes. The authors highlight opportunities to advance DEI through improved data collection, reformed licensure processes, and the reframing of standardized testing as one of many tools to determine competency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa Muirhead
- Emory University, Nell Hodgson Woodruff School of Nursing, Atlanta, GA
| | | | - Rose Hayes
- Emory University, Nell Hodgson Woodruff School of Nursing, Atlanta, GA.
| | | | - Kristy Martyn
- Emory University, Nell Hodgson Woodruff School of Nursing, Atlanta, GA
| | - Melissa Owen
- Emory University, Nell Hodgson Woodruff School of Nursing, Atlanta, GA
| | - Linda McCauley
- Emory University, Nell Hodgson Woodruff School of Nursing, Atlanta, GA
| |
Collapse
|
91
|
Block K, Aday A, Hall WM, Schmader T. Making America great for whom?: How Trump’s Presidency affected fit and national identity among targets of bias. SELF AND IDENTITY 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/15298868.2022.2113124] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Katharina Block
- Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Audrey Aday
- Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
| | - William M. Hall
- Department of Psychology, Brock University, St. Catharines, Canada
| | - Toni Schmader
- Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
92
|
Hall W, Schmader T, Cyr E, Bergsieker H. Collectively constructing Gender-Inclusive work cultures in STEM. EUROPEAN REVIEW OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/10463283.2022.2109294] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/15/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- William Hall
- Department of Psychology, Brock University, St. Catharines, ON, Canada
| | - Toni Schmader
- Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
| | - Emily Cyr
- Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | - Hilary Bergsieker
- Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
93
|
Draheim C, Pak R, Draheim AA, Engle RW. The role of attention control in complex real-world tasks. Psychon Bull Rev 2022; 29:1143-1197. [PMID: 35167106 PMCID: PMC8853083 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-021-02052-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/14/2021] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Working memory capacity is an important psychological construct, and many real-world phenomena are strongly associated with individual differences in working memory functioning. Although working memory and attention are intertwined, several studies have recently shown that individual differences in the general ability to control attention is more strongly predictive of human behavior than working memory capacity. In this review, we argue that researchers would therefore generally be better suited to studying the role of attention control rather than memory-based abilities in explaining real-world behavior and performance in humans. The review begins with a discussion of relevant literature on the nature and measurement of both working memory capacity and attention control, including recent developments in the study of individual differences of attention control. We then selectively review existing literature on the role of both working memory and attention in various applied settings and explain, in each case, why a switch in emphasis to attention control is warranted. Topics covered include psychological testing, cognitive training, education, sports, police decision-making, human factors, and disorders within clinical psychology. The review concludes with general recommendations and best practices for researchers interested in conducting studies of individual differences in attention control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christopher Draheim
- Department of Psychology, Lawrence University, Appleton, WI, USA.
- School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA, USA.
| | - Richard Pak
- Department of Psychology, Clemson University, Clemson, SC, USA
| | - Amanda A Draheim
- Department of Psychology, Lawrence University, Appleton, WI, USA
| | - Randall W Engle
- School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
94
|
Chen YT, McDonough IM, Faig KE, Norman GJ, Gallo DA. Impact of stereotype threat on brain activity during memory tasks in older adults. Neuroimage 2022; 260:119413. [PMID: 35853542 PMCID: PMC9436003 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119413] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2022] [Accepted: 06/23/2022] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
We report the first neuroimaging experiment to investigate the impact of explicitly activating aging stereotypes (i.e., stereotype threat) on brain activity during cognitive tasks. Cognitively normal older adults read about aging stereotypes or a control passage prior to taking episodic memory, working memory, and a non-demanding control task during fMRI. At the group level, stereotype activation did not impact cognitive performance or measures sensitive to stress and anxiety (physiological or self-report), but like prior work, highly educated and retired adults exhibited greater stereotype effects on episodic memory. At the neural level, stereotype activation did not impact brain activity in executive control or emotional regulation regions previously linked to stereotype threat effects in younger adults, suggesting that stereotype threat operates differently in older adults. Instead, on each task, the stereotype group showed more brain activity than the control group in parietal midline regions (e.g., precuneus, posterior cingulate). Although activity in these regions can arise from many processes, they have previously been associated with self-referential thinking and error-prevention focus, and in our study, brain activity in these regions was associated with slower responses and lower false alarm errors on the episodic memory task. Collectively, these findings are more consistent with the regulatory fit hypothesis than an executive control interference hypothesis of stereotype threat effects in older adults, whereby older adults adopt an error-prevention mindset in response to explicit stereotype threat.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yung-Tsen Chen
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, 5848 S. University Ave., Chicago, IL, 60637, USA
| | - Ian M McDonough
- Department of Psychology, The University of Alabama, 505 Hackberry Lane, BOX 870348, Tuscaloosa, AL 35487, USA
| | - Kelly E Faig
- Psychology Department, Hamilton College, 198 College Hill Road, Clinton, NY 13323, USA
| | - Greg J Norman
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, 5848 S. University Ave., Chicago, IL, 60637, USA
| | - David A Gallo
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, 5848 S. University Ave., Chicago, IL, 60637, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
95
|
Can I dismiss the stereotype – as my teacher did? Influence of stereotype activation and an immigrant teacher on student learning. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF EDUCATION 2022; 25:745-766. [PMID: 35812762 PMCID: PMC9255841 DOI: 10.1007/s11218-022-09707-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/24/2021] [Revised: 06/04/2022] [Accepted: 06/07/2022] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Lower vocabulary in German is repeatedly reported for students with Turkish migration background attending school in Germany. We investigated whether in students of Turkish descent (a) learning vocabulary is impaired when the teacher activates the negative stereotype that students with Turkish family language learn less well and (b) whether a Turkish-origin teacher, as an ingroup expert model, can mitigate negative effects of the activation of the stereotype. In an experimental study, Turkish- and German-origin students (N = 182) living in Germany worked individually on a tablet on a vocabulary learning task instructed by a teacher in a video tutorial who introduced herself with either a Turkish or German name. Before the task, the teacher either mentioned that students in general (no stereotype activation) or students who speak Turkish in their families (stereotype activation) often have difficulties acquiring new vocabulary. A multiple-group regression analysis showed that Turkish-origin students learned significantly more under stereotype activation with the Turkish-origin teacher than in all other conditions. Results suggest that students are particularly motivated to learn when the teacher represents their ingroup targeted by negative stereotypes and openly addresses potential difficulties students of the stigmatized ingroup may encounter. We discuss the findings in light of the literature on stereotype threat and on the role of ingroup expert models.
Collapse
|
96
|
Piñero DP. Peer-reviewed process: is everything valid? JOURNAL OF OPTOMETRY 2022; 15:189-190. [PMID: 35728878 PMCID: PMC9237586 DOI: 10.1016/j.optom.2022.06.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- David P Piñero
- Department of Optics, Pharmacology and Anatomy, University of Alicante, Alicante, Spain; Department of Ophthalmology, Vithas Medimar International Hospital, Alicante, Spain.
| |
Collapse
|
97
|
Whaley AL. Ethnicity, nativity, and the effects of stereotypes on cardiovascular health among people of African ancestry in the United States: internal versus external sources of racism. ETHNICITY & HEALTH 2022; 27:1010-1030. [PMID: 33222505 DOI: 10.1080/13557858.2020.1847257] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2020] [Accepted: 10/30/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The present study tests the hypothesis that ethnicity and nativity moderate the association of negative racial stereotypes versus perceived discrimination to cardiovascular health among Black respondents to the National Survey of American Life (NSAL). It is also hypothesized that the relationship is strongest in African Americans and weakest in foreign-born Caribbean Blacks with U.S.-born Caribbean Blacks falling in the middle. The same pattern of results is expected to occur for the correlation between perceived discrimination and cardiovascular health. METHOD A representative sample of 3570 (100%) African American and 1419 (87.4%) of 1623 Caribbean Black respondents to the NSAL had complete data for use in this study. The Caribbean subsample was divided by nativity into 373 (26.3%) U.S.-born and 1044 (73.7%) foreign-born participants. Structural equation modeling (SEM) tested the measurement model for effects of internalized racism and perceived racism on cardiovascular health mediated by perceived mastery and performance apprehension. RESULTS SEM analyses revealed that perceived discrimination had a much stronger effect on cardiovascular health than internalized racism for African Americans and foreign-born Caribbean Blacks, but the diametrically opposite pattern was the case for U.S.-born Caribbean Blacks who reported greater effects for internalized racism than perceived discrimination. CONCLUSION Interventions and policies to eliminate disparities in cardiovascular health for the U.S. Black population must address internal and external sources of racism by ethnicity and nativity.
Collapse
|
98
|
Advancing Diversity in Nursing Education: A Groundwater Approach. J Prof Nurs 2022; 41:140-148. [DOI: 10.1016/j.profnurs.2022.05.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2022] [Revised: 05/06/2022] [Accepted: 05/10/2022] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
99
|
Hassan M, Luo Y, Gu J, Mushtaque I, Rizwan M. Investigating the Parental and Media Influence on Gender Stereotypes and Young Student’s Career Choices in Pakistan. Front Psychol 2022; 13:890680. [PMID: 35837626 PMCID: PMC9274975 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.890680] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2022] [Accepted: 05/23/2022] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
The study aimed to examine the impact of parental influence and media richness on gender stereotypes and career decisions among students at the secondary level in Pakistan. The sample size was 200 students, selected through a simple random sampling technique from government and private schools. Four questionnaires were used to gather data. The data was analyzed quantitatively using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS). Regression analyses were used to investigate the impact of parental influence (β = 0.50) on gender stereotypes and media richness influence (β = 0.26) on gender stereotype beliefs. Furthermore, parental, media, and gender stereotype behavior all have a significant impact on students’ career choices (R2 = 0.694). On the scale of the parental influence and media richness, no significant gender differences were found. It is concluded that parental influence has a greater effect on students’ gender stereotyping behavior and career choices.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mehdi Hassan
- College of Public Administration, Nanjing Agricultural University, Nanjing, China
| | - Yingzi Luo
- College of Public Administration, Nanjing Agricultural University, Nanjing, China
- *Correspondence: Yingzi Luo,
| | - Jianxiu Gu
- College of Public Administration, Nanjing Agricultural University, Nanjing, China
| | - Iqra Mushtaque
- Department of Psychology, Bahauddin Zakariya University, Multan, Pakistan
| | - Muhammad Rizwan
- Department of Sociology, Nanjing Normal Univeristy, Nanjing, China
| |
Collapse
|
100
|
Samantha Green vs. Peter Stevens: Reversed Gender Stereotype Threat in Online Chess. EDUCATION SCIENCES 2022. [DOI: 10.3390/educsci12070433] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
Previous studies have shown that exposure to gender stereotypes has a detrimental impact on women’s performance. In chess, it has been demonstrated that the performance level of women is negatively influenced when they are exposed to negative stereotypes about their ability to play chess. However, it is still largely unclear whether the influence of a negative stereotype of women’s ability to play chess is only limited to their level of performance, or whether it could also affect their opponent’s performance. The present study investigated this reversed stereotype threat in online chess playing an unrated game. It was expected that a chess player’s performance would be influenced by the gender of their opponent. However, the participants’ online opponent was neither a female nor male chess player, but rather, unknown to the participants, it was a computer program that either played with a male or female nickname. The results showed that participants who played against a female nickname played less well, lost more games, and made more mistakes and blunders than participants who played against a male nickname. In sum, findings indicate that, in chess, the influence of a gender stereotype is not limited to the group the stereotype is targeted at, but also reduces the performance of the opponent’s level of play, leading to a reversed stereotype threat.
Collapse
|