301
|
Liao X, Wang K, Lin K, Chan RCK, Zhang X. Neural Temporal Dynamics of Facial Emotion Processing: Age Effects and Relationship to Cognitive Function. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1110. [PMID: 28713312 PMCID: PMC5492800 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01110] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2017] [Accepted: 06/15/2017] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
This study used event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate the effects of age on neural temporal dynamics of processing task-relevant facial expressions and their relationship to cognitive functions. Negative (sad, afraid, angry, and disgusted), positive (happy), and neutral faces were presented to 30 older and 31 young participants who performed a facial emotion categorization task. Behavioral and ERP indices of facial emotion processing were analyzed. An enhanced N170 for negative faces, in addition to intact right-hemispheric N170 for positive faces, was observed in older adults relative to their younger counterparts. Moreover, older adults demonstrated an attenuated within-group N170 laterality effect for neutral faces, while younger adults showed the opposite pattern. Furthermore, older adults exhibited sustained temporo-occipital negativity deflection over the time range of 200–500 ms post-stimulus, while young adults showed posterior positivity and subsequent emotion-specific frontal negativity deflections. In older adults, decreased accuracy for labeling negative faces was positively correlated with Montreal Cognitive Assessment Scores, and accuracy for labeling neutral faces was negatively correlated with age. These findings suggest that older people may exert more effort in structural encoding for negative faces and there are different response patterns for the categorization of different facial emotions. Cognitive functioning may be related to facial emotion categorization deficits observed in older adults. This may not be attributable to positivity effects: it may represent a selective deficit for the processing of negative facial expressions in older adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiaoyan Liao
- Department of Psychology, School of Public Health, Southern Medical UniversityGuangzhou, China.,Nanfang Hospital, Southern Medical UniversityGuangzhou, China
| | - Kui Wang
- Neuropsychology and Applied Cognitive Neurosciences Laboratory, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China.,CAS Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China
| | - Kai Lin
- Nanfang Hospital, Southern Medical UniversityGuangzhou, China
| | - Raymond C K Chan
- Neuropsychology and Applied Cognitive Neurosciences Laboratory, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China.,CAS Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China
| | - Xiaoyuan Zhang
- Department of Psychology, School of Public Health, Southern Medical UniversityGuangzhou, China
| |
Collapse
|
302
|
Kalampokini S, Lyros E, Luley M, Schöpe J, Spiegel J, Bürmann J, Dillmann U, Fassbender K, Unger MM. Facial emotion recognition in Parkinson's disease: Association with age and olfaction. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2017. [PMID: 28637374 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2017.1341470] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The ability to recognize facial emotion expressions has been reported to be impaired in Parkinson's disease (PD), yet previous studies showed inconsistent findings. The aim of this study was to further investigate facial emotion recognition (FER) in PD patients and its association with demographic and clinical parameters (including motor and nonmotor symptoms). METHOD Thirty-four nondemented PD patients and 24 age- and sex-matched healthy controls (HC) underwent clinical neurological and neuropsychological assessment, standardized olfactory testing with Sniffin' Sticks, and the Ekman 60 Faces Emotion Recognition Test. RESULTS PD patients had a significantly lower score on the total FER task than HC (p = .006), even after controlling for the potential confounding factors depression and apathy. The PD group had a specific impairment in the recognition of surprise (p = .007). The recognition of anger approached statistical significance (p = .07). Increasing chronological age and age at disease onset were associated with worse performance on the FER task in PD patients. Olfactory function along with PD diagnosis predicted worse FER performance within all study participants. CONCLUSION Facial emotion recognition and especially the recognition of surprise are significantly impaired in PD patients compared with age- and sex-matched HC. The association of FER with age and olfactory function is endorsed by common structures that undergo neurodegeneration in PD. The relevance of FER in social interaction stresses the clinical relevance and the need for further investigation in this field. Future studies should also determine whether impaired FER is already present in premotor stages of PD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Kalampokini
- a Department of Neurology , University Hospital of Saarland , Homburg , Germany
| | - E Lyros
- a Department of Neurology , University Hospital of Saarland , Homburg , Germany
| | - M Luley
- a Department of Neurology , University Hospital of Saarland , Homburg , Germany
| | - J Schöpe
- b Institute for Medical Biometry, Epidemiology and Medical Informatics , Saarland University , Homburg , Germany
| | - J Spiegel
- a Department of Neurology , University Hospital of Saarland , Homburg , Germany
| | - J Bürmann
- a Department of Neurology , University Hospital of Saarland , Homburg , Germany
| | - U Dillmann
- a Department of Neurology , University Hospital of Saarland , Homburg , Germany
| | - K Fassbender
- a Department of Neurology , University Hospital of Saarland , Homburg , Germany
| | - M M Unger
- a Department of Neurology , University Hospital of Saarland , Homburg , Germany
| |
Collapse
|
303
|
Joussain P, Ferdenzi C, Djordjevic J, Bensafi M. Relationship Between Psychophysiological Responses to Aversive Odors and Nutritional Status During Normal Aging. Chem Senses 2017; 42:465-472. [DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjx027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|
304
|
Everaerd D, Klumpers F, Oude Voshaar R, Fernández G, Tendolkar I. Acute Stress Enhances Emotional Face Processing in the Aging Brain. BIOLOGICAL PSYCHIATRY: COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROIMAGING 2017; 2:591-598. [PMID: 29560910 DOI: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2017] [Revised: 04/19/2017] [Accepted: 05/05/2017] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Healthy aging has been associated with stable emotional well-being and attenuated brain responses to negative stimuli. At the same time, depressive symptoms are common in older adults. The neural mechanisms behind this paradox remain to be clarified. We hypothesized that acute stress could alter emotion processing in healthy aging brain and constitute a pathway to vulnerability. METHODS Using a randomized, controlled crossover design, we explored the influence of acute stress on brain responses to happy and fearful facial expressions in 25 older adults (60-75 years of age) and 25 young (18-30 years of age) control subjects. Groups were matched on trait anxiety and education. Subjects underwent two separate functional magnetic resonance imaging sessions involving acute stress or a control procedure. RESULTS Affective and physiological responses to the stressor were similar between the two age groups. On a whole-brain level, we revealed a significant age by stress interaction in the fusiform gyrus, indicating a selective enhancement of neural activity with stress in elderly subjects only. When specifically aiming analysis at the amygdala, we found the same stress-related increase in activity in elderly subjects only. Modulation of amygdala reactivity due to stress correlated with trait conscientiousness in elderly subjects exclusively. CONCLUSIONS Compared with younger adults, healthy older adults showed increased responsivity of brain regions involved in face and emotion processing while stressed. These findings suggest that increased reactivity of this neural circuitry after acute stress may constitute one mechanism by which emotional well-being during healthy aging could rapidly change into heightened vulnerability for affective disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daphne Everaerd
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Department of Psychiatry, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| | - Floris Klumpers
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Richard Oude Voshaar
- Department of Psychiatry, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; University Center of Psychiatry & Interdisciplinary Center of Psychiatric Epidemiology, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Guillén Fernández
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Indira Tendolkar
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Department of Psychiatry, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Essen, Essen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
305
|
Ferreira CD, Torro-Alves N. Reconhecimento de Emoções Faciais no Envelhecimento: Uma Revisão Sistemática. UNIVERSITAS PSYCHOLOGICA 2017. [DOI: 10.11144/javeriana.upsy15-5.refe] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
No processo de envelhecimento, alterações na percepção e na cognição podem gerar prejuízos no reconhecimento de emoções faciais. No presente trabalho, foi realizada uma revisão sistemática, de acordo com as diretrizes do PRISMA, de estudos recentes que avaliaram a percepção e o reconhecimento facial de emoções em idosos sem patologias. As bases eletrônicas de dados pesquisadas foram: MEDLINE, PsycoINFO e Web of Science, sendo selecionados 22 artigos publicados entre 2009 e 2016. De um modo geral, verificou-se que os idosos apresentaram um declínio no reconhecimento de emoções, principalmente para as emoções negativas. Tais resultados podem ser explicados tanto pela teoria estrutural, quanto pela teoria da seletividade socioemocional. Os resultados têm importantes implicações na medida em que sinalizam a relevância da avaliação cognitiva e do uso de estímulos mais ecológicos nas tarefas de reconhecimento emocional em idosos.
Collapse
|
306
|
Kim SM, Kwon YJ, Jung SY, Kim MJ, Cho YS, Kim HT, Nam KC, Kim H, Choi KH, Choi JS. Development of the Korean Facial Emotion Stimuli: Korea University Facial Expression Collection 2nd Edition. Front Psychol 2017; 8:769. [PMID: 28553255 PMCID: PMC5427125 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00769] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/28/2016] [Accepted: 04/26/2017] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Background: Developing valid emotional facial stimuli for specific ethnicities creates ample opportunities to investigate both the nature of emotional facial information processing in general and clinical populations as well as the underlying mechanisms of facial emotion processing within and across cultures. Given that most entries in emotional facial stimuli databases were developed with western samples, and given that very few of the eastern emotional facial stimuli sets were based strictly on the Ekman’s Facial Action Coding System, developing valid emotional facial stimuli of eastern samples remains a high priority. Aims: To develop and examine the psychometric properties of six basic emotional facial stimuli recruiting professional Korean actors and actresses based on the Ekman’s Facial Action Coding System for the Korea University Facial Expression Collection-Second Edition (KUFEC-II). Materials And Methods: Stimulus selection was done in two phases. First, researchers evaluated the clarity and intensity of each stimulus developed based on the Facial Action Coding System. Second, researchers selected a total of 399 stimuli from a total of 57 actors and actresses, which were then rated on accuracy, intensity, valence, and arousal by 75 independent raters. Conclusion: The hit rates between the targeted and rated expressions of the KUFEC-II were all above 80%, except for fear (50%) and disgust (63%). The KUFEC-II appears to be a valid emotional facial stimuli database, providing the largest set of emotional facial stimuli. The mean intensity score was 5.63 (out of 7), suggesting that the stimuli delivered the targeted emotions with great intensity. All positive expressions were rated as having a high positive valence, whereas all negative expressions were rated as having a high negative valence. The KUFEC II is expected to be widely used in various psychological studies on emotional facial expression. KUFEC-II stimuli can be obtained through contacting the corresponding authors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sun-Min Kim
- Department of Psychology, Korea UniversitySeoul, South Korea
| | - Ye-Jin Kwon
- Department of Psychology, Korea UniversitySeoul, South Korea
| | - Soo-Yun Jung
- Department of Psychology, Korea UniversitySeoul, South Korea
| | - Min-Ji Kim
- Department of Psychology, Korea UniversitySeoul, South Korea
| | - Yang Seok Cho
- Department of Psychology, Korea UniversitySeoul, South Korea
| | - Hyun Taek Kim
- Department of Psychology, Korea UniversitySeoul, South Korea
| | - Ki-Chun Nam
- Department of Psychology, Korea UniversitySeoul, South Korea
| | - Hackjin Kim
- Department of Psychology, Korea UniversitySeoul, South Korea
| | - Kee-Hong Choi
- Department of Psychology, Korea UniversitySeoul, South Korea
| | - June-Seek Choi
- Department of Psychology, Korea UniversitySeoul, South Korea
| |
Collapse
|
307
|
Oxytocin effects in schizophrenia: Reconciling mixed findings and moving forward. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017; 80:36-56. [PMID: 28506922 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.05.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 62] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/10/2016] [Revised: 05/06/2017] [Accepted: 05/09/2017] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
Schizophrenia is a severe mental illness that causes major functional impairment. Current pharmacologic treatments are inadequate, particularly for addressing negative and cognitive symptoms of the disorder. Oxytocin, a neuropeptide known to moderate social behaviors, has been investigated as a potential therapeutic for schizophrenia in recent years. Results have been decidedly mixed, leading to controversy regarding oxytocin's utility. In this review, we outline several considerations for interpreting the extant literature and propose a focused agenda for future work that builds on the most compelling findings regarding oxytocin effects in schizophrenia to date. Specifically, we examine underlying causes of heterogeneity in randomized clinical trials (RCTs) conducted thus far and highlight the complexity of the human oxytocin system. We then review evidence of oxytocin's effects on specific deficits in schizophrenia, arguing for further study using objective, precise outcome measures in order to determine whether oxytocin has the potential to improve functional impairment in schizophrenia.
Collapse
|
308
|
Abstract
Age, race, and sex are linked to social cognitive performance among healthy individuals, but whether similar effects are evident in schizophrenia is unknown. Data from 170 individuals with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and 98 healthy controls were used to examine relations between these demographic factors and performance across multiple domains and measures of social cognition. Sex was not related to performance on any domain, but older age was related to poorer emotion recognition from dynamic stimuli in both patients and controls. In patients, older age was also associated with better abilities to decipher hints. Both Caucasian patients and controls performed better than African American individuals on emotion recognition and mental state attribution tasks that use only Caucasian individuals as visual stimuli. Findings suggest rather limited influences of demographic factors but do demonstrate normative age and race effects among patients. Findings also highlight important methodological considerations for measurement of social cognition.
Collapse
|
309
|
Sullivan S, Campbell A, Hutton SB, Ruffman T. What's good for the goose is not good for the gander: Age and gender differences in scanning emotion faces. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2017; 72:441-447. [PMID: 25969472 DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbv033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2014] [Accepted: 01/13/2015] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Objectives Research indicates that older adults' (≥60 years) emotion recognition is worse than that of young adults, young and older men's emotion recognition is worse than that of young and older women (respectively), older adults' looking at mouths compared with eyes is greater than that of young adults. Nevertheless, previous research has not compared older men's and women's looking at emotion faces so the present study had two aims: (a) to examine whether the tendency to look at mouths is stronger amongst older men compared with older women and (b) to examine whether men's mouth looking correlates with better emotion recognition. Method We examined the emotion recognition abilities and spontaneous gaze patterns of young (n = 60) and older (n = 58) males and females as they labelled emotion faces. Results Older men spontaneously looked more to mouths than older women, and older men's looking at mouths correlated with their emotion recognition, whereas women's looking at eyes correlated with their emotion recognition. Discussion The findings are discussed in relation to a growing body of research suggesting both age and gender differences in response to emotional stimuli and the differential efficacy of mouth and eyes looking for men and women.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susan Sullivan
- School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, BN1 9RH, UK
| | - Anna Campbell
- Psychology Department, University of Otago, PO Box 56, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand
| | - Sam B Hutton
- School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, BN1 9RH, UK
| | - Ted Ruffman
- Psychology Department, University of Otago, PO Box 56, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
310
|
Madill M, Murray JE. Processing Distracting Non-face Emotional Images: No Evidence of an Age-Related Positivity Effect. Front Psychol 2017; 8:591. [PMID: 28450848 PMCID: PMC5389978 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00591] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2016] [Accepted: 03/30/2017] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Cognitive aging may be accompanied by increased prioritization of social and emotional goals that enhance positive experiences and emotional states. The socioemotional selectivity theory suggests this may be achieved by giving preference to positive information and avoiding or suppressing negative information. Although there is some evidence of a positivity bias in controlled attention tasks, it remains unclear whether a positivity bias extends to the processing of affective stimuli presented outside focused attention. In two experiments, we investigated age-related differences in the effects of to-be-ignored non-face affective images on target processing. In Experiment 1, 27 older (64-90 years) and 25 young adults (19-29 years) made speeded valence judgments about centrally presented positive or negative target images taken from the International Affective Picture System. To-be-ignored distractor images were presented above and below the target image and were either positive, negative, or neutral in valence. The distractors were considered task relevant because they shared emotional characteristics with the target stimuli. Both older and young adults responded slower to targets when distractor valence was incongruent with target valence relative to when distractors were neutral. Older adults responded faster to positive than to negative targets but did not show increased interference effects from positive distractors. In Experiment 2, affective distractors were task irrelevant as the target was a three-digit array and did not share emotional characteristics with the distractors. Twenty-six older (63-84 years) and 30 young adults (18-30 years) gave speeded responses on a digit disparity task while ignoring the affective distractors positioned in the periphery. Task performance in either age group was not influenced by the task-irrelevant affective images. In keeping with the socioemotional selectivity theory, these findings suggest that older adults preferentially process task-relevant positive non-face images but only when presented within the main focus of attention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark Madill
- Department of Psychology, University of OtagoDunedin, New Zealand
| | - Janice E Murray
- Department of Psychology, University of OtagoDunedin, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
311
|
Chaby L, Hupont I, Avril M, Luherne-du Boullay V, Chetouani M. Gaze Behavior Consistency among Older and Younger Adults When Looking at Emotional Faces. Front Psychol 2017; 8:548. [PMID: 28450841 PMCID: PMC5390044 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00548] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2017] [Accepted: 03/24/2017] [Indexed: 01/27/2023] Open
Abstract
The identification of non-verbal emotional signals, and especially of facial expressions, is essential for successful social communication among humans. Previous research has reported an age-related decline in facial emotion identification, and argued for socio-emotional or aging-brain model explanations. However, more perceptual differences in the gaze strategies that accompany facial emotional processing with advancing age have been under-explored yet. In this study, 22 young (22.2 years) and 22 older (70.4 years) adults were instructed to look at basic facial expressions while their gaze movements were recorded by an eye-tracker. Participants were then asked to identify each emotion, and the unbiased hit rate was applied as performance measure. Gaze data were first analyzed using traditional measures of fixations over two preferential regions of the face (upper and lower areas) for each emotion. Then, to better capture core gaze changes with advancing age, spatio-temporal gaze behaviors were deeper examined using data-driven analysis (dimension reduction, clustering). Results first confirmed that older adults performed worse than younger adults at identifying facial expressions, except for "joy" and "disgust," and this was accompanied by a gaze preference toward the lower-face. Interestingly, this phenomenon was maintained during the whole time course of stimulus presentation. More importantly, trials corresponding to older adults were more tightly clustered, suggesting that the gaze behavior patterns of older adults are more consistent than those of younger adults. This study demonstrates that, confronted to emotional faces, younger and older adults do not prioritize or ignore the same facial areas. Older adults mainly adopted a focused-gaze strategy, consisting in focusing only on the lower part of the face throughout the whole stimuli display time. This consistency may constitute a robust and distinctive "social signature" of emotional identification in aging. Younger adults, however, were more dispersed in terms of gaze behavior and used a more exploratory-gaze strategy, consisting in repeatedly visiting both facial areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laurence Chaby
- Institut de Psychologie, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Université Paris DescartesBoulogne-Billancourt, France
- Sorbonne Universités, Université Pierre et Marie Curie - Paris 06, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique UMR 7222Paris, France
| | - Isabelle Hupont
- Sorbonne Universités, Université Pierre et Marie Curie - Paris 06, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique UMR 7222Paris, France
| | - Marie Avril
- Sorbonne Universités, Université Pierre et Marie Curie - Paris 06, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique UMR 7222Paris, France
| | - Viviane Luherne-du Boullay
- Service de Neurochirurgie, Assistance Publique – Hôpitaux de Paris, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-SalpetrièreParis, France
| | - Mohamed Chetouani
- Sorbonne Universités, Université Pierre et Marie Curie - Paris 06, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique UMR 7222Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
312
|
Jalilifard A, Brigante Pizzolato E, Kafiul Islam M. Emotion classification using single-channel scalp-EEG recording. ANNUAL INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE OF THE IEEE ENGINEERING IN MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY SOCIETY. IEEE ENGINEERING IN MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY SOCIETY. ANNUAL INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE 2017; 2016:845-849. [PMID: 28268456 DOI: 10.1109/embc.2016.7590833] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
Several studies have found evidence for corticolimbic Theta electroencephalographic (EEG) oscillation in the neural processing of visual stimuli perceived as fear or threatening scene. Recent studies showed that neural oscillations' patterns in Theta, Alpha, Beta and Gamma sub-bands play a main role in brain's emotional processing. The main goal of this study is to classify two different emotional states by means of EEG data recorded through a single-electrode EEG headset. Nineteen young subjects participated in an EEG experiment while watching a video clip that evoked three emotional states: neutral, relaxation and scary. Following each video clip, participants were asked to report on their subjective affect by giving a score between 0 to 10. First, recorded EEG data were preprocessed by stationary wavelet transform (SWT) based denoising to remove artifacts. Afterward, the distribution of power in time-frequency space was obtained using short-time Fourier transform (STFT) and then, the mean value of energy was calculated for each EEG sub-band. Finally, 46 features, as the mean energy of frequency bands between 4 and 50 Hz, containing 689 instances - for each subject -were collected in order to classify the emotional states. Our experimental results show that EEG dynamics induced by horror and relaxing movies can be classified with average classification rate of 92% using support vector machine (SVM) classifier. We also compared the performance of SVM to K-nearest neighbors (K-NN). The results show that K-NN achieves a better classification rate by 94% accuracy. The findings of this work are expected to pave the way to a new horizon in neuroscience by proving the point that only single-channel EEG data carry enough information for emotion classification.
Collapse
|
313
|
Murphy J, Brewer R, Catmur C, Bird G. Interoception and psychopathology: A developmental neuroscience perspective. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2017; 23:45-56. [PMID: 28081519 PMCID: PMC6987654 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.12.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 210] [Impact Index Per Article: 26.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/16/2016] [Revised: 12/19/2016] [Accepted: 12/19/2016] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Interoception refers to the perception of the physiological condition of the body, including hunger, temperature, and heart rate. There is a growing appreciation that interoception is integral to higher-order cognition. Indeed, existing research indicates an association between low interoceptive sensitivity and alexithymia (a difficulty identifying one's own emotion), underscoring the link between bodily and emotional awareness. Despite this appreciation, the developmental trajectory of interoception across the lifespan remains under-researched, with clear gaps in our understanding. This qualitative review and opinion paper provides a brief overview of interoception, discussing its relevance for developmental psychopathology, and highlighting measurement issues, before surveying the available work on interoception across four stages of development: infancy, childhood, adolescence and late adulthood. Where gaps in the literature addressing the development of interoception exist, we draw upon the association between alexithymia and interoception, using alexithymia as a possible marker of atypical interoception. Evidence indicates that interoceptive ability varies across development, and that this variance correlates with established age-related changes in cognition and with risk periods for the development of psychopathology. We suggest a theory within which atypical interoception underlies the onset of psychopathology and risky behaviour in adolescence, and the decreased socio-emotional competence observed in late adulthood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer Murphy
- MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK.
| | - Rebecca Brewer
- MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK; School of Psychology, The University of East London, London, UK
| | - Caroline Catmur
- MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
| | - Geoffrey Bird
- MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK; Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, UCL, London, UK; Dept of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
314
|
Jin Y, Mao Z, Ling Z, Xu X, Zhang Z, Yu X. Altered emotional recognition and expression in patients with Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2017; 13:2891-2902. [PMID: 29225467 PMCID: PMC5708195 DOI: 10.2147/ndt.s149227] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/25/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Parkinson's disease (PD) patients exhibit deficits in emotional recognition and expression abilities, including emotional faces and voices. The aim of this study was to explore emotional processing in pre-deep brain stimulation (pre-DBS) PD patients using two sensory modalities (visual and auditory). METHODS Fifteen PD patients who needed DBS surgery and 15 healthy, age- and gender-matched controls were recruited as participants. All participants were assessed by the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces database 50 Faces Recognition test. Vocal recognition was evaluated by the Montreal Affective Voices database 50 Voices Recognition test. For emotional facial expression, the participants were asked to imitate five basic emotions (neutral, happiness, anger, fear, and sadness). The subjects were required to express nonverbal vocalizations of the five basic emotions. Fifteen Chinese native speakers were recruited as decoders. We recorded the accuracy of the responses, reaction time, and confidence level. RESULTS For emotional recognition and expression, the PD group scored lower on both facial and vocal emotional processing than did the healthy control group. There were significant differences between the two groups in both reaction time and confidence level. A significant relationship was also found between emotional recognition and emotional expression when considering all participants between the two groups together. CONCLUSION The PD group exhibited poorer performance on both the recognition and expression tasks. Facial emotion deficits and vocal emotion abnormalities were associated with each other. In addition, our data allow us to speculate that emotional recognition and expression may share a common system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yazhou Jin
- Department of Neurosurgery, People's Liberation Army General Hospital, Beijing, People's Republic of China
| | - Zhiqi Mao
- Department of Neurosurgery, People's Liberation Army General Hospital, Beijing, People's Republic of China
| | - Zhipei Ling
- Department of Neurosurgery, People's Liberation Army General Hospital, Beijing, People's Republic of China
| | - Xin Xu
- Department of Neurosurgery, People's Liberation Army General Hospital, Beijing, People's Republic of China
| | - Zhiyuan Zhang
- Department of Neurosurgery, People's Liberation Army General Hospital, Beijing, People's Republic of China
| | - Xinguang Yu
- Department of Neurosurgery, People's Liberation Army General Hospital, Beijing, People's Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
315
|
Gillmeister H, Bowling N, Rigato S, Banissy MJ. Inter-Individual Differences in Vicarious Tactile Perception: a View Across the Lifespan in Typical and Atypical Populations. Multisens Res 2017; 30:485-508. [DOI: 10.1163/22134808-00002543] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2016] [Accepted: 12/01/2016] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
Touch is our most interpersonal sense, and so it stands to reason that we represent not only our own bodily experiences, but also those felt by others. This review will summarise brain and behavioural research on vicarious tactile perception (mirror touch). Specifically, we will focus on vicarious touch across the lifespan in typical and atypical groups, and will identify the knowledge gaps that are in urgent need of filling by examining what is known about how individuals differ within and between typical and atypical groups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Helge Gillmeister
- Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester, CO4 3SQ, UK
| | - Natalie Bowling
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, New Cross, London, SE14 6NW, UK
| | - Silvia Rigato
- Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester, CO4 3SQ, UK
| | - Michael J. Banissy
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, New Cross, London, SE14 6NW, UK
| |
Collapse
|
316
|
Ziaei M, Salami A, Persson J. Age-related alterations in functional connectivity patterns during working memory encoding of emotional items. Neuropsychologia 2017; 94:1-12. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2016] [Revised: 11/07/2016] [Accepted: 11/15/2016] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
|
317
|
Morin O, Acerbi A. Birth of the cool: a two-centuries decline in emotional expression in Anglophone fiction. Cogn Emot 2016; 31:1663-1675. [PMID: 27910735 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1260528] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
The presence of emotional words and content in stories has been shown to enhance a story's memorability, and its cultural success. Yet, recent cultural trends run in the opposite direction. Using the Google Books corpus, coupled with two metadata-rich corpora of Anglophone fiction books, we show a decrease in emotionality in English-speaking literature starting plausibly in the nineteenth century. We show that this decrease cannot be explained by changes unrelated to emotionality (such as demographic dynamics concerning age or gender balance, changes in vocabulary richness, or changes in the prevalence of literary genres), and that, in our three corpora, the decrease is driven almost entirely by a decline in the proportion of positive emotion-related words, while the frequency of negative emotion-related words shows little if any decline. Consistently with previous studies, we also find a link between ageing and negative emotionality at the individual level.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Olivier Morin
- a Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History , Jena , Germany
| | - Alberto Acerbi
- b School of Innovation Sciences , Eindhoven University of Technology , Eindhoven , The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
318
|
Ziaei M, Burianová H, von Hippel W, Ebner NC, Phillips LH, Henry JD. The impact of aging on the neural networks involved in gaze and emotional processing. Neurobiol Aging 2016; 48:182-194. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.08.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2016] [Revised: 08/21/2016] [Accepted: 08/29/2016] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
319
|
Visser-Keizer AC, Westerhof-Evers HJ, Gerritsen MJJ, van der Naalt J, Spikman JM. To Fear Is to Gain? The Role of Fear Recognition in Risky Decision Making in TBI Patients and Healthy Controls. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0166995. [PMID: 27870900 PMCID: PMC5117759 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166995] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/08/2016] [Accepted: 11/07/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Fear is an important emotional reaction that guides decision making in situations of ambiguity or uncertainty. Both recognition of facial expressions of fear and decision making ability can be impaired after traumatic brain injury (TBI), in particular when the frontal lobe is damaged. So far, it has not been investigated how recognition of fear influences risk behavior in healthy subjects and TBI patients. The ability to recognize fear is thought to be related to the ability to experience fear and to use it as a warning signal to guide decision making. We hypothesized that a better ability to recognize fear would be related to a better regulation of risk behavior, with healthy controls outperforming TBI patients. To investigate this, 59 healthy subjects and 49 TBI patients were assessed with a test for emotion recognition (Facial Expression of Emotion: Stimuli and Tests) and a gambling task (Iowa Gambling Task (IGT)). The results showed that, regardless of post traumatic amnesia duration or the presence of frontal lesions, patients were more impaired than healthy controls on both fear recognition and decision making. In both groups, a significant relationship was found between better fear recognition, the development of an advantageous strategy across the IGT and less risk behavior in the last blocks of the IGT. Educational level moderated this relationship in the final block of the IGT. This study has important clinical implications, indicating that impaired decision making and risk behavior after TBI can be preceded by deficits in the processing of fear.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Annemarie C. Visser-Keizer
- University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Neurology, Groningen, the Netherlands
- * E-mail:
| | - Herma J. Westerhof-Evers
- University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Center for Rehabilitation, Groningen, the Netherlands
- University of Groningen, Department of Clinical and Developmental Neuropsychology, Groningen, the Netherlands
| | - Marleen J. J. Gerritsen
- University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Neurology, Groningen, the Netherlands
| | - Joukje van der Naalt
- University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Neurology, Groningen, the Netherlands
| | - Jacoba M. Spikman
- University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Neurology, Groningen, the Netherlands
- University of Groningen, Department of Clinical and Developmental Neuropsychology, Groningen, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
320
|
Abstract
BACKGROUND/STUDY CONTEXT In a variety of collaborative circumstances, participants must adopt the perspective of a partner and establish a shared mental representation that helps mediate common understanding. This process is referred to as social coordination. Here, the authors investigate the effect of aging on social coordination and consider separately the component processes related to perspective-taking and working memory. METHODS Twelve young adults and 14 older adults completed an experimental, language-based coordination task. Subjects were asked to describe a scene with sufficient detail so that a conversational partner could identify a target object in the context of other, competing objects that shared a variable number of features. Trials varied in the information available to the partner (perspective-taking demand) and in the number of competing objects present in the scene (working memory demand). Responses were scored according to adjective use. RESULTS Results indicated that social coordination performance decreases with age. Whereas young adults performed close to ceiling, older adults were only precise in 49.70% of trials. In analyses examining perspective-taking conditions with no competitors, older adults were consistently impaired relative to young adults; in analyses examining the number of competitors during the simplest perspective-taking condition, both older and younger adults became more impaired with increasing numbers of competitors. CONCLUSION The experimental data suggest that social coordination decreases with age, which may affect communicative efficacy. Older adults' tendency to provide insufficient responses suggests a limitation in perspective-taking, and the pattern of decline in common ground performance with increasing competitors suggests that this is independent of working memory decline. In sum, our results suggest that social coordination deficits in aging may be multifactorial.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Meghan L Healey
- a Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, Department of Neurology , University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania and Neuroscience Graduate Group, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine , Philadelphia , Pennsylvania
| | - Murray Grossman
- a Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, Department of Neurology , University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania and Neuroscience Graduate Group, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine , Philadelphia , Pennsylvania
| |
Collapse
|
321
|
Sava AA, Krolak-Salmon P, Delphin-Combe F, Cloarec M, Chainay H. Memory for faces with emotional expressions in Alzheimer's disease and healthy older participants: positivity effect is not only due to familiarity. AGING NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND COGNITION 2016; 24:1-28. [PMID: 26873302 DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2016.1143444] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
Young individuals better memorize initially seen faces with emotional rather than neutral expressions. Healthy older participants and Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients show better memory for faces with positive expressions. The socioemotional selectivity theory postulates that this positivity effect in memory reflects a general age-related preference for positive stimuli, subserving emotion regulation. Another explanation might be that older participants use compensatory strategies, often considering happy faces as previously seen. The question about the existence of this effect in tasks not permitting such compensatory strategies is still open. Thus, we compared the performance of healthy participants and AD patients for positive, neutral, and negative faces in such tasks. Healthy older participants and AD patients showed a positivity effect in memory, but there was no difference between emotional and neutral faces in young participants. Our results suggest that the positivity effect in memory is not entirely due to the sense of familiarity for smiling faces.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alina-Alexandra Sava
- a Institut de Psychologie, Laboratoire d'Etude de Mécanismes Cognitifs (EMC) , Université Lumière Lyon 2 , Lyon , France
| | - Pierre Krolak-Salmon
- b INSERM U1028 - CNRS UMR5292 , Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon , Bron , France.,c Hospices civils de Lyon, CM2R , Hôpital gériatrique des Charpennes , Villeurbane , France
| | - Floriane Delphin-Combe
- c Hospices civils de Lyon, CM2R , Hôpital gériatrique des Charpennes , Villeurbane , France
| | - Morgane Cloarec
- a Institut de Psychologie, Laboratoire d'Etude de Mécanismes Cognitifs (EMC) , Université Lumière Lyon 2 , Lyon , France
| | - Hanna Chainay
- a Institut de Psychologie, Laboratoire d'Etude de Mécanismes Cognitifs (EMC) , Université Lumière Lyon 2 , Lyon , France
| |
Collapse
|
322
|
Franklin RG, Zebrowitz LA. Aging-Related Changes in Decoding Negative Complex Mental States from Faces. Exp Aging Res 2016; 42:471-478. [PMID: 27749208 DOI: 10.1080/0361073x.2016.1224667] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Background/Study Context: Many studies have found age-related declines in emotion recognition, with older adult (OA) deficits strongest for negative emotions. Some evidence suggests that OA also show worse performance in decoding complex mental states. However, no research has investigated whether those deficits are stronger for negative states. METHODS The authors investigated OA (ages 65-93) and younger adult (YA; ages 18-22) performance on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RME), a well-validated measure of the ability to decode complex mental states from faces. RESULTS The authors replicated findings showing OA deficits in this task. Using a multilevel logistic model, the authors found that the poorer performance of OA was due to worse performance on items for which a negative state was the correct answer. When analyzing each age group separately, OA scored worse on negative than positive items, whereas YA performance did not vary as a function of item valence. These age differences on the RME could not be explained by differences in lower-level visual function. CONCLUSION These findings show that previously documented OA deficits in perceiving basic negative emotional expressions are also present in reading complex mental states.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert G Franklin
- a Department of Behavioral Sciences , Anderson University , Anderson , South Carolina , USA
| | - Leslie A Zebrowitz
- b Department of Psychology , Brandeis University , Waltham , Massachusetts , USA
| |
Collapse
|
323
|
Rana M, Varan AQ, Davoudi A, Cohen RA, Sitaram R, Ebner NC. Real-Time fMRI in Neuroscience Research and Its Use in Studying the Aging Brain. Front Aging Neurosci 2016; 8:239. [PMID: 27803662 PMCID: PMC5067937 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00239] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2016] [Accepted: 09/27/2016] [Indexed: 02/05/2023] Open
Abstract
Cognitive decline is a major concern in the aging population. It is normative to experience some deterioration in cognitive abilities with advanced age such as related to memory performance, attention distraction to interference, task switching, and processing speed. However, intact cognitive functioning in old age is important for leading an independent day-to-day life. Thus, studying ways to counteract or delay the onset of cognitive decline in aging is crucial. The literature offers various explanations for the decline in cognitive performance in aging; among those are age-related gray and white matter atrophy, synaptic degeneration, blood flow reduction, neurochemical alterations, and change in connectivity patterns with advanced age. An emerging literature on neurofeedback and Brain Computer Interface (BCI) reports exciting results supporting the benefits of volitional modulation of brain activity on cognition and behavior. Neurofeedback studies based on real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging (rtfMRI) have shown behavioral changes in schizophrenia and behavioral benefits in nicotine addiction. This article integrates research on cognitive and brain aging with evidence of brain and behavioral modification due to rtfMRI neurofeedback. We offer a state-of-the-art description of the rtfMRI technique with an eye towards its application in aging. We present preliminary results of a feasibility study exploring the possibility of using rtfMRI to train older adults to volitionally control brain activity. Based on these first findings, we discuss possible implementations of rtfMRI neurofeedback as a novel technique to study and alleviate cognitive decline in healthy and pathological aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mohit Rana
- Department of Psychiatry and Division of Neuroscience, School of Medicine, Pontificia Universidad Católica de ChileSantiago, Chile; Laboratory for Brain-Machine Interfaces and Neuromodulation, Pontificia Universidad Católica de ChileSantiago, Chile
| | - Andrew Q Varan
- Department of Psychology, University of Florida Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Anis Davoudi
- J. Crayton Pruitt Family Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Florida Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Ronald A Cohen
- Center for Cognitive Aging and Memory, Institute on Aging, University of FloridaGainesville, FL, USA; Department of Aging and Geriatric Research, College of Medicine, University of FloridaGainesville, FL, USA
| | - Ranganatha Sitaram
- Department of Psychiatry and Division of Neuroscience, School of Medicine, Pontificia Universidad Católica de ChileSantiago, Chile; Laboratory for Brain-Machine Interfaces and Neuromodulation, Pontificia Universidad Católica de ChileSantiago, Chile; Institute for Biological and Medical Engineering, Schools of Engineering, Biology and Medicine, Pontificia Universidad Católica de ChileSantiago, Chile
| | - Natalie C Ebner
- Department of Psychology, University of FloridaGainesville, FL, USA; Center for Cognitive Aging and Memory, Institute on Aging, University of FloridaGainesville, FL, USA; Department of Aging and Geriatric Research, College of Medicine, University of FloridaGainesville, FL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
324
|
Khalaf A, Karim H, Berkout OV, Andreescu C, Tudorascu D, Reynolds CF, Aizenstein H. Altered Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Markers of Affective Processing During Treatment of Late-Life Depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2016; 24:791-801. [PMID: 27364483 PMCID: PMC5026904 DOI: 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.03.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2015] [Revised: 03/25/2016] [Accepted: 03/30/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE This study investigated neural substrate changes in affective processing among late-life depression (LLD) patients undergoing antidepressant treatment and determined if these changes correlated with remission status. METHODS Thirty-three LLD patients were enrolled in a 12-week venlafaxine treatment course. During treatment functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scans, paired with an affective task that assessed emotional reactivity and regulation, were performed on days 1, 2, 3, and 7 and at week 12. Following treatment patients were classified as remitters or non-remitters. A voxel-wise two-way repeated-measures ANOVA was performed to assess the fMRI data at a significance level of α = 0.05, corrected. RESULTS The emotional reactivity contrast demonstrated a significant interaction between remission status and scan time in the right middle temporal gyrus (MTG) (F = 24.1, df = 1,112, k = 102). Further analysis showed increased emotional reactivity-induced activity among non-remitters, and decreased activity among remitters, which significantly differed from baseline at day 7 (95% CI: 0.027, 0.540; Cohen's d = -1.35) and week 12 (95% CI: -0.171, -0.052; Cohen's d = 0.68), respectively. No significant interaction was observed with the emotional regulation contrast, but multiple regions had significant main effects of scan time, including the cuneus, occipital lobe, insula, lingual gyrus, posterior cingulate cortex, and MTG. CONCLUSIONS During treatment of LLD patients, affective processing-induced activity in the right MTG shows changes based on remission status. This alteration becomes evident early during the course of treatment, suggesting that antidepressant pharmacotherapy may acutely affect the neural basis of emotional reactivity in a differential manner that is relevant to illness remission.
Collapse
|
325
|
Erel H, Levy DA. Orienting of visual attention in aging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016; 69:357-80. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/27/2015] [Revised: 08/01/2016] [Accepted: 08/06/2016] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
326
|
Shiroma PR, Thuras P, Johns B, Lim KO. Facial recognition of happiness among older adults with active and remitted major depression. Psychiatry Res 2016; 243:287-91. [PMID: 27428081 DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.06.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/21/2015] [Revised: 06/13/2016] [Accepted: 06/14/2016] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
Biased emotion processing in depression might be a trait characteristic independent of mood improvement and a vulnerable factor to develop further depressive episodes. This phenomenon of among older adults with depression has not been adequately examined. In a 2-year cross-sectional study, 59 older patients with either active or remitted major depression, or never-depressed, completed a facial emotion recognition task (FERT) to probe perceptual bias of happiness. The results showed that depressed patients, compared with never depressed subjects, had a significant lower sensitivity to identify happiness particularly at moderate intensity of facial stimuli. Patients in remission from a previous major depressive episode but with none or minimal symptoms had similar sensitivity rate to identify happy facial expressions as compared to patients with an active depressive episode. Further studies would be necessary to confirm whether recognition of happy expression reflects a persistent perceptual bias of major depression in older adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paulo R Shiroma
- Mental Health Service Line, Minneapolis VA Medical Center, Minneapolis, MN, USA; Department of Psychiatry, University of Minnesota Medical School, Minneapolis, MN, USA.
| | - Paul Thuras
- Mental Health Service Line, Minneapolis VA Medical Center, Minneapolis, MN, USA; Department of Psychiatry, University of Minnesota Medical School, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Brian Johns
- Department of Psychiatry, North Memorial Medical Center, Minneapolis, MN, USA; Department of Psychiatry, University of Minnesota Medical School, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Kelvin O Lim
- Mental Health Service Line, Minneapolis VA Medical Center, Minneapolis, MN, USA; Department of Psychiatry, University of Minnesota Medical School, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
327
|
Murry MWE, Isaacowitz DM. Age differences in emotion perception. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT 2016. [DOI: 10.1177/0165025416667493] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Older adults tend to have lower emotion-perception accuracy compared to younger adults. Previous studies have centered on individual characteristics, including cognitive decline and positive attentional preferences, as possible mechanisms underlying these age differences in emotion perception; however, thus far, no perceiver-focused factor has accounted for the age differences. The present study focuses on perceived social-context factors and uses the Social Input Model as the framework for investigating the relation between the expressivity of the social environment and emotion-perception accuracy in younger and older adults. Younger ( n = 32) and older adults ( n = 29) reported on the make-up of their social circles and the expressivity of their three closest social partners and then completed a static facial emotion-perception task. Older adults reported greater positive and negative expressivity in their social partners compared to younger adults. Moreover, older adults were marginally less accurate than younger adults when perceiving emotions. Positive expressivity of the social partners predicted lower emotion-perception accuracy in younger but not older adults. Our findings mark the first step to identifying possible characteristics of the social environment that may contribute to the age difference in emotion-perception accuracy.
Collapse
|
328
|
Kiiski HSM, Cullen B, Clavin SL, Newell FN. Perceptual and Social Attributes Underlining Age-Related Preferences for Faces. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:437. [PMID: 27630553 PMCID: PMC5005962 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00437] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/15/2015] [Accepted: 08/16/2016] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Although aesthetic preferences are known to be important in person perception and can play a significant role in everyday social decisions, the effect of the age of the observer on aesthetic preferences for faces of different ages has not yet been fully investigated. In the present study we investigated whether aesthetic preferences change with aging, with an age-related bias in favoring faces from one’s own age group. In addition, we examined the role of age on both the perceptual qualities and the social attributes of faces that may influence these aesthetic judgements. Both younger and older adult observers provided ratings to images of younger, middle-aged and older unfamiliar faces. As well as attractiveness, the rating dimensions included other perceptual (distinctiveness, familiarity) and social (competence, trustworthiness and dominance) factors. The results suggested a consistent aesthetic preference for youthful faces across all ages of the observers but, surprisingly, no evidence for an age-related bias in attractiveness ratings. Older adults tended to provide higher ratings of attractiveness, competence and trustworthiness to the unfamiliar faces, consistent with the positivity effect previously reported. We also tested whether perceptual factors such as face familiarity or distinctiveness affected aesthetic ratings. Only ratings of familiarity, but not distinctiveness, were positively associated with the attractiveness of the faces. Moreover, ratings of familiarity decreased with increasing age of the face. With regard to the social characteristics of the faces, we found that the age of the face negatively correlated with ratings of trustworthiness provided by all observers, but with the competence ratings of older observers only. Interestingly, older adults provided higher ratings of perceived competence and trustworthiness to younger than older faces. However, our results also suggest that higher attractiveness ratings, together with older aged faces, led to more positive evaluations of competence. The results are discussed within the context of an age-related decline in the differentiation of faces in memory. Our findings have important implications for a better understanding of age-related perceptual factors and cognitive determinants of social interactions with unfamiliar others across the adult lifespan.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hanni S M Kiiski
- Multisensory Cognition Research Group, Institute of Neuroscience and School of Psychology, Trinity College Dublin Dublin, Ireland
| | - Brendan Cullen
- Multisensory Cognition Research Group, Institute of Neuroscience and School of Psychology, Trinity College Dublin Dublin, Ireland
| | - Sarah L Clavin
- Multisensory Cognition Research Group, Institute of Neuroscience and School of Psychology, Trinity College Dublin Dublin, Ireland
| | - Fiona N Newell
- Multisensory Cognition Research Group, Institute of Neuroscience and School of Psychology, Trinity College Dublin Dublin, Ireland
| |
Collapse
|
329
|
Szymkowicz SM, Persson J, Lin T, Fischer H, Ebner NC. Hippocampal Brain Volume Is Associated with Faster Facial Emotion Identification in Older Adults: Preliminary Results. Front Aging Neurosci 2016; 8:203. [PMID: 27610082 PMCID: PMC4997967 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00203] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/28/2016] [Accepted: 08/08/2016] [Indexed: 01/16/2023] Open
Abstract
Quick correct identification of facial emotions is highly relevant for successful social interactions. Research suggests that older, compared to young, adults experience increased difficulty with face and emotion processing skills. While functional neuroimaging studies suggest age differences in neural processing of faces and emotions, evidence about age-associated structural brain changes and their involvement in face and emotion processing is scarce. Using structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), this study investigated the extent to which volumes of frontal and temporal brain structures were related to reaction time in accurate identification of facial emotions in 30 young and 30 older adults. Volumetric segmentation was performed using FreeSurfer and gray matter volumes from frontal and temporal regions were extracted. Analysis of covariances (ANCOVAs) models with response time (RT) as the dependent variable and age group and regional volume, and their interaction, as independent variables were conducted, controlling for total intracranial volume (ICV). Results indicated that, in older adults, larger hippocampal volumes were associated with faster correct facial emotion identification. These preliminary observations suggest that greater volume in brain regions associated with face and emotion processing contributes to improved facial emotion identification performance in aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah M Szymkowicz
- Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Jonas Persson
- Aging Research Center, Karolinska Institute Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Tian Lin
- Department of Psychology, University of Florida Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Håkan Fischer
- Department of Psychology, Stockholm University Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Natalie C Ebner
- Department of Psychology, University of FloridaGainesville, FL, USA; Department of Aging and Geriatric Research, University of FloridaGainesville, FL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
330
|
Schwartz R, Pell MD. When emotion and expression diverge: The social costs of Parkinson’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2016; 39:211-230. [DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2016.1216090] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
|
331
|
Argaud S, Delplanque S, Houvenaghel JF, Auffret M, Duprez J, Vérin M, Grandjean D, Sauleau P. Does Facial Amimia Impact the Recognition of Facial Emotions? An EMG Study in Parkinson's Disease. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0160329. [PMID: 27467393 PMCID: PMC4965153 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160329] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2016] [Accepted: 07/18/2016] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
According to embodied simulation theory, understanding other people’s emotions is fostered by facial mimicry. However, studies assessing the effect of facial mimicry on the recognition of emotion are still controversial. In Parkinson’s disease (PD), one of the most distinctive clinical features is facial amimia, a reduction in facial expressiveness, but patients also show emotional disturbances. The present study used the pathological model of PD to examine the role of facial mimicry on emotion recognition by investigating EMG responses in PD patients during a facial emotion recognition task (anger, joy, neutral). Our results evidenced a significant decrease in facial mimicry for joy in PD, essentially linked to the absence of reaction of the zygomaticus major and the orbicularis oculi muscles in response to happy avatars, whereas facial mimicry for expressions of anger was relatively preserved. We also confirmed that PD patients were less accurate in recognizing positive and neutral facial expressions and highlighted a beneficial effect of facial mimicry on the recognition of emotion. We thus provide additional arguments for embodied simulation theory suggesting that facial mimicry is a potential lever for therapeutic actions in PD even if it seems not to be necessarily required in recognizing emotion as such.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Soizic Argaud
- Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA4712), University of Rennes 1, Rennes, France
- Neuroscience of Emotion and Affective Dynamics laboratory, Department of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
- * E-mail:
| | - Sylvain Delplanque
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, Campus Biotech, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Jean-François Houvenaghel
- Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA4712), University of Rennes 1, Rennes, France
- Department of Neurology, Rennes University Hospital, Rennes, France
| | - Manon Auffret
- Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA4712), University of Rennes 1, Rennes, France
| | - Joan Duprez
- Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA4712), University of Rennes 1, Rennes, France
| | - Marc Vérin
- Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA4712), University of Rennes 1, Rennes, France
- Department of Neurology, Rennes University Hospital, Rennes, France
| | - Didier Grandjean
- Neuroscience of Emotion and Affective Dynamics laboratory, Department of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, Campus Biotech, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Paul Sauleau
- Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA4712), University of Rennes 1, Rennes, France
- Department of Neurophysiology, Rennes University Hospital, Rennes, France
| |
Collapse
|
332
|
Adams RB, Garrido CO, Albohn DN, Hess U, Kleck RE. What Facial Appearance Reveals Over Time: When Perceived Expressions in Neutral Faces Reveal Stable Emotion Dispositions. Front Psychol 2016; 7:986. [PMID: 27445944 PMCID: PMC4927908 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00986] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2015] [Accepted: 06/14/2016] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
It might seem a reasonable assumption that when we are not actively using our faces to express ourselves (i.e., when we display nonexpressive, or neutral faces), those around us will not be able to read our emotions. Herein, using a variety of expression-related ratings, we examined whether age-related changes in the face can accurately reveal one’s innermost affective dispositions. In each study, we found that expressive ratings of neutral facial displays predicted self-reported positive/negative dispositional affect, but only for elderly women, and only for positive affect. These findings meaningfully replicate and extend earlier work examining age-related emotion cues in the face of elderly women (Malatesta et al., 1987a). We discuss these findings in light of evidence that women are expected to, and do, smile more than men, and that the quality of their smiles predicts their life satisfaction. Although ratings of old male faces did not significantly predict self-reported affective dispositions, the trend was similar to that found for old female faces. A plausible explanation for this gender difference is that in the process of attenuating emotional expressions over their lifetimes, old men reveal less evidence of their total emotional experiences in their faces than do old women.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Reginald B Adams
- Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park PA, USA
| | - Carlos O Garrido
- Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park PA, USA
| | - Daniel N Albohn
- Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park PA, USA
| | - Ursula Hess
- Department of Psychology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin Berlin, Germany
| | - Robert E Kleck
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover NH, USA
| |
Collapse
|
333
|
Zhao MF, Zimmer HD, Shen X, Chen W, Fu X. Exploring the Cognitive Processes Causing the Age-Related Categorization Deficit in the Recognition of Facial Expressions. Exp Aging Res 2016; 42:348-64. [DOI: 10.1080/0361073x.2016.1191854] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
334
|
Pietschnig J, Schröder L, Ratheiser I, Kryspin-Exner I, Pflüger M, Moser D, Auff E, Pirker W, Pusswald G, Lehrner J. Facial emotion recognition and its relationship to cognition and depressive symptoms in patients with Parkinson's disease. Int Psychogeriatr 2016; 28:1165-79. [PMID: 26987816 DOI: 10.1017/s104161021600034x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Impairments in facial emotion recognition (FER) have been detected in patients with Parkinson disease (PD). Presently, we aim at assessing differences in emotion recognition performance in PD patient groups with and without mild forms of cognitive impairment (MCI) compared to healthy controls. METHODS Performance on a concise emotion recognition test battery (VERT-K) of three groups of 97 PD patients was compared with an age-equivalent sample of 168 healthy controls. Patients were categorized into groups according to two well-established classifications of MCI according to Petersen's (cognitively intact vs. amnestic MCI, aMCI, vs. non-amnestic MCI, non-aMCI) and Litvan's (cognitively intact vs. single-domain MCI, sMCI, vs. multi-domain MCI, mMCI) criteria. Patients and controls underwent individual assessments using a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery examining attention, executive functioning, language, and memory (Neuropsychological Test Battery Vienna, NTBV), the Beck Depression Inventory, and a measure of premorbid IQ (WST). RESULTS Cognitively intact PD patients and patients with MCI in PD (PD-MCI) showed significantly worse emotion recognition performance when compared to healthy controls. Between-groups effect sizes were substantial, showing non-trivial effects in all comparisons (Cohen's ds from 0.31 to 1.22). Moreover, emotion recognition performance was higher in women, positively associated with premorbid IQ and negatively associated with age. Depressive symptoms were not related to FER. CONCLUSIONS The present investigation yields further evidence for impaired FER in PD. Interestingly, our data suggest FER deficits even in cognitively intact PD patients indicating FER dysfunction prior to the development of overt cognitive dysfunction. Age showed a negative association whereas IQ showed a positive association with FER.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Pietschnig
- Department of Applied Psychology: Health, Development, Enhancement and Intervention,Faculty of Psychology,University of Vienna,Vienna,Austria
| | - L Schröder
- Department of Applied Psychology: Health, Development, Enhancement and Intervention,Faculty of Psychology,University of Vienna,Vienna,Austria
| | - I Ratheiser
- Department of Applied Psychology: Health, Development, Enhancement and Intervention,Faculty of Psychology,University of Vienna,Vienna,Austria
| | - I Kryspin-Exner
- Department of Applied Psychology: Health, Development, Enhancement and Intervention,Faculty of Psychology,University of Vienna,Vienna,Austria
| | - M Pflüger
- Department of Neurology,Medical University of Vienna,Vienna,Austria
| | - D Moser
- Department of Neurology,Medical University of Vienna,Vienna,Austria
| | - E Auff
- Department of Neurology,Medical University of Vienna,Vienna,Austria
| | - W Pirker
- Department of Neurology,Medical University of Vienna,Vienna,Austria
| | - G Pusswald
- Department of Neurology,Medical University of Vienna,Vienna,Austria
| | - J Lehrner
- Department of Neurology,Medical University of Vienna,Vienna,Austria
| |
Collapse
|
335
|
Feingold D, Hasson-Ohayon I, Laukka P, Vishne T, Dembinsky Y, Kravets S. Emotion recognition deficits among persons with schizophrenia: Beyond stimulus complexity level and presentation modality. Psychiatry Res 2016; 240:60-65. [PMID: 27085665 DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/02/2015] [Revised: 02/14/2016] [Accepted: 04/08/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Studies have shown that persons with schizophrenia have lower accuracy in emotion recognition compared to persons without schizophrenia. However, the impact of the complexity level of the stimuli or the modality of presentation has not been extensively addressed. Forty three persons with a diagnosis of schizophrenia and 43 healthy controls, matched for age and gender, were administered tests assessing emotion recognition from stimuli with low and high levels of complexity presented via visual, auditory and semantic channels. For both groups, recognition rates were higher for high-complexity stimuli compared to low-complexity stimuli. Additionally, both groups obtained higher recognition rates for visual and semantic stimuli than for auditory stimuli, but persons with schizophrenia obtained lower accuracy than persons in the control group for all presentation modalities. Persons diagnosed with schizophrenia did not present a level of complexity specific deficit or modality-specific deficit compared to healthy controls. Results suggest that emotion recognition deficits in schizophrenia are beyond level of complexity of stimuli and modality, and present a global difficulty in cognitive functioning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Feingold
- Department of Psychology, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel; Department of Psychiatry, Sheba Medical Center, Tel Hashomer, Israel.
| | | | - Petri Laukka
- Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden; Department of Social Sciences, Södertörn University, Huddinge, Sweden
| | | | - Yael Dembinsky
- Psychiatry Department, Sourasky Medical Center, Tel-Aviv, Israel
| | - Shlomo Kravets
- Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
336
|
Tskhay KO, Krendl AC, Rule NO. Age-Related Physical Changes Interfere With Judgments of Male Sexual Orientation From Faces. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2016; 42:1217-26. [PMID: 27340151 DOI: 10.1177/0146167216653585] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2014] [Accepted: 05/17/2016] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Although studies have shown that sexual orientation can be judged from faces, this research has not considered how age-related differences in perceivers or targets affect such judgments. In the current work, we evaluated whether accuracy differed among young adults (YA) and older adults (OA) for young and old men's faces by recruiting a sample of YA and OA in the lab, a community sample of sexual minority men, and a sample of online participants. We found that OA and YA judged sexual orientation with similar accuracy. Perceptions of gender atypicality mediated the difference in judging older and younger targets' sexual orientation. Although participants used positive affect to correctly discern sexual orientation regardless of target age, perceptions of masculinity were valid only for judgments of YA.
Collapse
|
337
|
Silver H, Bilker WB. Colour influences perception of facial emotions but this effect is impaired in healthy ageing and schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2016; 20:438-55. [PMID: 26395165 DOI: 10.1080/13546805.2015.1080157] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/27/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Social cognition is commonly assessed by identification of emotions in facial expressions. Presence of colour, a salient feature of stimuli, might influence emotional face perception. METHODS We administered 2 tests of facial emotion recognition, the Emotion Recognition Test (ER40) using colour pictures and the Penn Emotional Acuity Test using monochromatic pictures, to 37 young healthy, 39 old healthy and 37 schizophrenic men. RESULTS Among young healthy individuals recognition of emotions was more accurate and faster in colour than in monochromatic pictures. Compared to the younger group, older healthy individuals revealed impairment in identification of sad expressions in colour but not monochromatic pictures. Schizophrenia patients showed greater impairment in colour than monochromatic pictures of neutral and sad expressions and overall total score compared to both healthy groups. Patients showed significant correlations between cognitive impairment and perception of emotion in colour but not monochromatic pictures. CONCLUSIONS Colour enhances perception of general emotional clues and this contextual effect is impaired in healthy ageing and schizophrenia. The effects of colour need to be considered in interpreting and comparing studies of emotion perception. Coloured face stimuli may be more sensitive to emotion processing impairments but less selective for emotion-specific information than monochromatic stimuli. This may impact on their utility in early detection of impairments and investigations of underlying mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Henry Silver
- a Brain Behavior Laboratory , Sha'ar Menashe Mental Health Center , Mobile Post Hefer 37806, Israel.,b Rappaport Faculty of Medicine , Technion Institute of Technology , Haifa , Israel
| | - Warren B Bilker
- c Department of Biostatistics and Epidemiology , University of Pennsylvania , Philadelphia , PA , USA
| |
Collapse
|
338
|
Mikels JA, Shuster MM, Thai ST, Smith-Ray R, Waugh CE, Roth K, Keilly A, Stine-Morrow EAL. Messages that matter: Age differences in affective responses to framed health messages. Psychol Aging 2016; 31:409-14. [PMID: 27294720 PMCID: PMC4910631 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Age differences in responses to framed health messages-which can influence judgments and decisions-are critical to understand yet relatively unexplored. Age-related emotional shifts toward positivity would be expected to differentially impact the affective responses of older and younger adults to framed messages. In this study, we measured the subjective and physiological affective responses of older and younger adults to gain- and loss-framed exercise promotion messages. Relative to older adults, younger adults exhibited greater negative reactivity to loss-framed health messages. These results suggest that health message framing does matter, but it depends on the age of the message recipient. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
|
339
|
Schmidt J, Janse E, Scharenborg O. Perception of Emotion in Conversational Speech by Younger and Older Listeners. Front Psychol 2016; 7:781. [PMID: 27303340 PMCID: PMC4885861 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00781] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/22/2016] [Accepted: 05/09/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
This study investigated whether age and/or differences in hearing sensitivity influence the perception of the emotion dimensions arousal (calm vs. aroused) and valence (positive vs. negative attitude) in conversational speech. To that end, this study specifically focused on the relationship between participants' ratings of short affective utterances and the utterances' acoustic parameters (pitch, intensity, and articulation rate) known to be associated with the emotion dimensions arousal and valence. Stimuli consisted of short utterances taken from a corpus of conversational speech. In two rating tasks, younger and older adults either rated arousal or valence using a 5-point scale. Mean intensity was found to be the main cue participants used in the arousal task (i.e., higher mean intensity cueing higher levels of arousal) while mean F 0 was the main cue in the valence task (i.e., higher mean F 0 being interpreted as more negative). Even though there were no overall age group differences in arousal or valence ratings, compared to younger adults, older adults responded less strongly to mean intensity differences cueing arousal and responded more strongly to differences in mean F 0 cueing valence. Individual hearing sensitivity among the older adults did not modify the use of mean intensity as an arousal cue. However, individual hearing sensitivity generally affected valence ratings and modified the use of mean F 0. We conclude that age differences in the interpretation of mean F 0 as a cue for valence are likely due to age-related hearing loss, whereas age differences in rating arousal do not seem to be driven by hearing sensitivity differences between age groups (as measured by pure-tone audiometry).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juliane Schmidt
- Centre for Language Studies, Radboud UniversityNijmegen, Netherlands
- International Max Planck Research School for Language SciencesNijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Esther Janse
- Centre for Language Studies, Radboud UniversityNijmegen, Netherlands
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and BehaviourNijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Odette Scharenborg
- Centre for Language Studies, Radboud UniversityNijmegen, Netherlands
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and BehaviourNijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
340
|
Kensinger EA, Gutchess AH. Cognitive Aging in a Social and Affective Context: Advances Over the Past 50 Years. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2016; 72:61-70. [DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbw056] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2016] [Accepted: 04/27/2016] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|
341
|
Zebrowitz L, Ward N, Boshyan J, Gutchess A, Hadjikhani N. Dedifferentiated face processing in older adults is linked to lower resting state metabolic activity in fusiform face area. Brain Res 2016; 1644:22-31. [PMID: 27163722 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.05.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/11/2016] [Revised: 03/21/2016] [Accepted: 05/05/2016] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
We used multimodal brain imaging to examine possible mediators of age-related neural dedifferentiation (less specific neural activation) to different categories of stimuli that had been shown in previous research. Specifically, we examined resting blood flow and brain activation in areas involved in object, place and face perception. We observed lower activation, specificity, and resting blood flow for older adults (OA) than younger adults (YA) in the fusiform face area (FFA) but not in the other regions of interest. Mediation analyses further revealed that FFA resting state blood flow mediated age differences in FFA specificity, whereas age differences in visual and cognitive function and cortical thickness did not. Whole brain analyses also revealed more activated voxels for all categories in OA, as well as more frontal activation for faces but not for the other categories in OA than YA. Less FFA specificity coupled with more frontal activation when passively viewing faces suggest that OA have more difficulty recruiting specialized face processing mechanisms, and the lower FFA metabolic activity even when faces are not being processed suggests an OA deficiency in the neural substrate underlying face processing. Our data point to a detuning of face-selective mechanisms in older adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leslie Zebrowitz
- Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA 02453, USA
| | - Noreen Ward
- MGH/HST Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Harvard University, Boston, MA 02129 USA
| | - Jasmine Boshyan
- Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA 02453, USA
| | - Angela Gutchess
- Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA 02453, USA
| | - Nouchine Hadjikhani
- MGH/HST Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Harvard University, Boston, MA 02129 USA; Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Center, Gothenburg University, Sweden.
| |
Collapse
|
342
|
Stanley JT, Isaacowitz DM. Caring more and knowing more reduces age-related differences in emotion perception. Psychol Aging 2016; 30:383-395. [PMID: 26030775 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Traditional emotion perception tasks show that older adults are less accurate than are young adults at recognizing facial expressions of emotion. Recently, we proposed that socioemotional factors might explain why older adults seem impaired in lab tasks but less so in everyday life (Isaacowitz & Stanley, 2011). Thus, in the present research we empirically tested whether socioemotional factors such as motivation and familiarity can alter this pattern of age effects. In 1 task, accountability instructions eliminated age differences in the traditional emotion perception task. Using a novel emotion perception paradigm featuring spontaneous dynamic facial expressions of a familiar romantic partner versus a same-age stranger, we found that age differences in emotion perception accuracy were attenuated in the familiar partner condition, relative to the stranger condition. Taken together, the results suggest that both overall accuracy as well as specific patterns of age effects differ appreciably between traditional emotion perception tasks and emotion perception within a socioemotional context.
Collapse
|
343
|
Ruffman T, Zhang J, Taumoepeau M, Skeaff S. Your Way to a Better Theory of Mind: A Healthy Diet Relates to Better Faux Pas Recognition in Older Adults. Exp Aging Res 2016; 42:279-88. [DOI: 10.1080/0361073x.2016.1156974] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
344
|
Shou J, Ren L, Wang H, Yan F, Cao X, Wang H, Wang Z, Zhu S, Liu Y. Reliability and validity of 12-item Short-Form health survey (SF-12) for the health status of Chinese community elderly population in Xujiahui district of Shanghai. Aging Clin Exp Res 2016; 28:339-46. [PMID: 26142623 DOI: 10.1007/s40520-015-0401-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2015] [Accepted: 06/12/2015] [Indexed: 01/06/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The 12-item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-12) is the abridged practical version of SF-36. AIMS This cross-sectional study was aimed to assess the reliability and validity of SF-12 for the health status of Chinese community elderly population. METHODS The Chinese community elderly people in Xujiahui district of Shanghai were investigated. The internal consistency reliability was assessed using Cronbach's alpha and split-half reliability coefficients. Construct validity was analyzed using exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). Spearman's correlation coefficient (ρ) was used for the evaluation of criterion, convergent, and discriminant validity with Spearman's ρ ≥ 0.4 as satisfactory. Comparisons of the SF-12 summary scores among populations that differed in demographics were performed for discriminant validity. RESULTS Total 1343 individuals aged ≥60 and <85 years old (response rate: 91.3 %) were analyzed. The Cronbach's α value (0.910) and the split-half reliability coefficient (0.812) reflected satisfactory internal consistency reliability of SF-12. EFA extracted a two-factor model (physical and mental health). About 60.7 % of the total variance was explained by the two factors. CFA showed that the two-factor solution provided a good fit to the data. Good convergent validity and discriminant validity of SF-12 were proved by the correction analyses (Spearman's ρ > 0.4) and the comparisons of the SF-12 summary scores among populations (P < 0.05). SF-12 summary scores were significantly correlated with the SF-36 summary scores (Spearman's ρ > 0.4, P < 0.05). CONCLUSIONS In conclusion, SF-12 had satisfactory reliability and validity in measuring health status of Chinese community elderly population in Xujiahui district of Shanghai.
Collapse
|
345
|
Romero-Ferreiro MV, Aguado L, Rodriguez-Torresano J, Palomo T, Rodriguez-Jimenez R, Pedreira-Massa JL. Facial affect recognition in early and late-stage schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res 2016; 172:177-83. [PMID: 26874869 DOI: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.02.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2015] [Revised: 02/03/2016] [Accepted: 02/03/2016] [Indexed: 01/14/2023]
Abstract
Prior studies have shown deficits in social cognition and emotion perception in first-episode psychosis (FEP) and multi-episode schizophrenia (MES) patients. These studies compared patients at different stages of the illness with only a single control group which differed in age from at least one clinical group. The present study provides new evidence of a differential pattern of deficit in facial affect recognition in FEP and MES patients using a double age-matched control design. Compared to their controls, FEP patients only showed impaired recognition of fearful faces (p=.007). In contrast to this, the MES patients showed a more generalized deficit compared to their age-matched controls, with impaired recognition of angry, sad and fearful faces (ps<.01) and an increased misattribution of emotional meaning to neutral faces. PANSS scores of FEP patients on Depressed factor correlated positively with the accuracy to recognize fearful expressions (r=.473). For the MES group fear recognition correlated positively with negative PANSS factor (r=.498) and recognition of sad and neutral expressions was inversely correlated with disorganized PANSS factor (r=-.461 and r=-.541, respectively). These results provide evidence that a generalized impairment of affect recognition is observed in advanced-stage patients and is not characteristic of the early stages of schizophrenia. Moreover, the finding that anomalous attribution of emotional meaning to neutral faces is observed only in MES patients suggests that an increased attribution of salience to social stimuli is a characteristic of social cognition in advanced stages of the disorder.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Javier Rodriguez-Torresano
- Department of Psychiatry, Instituto de Investigación Hospital Universitario 12 de Octubre (i+12), Avda. de Córdoba s/n, 28041 Madrid, Spain
| | - Tomás Palomo
- Complutense University of Madrid, Spain; Department of Psychiatry, Instituto de Investigación Hospital Universitario 12 de Octubre (i+12), Avda. de Córdoba s/n, 28041 Madrid, Spain; Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Spain
| | - Roberto Rodriguez-Jimenez
- Complutense University of Madrid, Spain; Department of Psychiatry, Instituto de Investigación Hospital Universitario 12 de Octubre (i+12), Avda. de Córdoba s/n, 28041 Madrid, Spain; Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Spain
| | - José Luis Pedreira-Massa
- Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Infantil Universitario Niño Jesús, Avda. Menéndez Pelayo, N° 65, 28009 Madrid, Spain; The National University of Distance Education, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
346
|
Heitz C, Noblet V, Phillipps C, Cretin B, Vogt N, Philippi N, Kemp J, de Petigny X, Bilger M, Demuynck C, Martin-Hunyadi C, Armspach JP, Blanc F. Cognitive and affective theory of mind in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer's disease. ALZHEIMERS RESEARCH & THERAPY 2016; 8:10. [PMID: 26979460 PMCID: PMC4793654 DOI: 10.1186/s13195-016-0179-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2015] [Accepted: 01/29/2016] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
Background Theory of mind (ToM) refers to the ability to attribute mental states, thoughts (cognitive component) or feelings (affective component) to others. This function has been studied in many neurodegenerative diseases; however, to our knowledge, no studies investigating ToM in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) have been published. The aim of our study was to assess ToM in patients with DLB and to search for neural correlates of potential deficits. Methods Thirty-three patients with DLB (DLB group) and 15 patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD group), all in the early stage of the disease, as well as 16 healthy elderly control subjects (HC group), were included in the study. After a global cognitive assessment, we used the Faux Pas Recognition (FPR) test, the Reading the Mind in the Eyes (RME) test and Ekman’s Facial Emotion Recognition test to assess cognitive and affective components of ToM. Patients underwent cerebral 3-T magnetic resonance imaging, and atrophy of grey matter was analysed using voxel-based morphometry. We performed a one-sample t test to investigate the correlation between each ToM score and grey matter volume and a two-sample t test to compare patients with DLB impaired with those non-impaired for each test. Results The DLB group performed significantly worse than the HC group on the FPR test (P = 0.033) and the RME test (P = 0.015). There was no significant difference between the AD group and the HC group or between the DLB group and the AD group. Some brain regions were associated with ToM impairments. The prefrontal cortex, with the inferior frontal cortex and the orbitofrontal cortex, was the main region, but we also found correlations with the temporoparietal junction, the precuneus, the fusiform gyrus and the insula. Conclusions This study is the first one to show early impairments of ToM in DLB. The two cognitive and affective components both appear to be affected in this disease. Among patients with ToM difficulties, we found atrophy in brain regions classically involved in ToM, which reinforces the neuronal network of ToM. Further studies are now needed to better understand the neural basis of such impairment. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s13195-016-0179-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Camille Heitz
- Neuropsychology Unit, Memory Resource and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France. .,ICube Laboratory, IMIS Team, University of Strasbourg, CNRS, FMTS, Strasbourg, France. .,Day Hospital, Memory Resources and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Geriatrics, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France.
| | - Vincent Noblet
- ICube Laboratory, IMIS Team, University of Strasbourg, CNRS, FMTS, Strasbourg, France
| | - Clélie Phillipps
- Neuropsychology Unit, Memory Resource and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France
| | - Benjamin Cretin
- Neuropsychology Unit, Memory Resource and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France.,ICube Laboratory, IMIS Team, University of Strasbourg, CNRS, FMTS, Strasbourg, France
| | - Natacha Vogt
- Neuropsychology Unit, Memory Resource and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France
| | - Nathalie Philippi
- Neuropsychology Unit, Memory Resource and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France.,ICube Laboratory, IMIS Team, University of Strasbourg, CNRS, FMTS, Strasbourg, France
| | - Jennifer Kemp
- Neuropsychology Unit, Memory Resource and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France
| | - Xavier de Petigny
- Day Hospital, Memory Resources and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Geriatrics, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France
| | - Mathias Bilger
- Neuropsychology Unit, Memory Resource and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France
| | - Catherine Demuynck
- Day Hospital, Memory Resources and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Geriatrics, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France
| | - Catherine Martin-Hunyadi
- Day Hospital, Memory Resources and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Geriatrics, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France
| | - Jean-Paul Armspach
- ICube Laboratory, IMIS Team, University of Strasbourg, CNRS, FMTS, Strasbourg, France
| | - Frédéric Blanc
- Neuropsychology Unit, Memory Resource and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France.,ICube Laboratory, IMIS Team, University of Strasbourg, CNRS, FMTS, Strasbourg, France.,Day Hospital, Memory Resources and Research Centre (CMRR), Department of Geriatrics, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France
| |
Collapse
|
347
|
Ng SY, Zebrowitz LA, Franklin RG. Age Differences in the Differentiation of Trait Impressions From Faces. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2016; 71:220-9. [PMID: 25194140 PMCID: PMC4757946 DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbu113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2014] [Accepted: 08/05/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVES We investigated whether evidence that older adults (OA) show less differentiation of visual stimuli than younger adults (YA) extends to trait impressions from faces and effects of face age. We also examined whether age differences in mood, vision, or cognition-mediated differentiation differences. Finally, we investigated whether age differences in trait differentiation mediated differences in impression positivity. METHOD We used a differentiation index adapted from previous work on stereotyping to assess OA and YA likelihood of assigning different faces to different levels on trait scales. We computed scores for ratings of older and younger faces' competence, health, hostility, and untrustworthiness. RESULTS OA showed less differentiated trait ratings than YA. Measures of mood, vision, and cognition did not mediate these rater age differences. Hostility was differentiated more for younger than older faces, while health was differentiated more for older faces, but only by OA. Age differences in differentiation mediated age differences in impression positivity. DISCUSSION Less differentiation of trait impressions from faces in OA is consistent with previous evidence for less differentiation in face and emotion recognition. Results indicated that that age-related dedifferentiation does not reflect narrow changes in visual function. They also provide a novel explanation for OA positivity effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stacey Y Ng
- Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts
| | | | - Robert G Franklin
- Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts
| |
Collapse
|
348
|
Casares-Guillén C, García-Rodríguez B, Delgado M, Ellgring H. Age-Related Changes in the Processing of Emotional Faces in a Dual-Task Paradigm. Exp Aging Res 2016; 42:129-43. [PMID: 26890631 DOI: 10.1080/0361073x.2016.1132819] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
UNLABELLED Background/ Study Context: Age-related changes appear to affect the ability to identify emotional facial expressions in dual-task conditions (i.e., while simultaneously performing a second visual task). The level of interference generated by the secondary task depends on the phase of emotional processing affected by the interference and the nature of the secondary task. The aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of these variables on age-related changes in the processing of emotional faces. METHODS The identification of emotional facial expressions (EFEs) was assessed in a dual-task paradigm using the following variables: (a) the phase during which interference was applied (encoding vs. retrieval phase); and (b) the nature of the interfering stimulus (visuospatial vs. verbal). The sample population consisted of 24 healthy aged adults (mean age = 75.38) and 40 younger adults (mean age = 26.90). The accuracy of EFE identification was calculated for all experimental conditions. RESULTS Consistent with our hypothesis, the performance of the older group was poorer than that of the younger group in all experimental conditions. Dual-task performance was poorer when the interference occurred during the encoding phase of emotional face processing and when both tasks were of the same nature (i.e., when the experimental condition was more demanding in terms of attention). CONCLUSIONS These results provide empirical evidence of age-related deficits in the identification of emotional facial expressions, which may be partially explained by the impairment of cognitive resources specific to this task. These findings may account for the difficulties experienced by the elderly during social interactions that require the concomitant processing of emotional and environmental information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carmen Casares-Guillén
- a Facultad de Psicología , Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia , Madrid , Spain
| | | | - Marisa Delgado
- b Facultad de Psicología, Departamento Psicología Básica II , Universidad Complutense , Madrid , Spain
| | - Heiner Ellgring
- c Department of Psychology , Julius Maximilians Universität , Würzburg , Germany
| |
Collapse
|
349
|
Tan JW, Andrade AO, Li H, Walter S, Hrabal D, Rukavina S, Limbrecht-Ecklundt K, Hoffman H, Traue HC. Recognition of Intensive Valence and Arousal Affective States via Facial Electromyographic Activity in Young and Senior Adults. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0146691. [PMID: 26761427 PMCID: PMC4712064 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0146691] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/19/2015] [Accepted: 12/21/2015] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Research suggests that interaction between humans and digital environments characterizes a form of companionship in addition to technical convenience. To this effect, humans have attempted to design computer systems able to demonstrably empathize with the human affective experience. Facial electromyography (EMG) is one such technique enabling machines to access to human affective states. Numerous studies have investigated the effects of valence emotions on facial EMG activity captured over the corrugator supercilii (frowning muscle) and zygomaticus major (smiling muscle). The arousal emotion, specifically, has not received much research attention, however. In the present study, we sought to identify intensive valence and arousal affective states via facial EMG activity. Methods Ten blocks of affective pictures were separated into five categories: neutral valence/low arousal (0VLA), positive valence/high arousal (PVHA), negative valence/high arousal (NVHA), positive valence/low arousal (PVLA), and negative valence/low arousal (NVLA), and the ability of each to elicit corresponding valence and arousal affective states was investigated at length. One hundred and thirteen participants were subjected to these stimuli and provided facial EMG. A set of 16 features based on the amplitude, frequency, predictability, and variability of signals was defined and classified using a support vector machine (SVM). Results We observed highly accurate classification rates based on the combined corrugator and zygomaticus EMG, ranging from 75.69% to 100.00% for the baseline and five affective states (0VLA, PVHA, PVLA, NVHA, and NVLA) in all individuals. There were significant differences in classification rate accuracy between senior and young adults, but there was no significant difference between female and male participants. Conclusion Our research provides robust evidences for recognition of intensive valence and arousal affective states in young and senior adults. These findings contribute to the successful future application of facial EMG for identifying user affective states in human machine interaction (HMI) or companion robotic systems (CRS).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jun-Wen Tan
- College of Teacher Education, Lishui University, Lishui, P.R. China
- * E-mail:
| | - Adriano O. Andrade
- Biomedical Engineering Laboratory, Faculty of Electrical Engineering, Federal University of Uberlândia, Uberlândia, Brazil
| | - Hang Li
- College of Teacher Education, Lishui University, Lishui, P.R. China
| | - Steffen Walter
- Emotion Lab, Section Medical Psychology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
| | - David Hrabal
- Emotion Lab, Section Medical Psychology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
| | - Stefanie Rukavina
- Emotion Lab, Section Medical Psychology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
| | | | - Holger Hoffman
- Emotion Lab, Section Medical Psychology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
| | - Harald C. Traue
- Emotion Lab, Section Medical Psychology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
350
|
Bottiroli S, Cavallini E, Ceccato I, Vecchi T, Lecce S. Theory of Mind in aging: Comparing cognitive and affective components in the faux pas test. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2016; 62:152-62. [DOI: 10.1016/j.archger.2015.09.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/18/2014] [Revised: 09/21/2015] [Accepted: 09/23/2015] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|