1
|
Koevoets EW, Schagen SB, May AM, Geerlings MI, Witlox L, van der Wall E, Stuiver MM, Sonke GS, Velthuis MJ, Jobsen JJ, van der Palen J, de Ruiter MB, Monninkhof EM. Effect of physical exercise on white matter microstructure in chemotherapy-treated breast cancer patients: a randomized controlled trial (PAM study). Brain Imaging Behav 2025; 19:291-301. [PMID: 39804457 PMCID: PMC11978695 DOI: 10.1007/s11682-024-00965-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/22/2024] [Indexed: 04/09/2025]
Abstract
Physical exercise is a promising intervention to improve brain white matter integrity. In the PAM study, exercise intervention effects on white matter integrity were investigated in breast cancer patients. Chemotherapy-treated breast cancer patients with cognitive problems were randomized 2-4 years post-diagnosis to an exercise (n = 91) or control group (n = 90). The 6-month exercise intervention consisted of four hours/week of aerobic and resistance training. White matter integrity was measured at baseline and 6-month follow-up with fractional anisotropy (FA) and mean diffusivity (MD), which were derived from magnetic resonance diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Both DTI metrics were analyzed whole brain and voxel-wise with a modified tract based spatial statistics (TBSS) procedure. Other measurements included cognition and physical fitness. Exercise effects were analyzed with multiple regression analyses. An explorative analysis was conducted in highly fatigued patients. DTI scans were available for 69 patients of the intervention (age = 52.3 ± 8.9yrs.) and 72 patients of the control group (age = 53.2 ± 8.6yrs.). Whole brain and voxel-wise analyses revealed no significant intervention effects on FA and MD. In highly fatigued patients (exercise: n = 32; control: n = 24), significant clusters of decreased FA post-intervention were observed in the left inferior and superior longitudinal fasciculus. Mean FA in these clusters was not predictive of cognition. A 6-month exercise intervention did not affect white matter integrity in chemotherapy-treated breast cancer patients. However, in highly fatigued breast cancer patients a significant FA decrease was observed post-intervention. The direction of these results is unexpected, and more research is needed to further understand these results.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emmie W Koevoets
- Julius Center for Health Sciences and Primary Care, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
- Division of Psychosocial Research and Epidemiology, Netherlands Cancer Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Sanne B Schagen
- Division of Psychosocial Research and Epidemiology, Netherlands Cancer Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
- Brain and Cognition Group, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | - Anne M May
- Julius Center for Health Sciences and Primary Care, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Mirjam I Geerlings
- Department of General Practice, Amsterdam UMC, Location University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Amsterdam Public Health, Aging & Later Life, and Personalized Medicine, Amsterdam UMC, Location University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Amsterdam Neuroscience, Neurodegeneration, and Mood, Anxiety, Psychosis, Stress, and Sleep, Amsterdam UMC, Location University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Lenja Witlox
- Julius Center for Health Sciences and Primary Care, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Elsken van der Wall
- Department of Medical Oncology, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Martijn M Stuiver
- Division of Psychosocial Research and Epidemiology, Netherlands Cancer Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Center for Quality of Life, Netherlands Cancer Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Gabe S Sonke
- Department of Medical Oncology, Netherlands Cancer Institute, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Miranda J Velthuis
- Netherlands Comprehensive Cancer Organisation (IKNL), Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Jan J Jobsen
- Medical School Twente, Medisch Spectrum Twente, Enschede, The Netherlands
| | - Job van der Palen
- Medical School Twente, Medisch Spectrum Twente, Enschede, The Netherlands
- Section Cognition, Data and Education, Universiteit Twente, Enschede, The Netherlands
| | - Michiel B de Ruiter
- Division of Psychosocial Research and Epidemiology, Netherlands Cancer Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Evelyn M Monninkhof
- Julius Center for Health Sciences and Primary Care, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Melcón M, van Bree S, Sánchez-Carro Y, Barreiro-Fernández L, Kolibius LD, Alzueta E, Wimber M, Capilla A, Hanslmayr S. Evidence for a constant occipital spotlight of attention using MVPA on EEG data. PLoS One 2025; 20:e0320233. [PMID: 40138358 PMCID: PMC11940686 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0320233] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/22/2024] [Accepted: 02/14/2025] [Indexed: 03/29/2025] Open
Abstract
While traditional behavioural and electroencephalographic studies claim that visuospatial attention stays fixed at one location at a time, recent research has rather shown that attention rhythmically fluctuates between locations at different rates. However, little is known about the temporal dynamics of this fluctuation and whether it changes over time. We addressed this question by investigating how the neural pattern of visuospatial attention behaves over space and time by employing classification and conventional analysis of occipito-parietal EEG activity. Furthermore, we simulated data with the attentional electrophysiological correlates to control for the ground truth that would give rise to certain classification patterns. We analysed two visuospatial cueing tasks, with a peripheral and a central cue to control for sensory-driven processes, where attention was covertly oriented to the left or right hemifield. First, to decode the spatial locus of attention from neural activity, we trained and tested a classifier on every timepoint from the attentional cue to the stimulus onset. This resulted in one temporal generalization matrix per participant, which was time-frequency decomposed to identify the sampling rhythm. Independently, we calculated a lateralization index based on ERPs and alpha-band power and correlated these indices with classifier performance. Eventually, we simulated two dataset, with ERPs and alpha-band attentional modulations, and employed the same decoding approach. Our results show that attention settled on the cued hemifield in a late time window, but an early and rhythmic sampling of both hemifields exclusively after the peripheral cue. Only the ERP lateralization index correlated with classifier performance in the periperhal cue dataset, suggesting that the early rhythmic state did not reflect attentional sampling but instead was driven by the cue location, idea also supported by our simulations. Together, our results characterise the occipital attentional sampling as a constant process slightly delayed after the cue.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- María Melcón
- Department of Biological and Health Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom
| | - Sander van Bree
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom
- Centre for Human Brain Health, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
- Department of Medicine, Justus Liebig University, Giessen, Germany
| | - Yolanda Sánchez-Carro
- Faculty of Psychology, European University of the Canary Islands, Santa Cruz de Tenerife, Spain
| | | | - Luca D. Kolibius
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom
- Centre for Human Brain Health, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Columbia University, New York, New York, United States of America
| | - Elisabet Alzueta
- Department of Biological and Health Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
- Biosciences Division, Center for Health Sciences, SRI International, Menlo Park, California, United States of America.
| | - Maria Wimber
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom
- Centre for Human Brain Health, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
| | - Almudena Capilla
- Department of Biological and Health Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | - Simon Hanslmayr
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom
- Centre for Human Brain Health, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Jurkiewicz T, Delporte L, Revol P, Rossetti Y, Pisella L. Effect of juggling expertise on pointing performance in peripheral vision. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0306630. [PMID: 38995902 PMCID: PMC11244809 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0306630] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/27/2023] [Accepted: 06/20/2024] [Indexed: 07/14/2024] Open
Abstract
Juggling is a very complex activity requiring motor, visual and coordination skills. Expert jugglers experience a "third eye" monitoring leftward and rightward ball zenith positions alternately, in the upper visual fields, while maintaining their gaze straight-ahead. This "third eye" reduces their motor noise (improved body stability and decrease in hand movement variability) as it avoids the numerous head and eye movements that add noise into the system and make trajectories more uncertain. Neuroimaging studies have shown that learning to juggle induces white and grey matter hypertrophy at the posterior intraparietal sulcus. Damage to this brain region leads to optic ataxia, a clinical condition characterised by peripheral pointing bias toward gaze position. We predicted that expert jugglers would, conversely, present better accuracy in a peripheral pointing task. The mean pointing accuracy of expert jugglers was better for peripheral pointing within the upper visual field, compatible with their subjective experience of the "third eye". Further analyses showed that experts exhibited much less between-subject variability than beginners, reinforcing the interpretation of a vertically asymmetrical calibration of peripheral space, characteristic of juggling and homogenous in the expert group. On the contrary, individual pointing variability did not differ between groups neither globally nor in any sector of space, showing that the reduced motor noise of experts in juggling did not transfer to pointing. It is concluded that the plasticity of the posterior intraparietal sulcus related to juggling expertise does not consist of globally improved visual-to-motor ability. It rather consists of peripheral space calibration by practicing horizontal covert shifts of the attentional spotlight within the upper visual field, between left and right ball zenith positions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tristan Jurkiewicz
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Trajectoires Team, Bron, France
- Centre d’Exploration de la Rétine Kléber, Ophthalmology Department, Lyon, France
| | - Ludovic Delporte
- Plateforme Mouvement et Handicap, Hôpital Henry Gabrielle, St-Genis-Laval, France
| | - Patrice Revol
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Trajectoires Team, Bron, France
- Plateforme Mouvement et Handicap, Hôpital Henry Gabrielle, St-Genis-Laval, France
| | - Yves Rossetti
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Trajectoires Team, Bron, France
- Plateforme Mouvement et Handicap, Hôpital Henry Gabrielle, St-Genis-Laval, France
| | - Laure Pisella
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Trajectoires Team, Bron, France
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Han Z, Sereno AB. A spatial map: a propitious choice for constraining the binding problem. Front Comput Neurosci 2024; 18:1397819. [PMID: 39015744 PMCID: PMC11250423 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2024.1397819] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/08/2024] [Accepted: 06/05/2024] [Indexed: 07/18/2024] Open
Abstract
Many studies have shown that the human visual system has two major functionally distinct cortical visual pathways: a ventral pathway, thought to be important for object recognition, and a dorsal pathway, thought to be important for spatial cognition. According to our and others previous studies, artificial neural networks with two segregated pathways can determine objects' identities and locations more accurately and efficiently than one-pathway artificial neural networks. In addition, we showed that these two segregated artificial cortical visual pathways can each process identity and spatial information of visual objects independently and differently. However, when using such networks to process multiple objects' identities and locations, a binding problem arises because the networks may not associate each object's identity with its location correctly. In a previous study, we constrained the binding problem by training the artificial identity pathway to retain relative location information of objects. This design uses a location map to constrain the binding problem. One limitation of that study was that we only considered two attributes of our objects (identity and location) and only one possible map (location) for binding. However, typically the brain needs to process and bind many attributes of an object, and any of these attributes could be used to constrain the binding problem. In our current study, using visual objects with multiple attributes (identity, luminance, orientation, and location) that need to be recognized, we tried to find the best map (among an identity map, a luminance map, an orientation map, or a location map) to constrain the binding problem. We found that in our experimental simulations, when visual attributes are independent of each other, a location map is always a better choice than the other kinds of maps examined for constraining the binding problem. Our findings agree with previous neurophysiological findings that show that the organization or map in many visual cortical areas is primarily retinotopic or spatial.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhixian Han
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, United States
| | - Anne B. Sereno
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, United States
- Weldon School of Biomedical Engineering, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, United States
- Department of Family Medicine, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, United States
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Park J, Soucy E, Segawa J, Mair R, Konkle T. Immersive scene representation in human visual cortex with ultra-wide-angle neuroimaging. Nat Commun 2024; 15:5477. [PMID: 38942766 PMCID: PMC11213904 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-49669-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2023] [Accepted: 06/13/2024] [Indexed: 06/30/2024] Open
Abstract
While human vision spans 220°, traditional functional MRI setups display images only up to central 10-15°. Thus, it remains unknown how the brain represents a scene perceived across the full visual field. Here, we introduce a method for ultra-wide angle display and probe signatures of immersive scene representation. An unobstructed view of 175° is achieved by bouncing the projected image off angled-mirrors onto a custom-built curved screen. To avoid perceptual distortion, scenes are created with wide field-of-view from custom virtual environments. We find that immersive scene representation drives medial cortex with far-peripheral preferences, but shows minimal modulation in classic scene regions. Further, scene and face-selective regions maintain their content preferences even with extreme far-periphery stimulation, highlighting that not all far-peripheral information is automatically integrated into scene regions computations. This work provides clarifying evidence on content vs. peripheral preferences in scene representation and opens new avenues to research immersive vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeongho Park
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA.
| | - Edward Soucy
- Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Jennifer Segawa
- Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Ross Mair
- Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
- Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
- Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Talia Konkle
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
- Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
- Kempner Institute for Biological and Artificial Intelligence, Harvard University, Boston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Faurite C, Aprile E, Kauffmann L, Mermillod M, Gallice M, Chiquet C, Cottereau BR, Peyrin C. Interaction between central and peripheral vision: Influence of distance and spatial frequencies. J Vis 2024; 24:3. [PMID: 38190145 PMCID: PMC10777871 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.1.3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2023] [Accepted: 10/30/2024] [Indexed: 01/09/2024] Open
Abstract
Visual scene perception is based on reciprocal interactions between central and peripheral information. Such interactions are commonly investigated through the semantic congruence effect, which usually reveals a congruence effect of central vision on peripheral vision as strong as the reverse. The aim of the present study was to further investigate the mechanisms underlying central-peripheral visual interactions using a central-peripheral congruence paradigm through three behavioral experiments. We presented simultaneously a central and a peripheral stimulus, that could be either semantically congruent or incongruent. To assess the congruence effect of central vision on peripheral vision, participants had to categorize the peripheral target stimulus while ignoring the central distractor stimulus. To assess the congruence effect of the peripheral vision on central vision, they had to categorize the central target stimulus while ignoring the peripheral distractor stimulus. Experiment 1 revealed that the physical distance between central and peripheral stimuli influences central-peripheral visual interactions: Congruence effect of central vision is stronger when the distance between the target and the distractor is the shortest. Experiments 2 and 3 revealed that the spatial frequency content of distractors also influence central-peripheral interactions: Congruence effect of central vision is observed only when the distractor contained high spatial frequencies while congruence effect of peripheral vision is observed only when the distractor contained low spatial frequencies. These results raise the question of how these influences are exerted (bottom-up vs. top-down) and are discussed based on the retinocortical properties of the visual system and the predictive brain hypothesis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cynthia Faurite
- Université Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, Grenoble, France
| | - Eva Aprile
- Université Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, Grenoble, France
| | - Louise Kauffmann
- Université Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, Grenoble, France
| | - Martial Mermillod
- Université Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, Grenoble, France
| | - Mathilde Gallice
- Department of Ophthalmology, Grenoble Alpes University Hospital, Grenoble, France
| | - Christophe Chiquet
- Department of Ophthalmology, Grenoble Alpes University Hospital, Grenoble, France
| | - Benoit R Cottereau
- Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition, Université Toulouse III-Paul Sabatier, Toulouse, France
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Toulouse, France
| | - Carole Peyrin
- Université Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, Grenoble, France
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Wojciechowski J, Jurewicz K, Dzianok P, Antonova I, Paluch K, Wolak T, Kublik E. Common and distinct BOLD correlates of Simon and flanker conflicts which can(not) be reduced to time-on-task effects. Hum Brain Mapp 2024; 45:e26549. [PMID: 38224538 PMCID: PMC10777776 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.26549] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/05/2023] [Revised: 10/25/2023] [Accepted: 11/16/2023] [Indexed: 01/17/2024] Open
Abstract
The ability to identify and resolve conflicts between standard, well-trained behaviors and behaviors required by the current context is an essential feature of cognitive control. To date, no consensus has been reached on the brain mechanisms involved in exerting such control: while some studies identified diverse patterns of activity across different conflicts, other studies reported common resources across conflict tasks or even across simple tasks devoid of the conflict component. The latter reports attributed the entire activity observed in the presence of conflict to longer time spent on the task (i.e., to the so-called time-on-task effects). Here, we used an extended Multi-Source Interference Task (MSIT) which combines Simon and flanker types of interference to determine shared and conflict-specific mechanisms of conflict resolution in fMRI and their separability from the time-on-task effects. Large portions of the activity in the dorsal attention network and decreases of activity in the default mode network were shared across the tasks and scaled in parallel with increasing reaction times. Importantly, the activity in the sensory and sensorimotor cortices, as well as in the posterior medial frontal cortex (pMFC) - a key region implicated in conflict processing - could not be exhaustively explained by the time-on-task effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jakub Wojciechowski
- Neurobiology of Emotions LaboratoryNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
- Bioimaging Research CenterInstitute of Physiology and Pathology of HearingWarsawPoland
| | - Katarzyna Jurewicz
- Neurobiology of Emotions LaboratoryNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
- Department of PhysiologyFaculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, McGill UniversityMontrealQuebecCanada
| | - Patrycja Dzianok
- Neurobiology of Emotions LaboratoryNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
| | - Ingrida Antonova
- Neurobiology of Emotions LaboratoryNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
- Laboratory of NeuroinformaticsNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
| | - Katarzyna Paluch
- Neurobiology of Emotions LaboratoryNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
- Laboratory of Neurophysiology of MindCenter of Excellence for Neural Plasticity and Brain Disorders: BRAINCITY, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
| | - Tomasz Wolak
- Bioimaging Research CenterInstitute of Physiology and Pathology of HearingWarsawPoland
| | - Ewa Kublik
- Neurobiology of Emotions LaboratoryNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Wang L, Li C, Han Z, Wu Q, Sun L, Zhang X, Go R, Wu J, Yan T. Spatiotemporal and sensory modality attention processing with domain-specific representations in frontoparietal areas. Cereb Cortex 2022; 32:5489-5502. [PMID: 35136999 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhac029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/06/2021] [Revised: 01/10/2022] [Accepted: 02/12/2022] [Indexed: 01/25/2023] Open
Abstract
The frontoparietal network (FPN), including bilateral frontal eye field, inferior parietal sulcus, and supplementary motor area, has been linked to attention processing, including spatiotemporal and sensory modality domains. However, it is unclear whether FPN encodes representations of these domains that are generalizable across subdomains. We decomposed multivariate patterns of functional magnetic resonance imaging activity from 20 participants into domain-specific components and identified latent multivariate representations that generalized across subdomains. The 30 experimental conditions were organized into unimodal-bimodal and spatial-temporal models. We found that brain areas in the FPN, form the primary network that modulated during attention across domains. However, the activation patterns of areas within the FPN were reorganized according to the specific attentional demand, especially when pay attention to different sensory, suggesting distinct regional neural representations associated with specific attentional processes within FPN. In addition, there were also other domain-specific areas outside the FPN, such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Our conclusion is that, according to the results of the analysis of representation similarity, 2 types of activated brain regions, related to attention domain detailed information processing and general information processing, can be revealed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luyao Wang
- School of Life Science, Shanghai University, Shanghai 200444, China
| | - Chunlin Li
- School of Biomedical Engineering, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100069, China.,Beijing Advanced Innovation Center for Big Data-Based Precision Medicine, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100069, China.,Beijing Key Laboratory of Fundamental Research on Biomechanics in Clinical Application, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100069, China
| | - Ziteng Han
- School of Mechatronical Engineering, Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing 100081, China
| | - Qiong Wu
- Department of Psychology, Suzhou University of Science and Technology, Suzhou 215009, China.,Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, Graduate School of Interdisciplinary Science and Engineering in Health Systems, Okayama University, Okayama 700-0084, Japan
| | - Liwei Sun
- School of Biomedical Engineering, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100069, China.,Beijing Advanced Innovation Center for Big Data-Based Precision Medicine, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100069, China.,Beijing Key Laboratory of Fundamental Research on Biomechanics in Clinical Application, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100069, China
| | - Xu Zhang
- School of Biomedical Engineering, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100069, China.,Beijing Advanced Innovation Center for Big Data-Based Precision Medicine, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100069, China.,Beijing Key Laboratory of Fundamental Research on Biomechanics in Clinical Application, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100069, China
| | - Ritsu Go
- School of Mechatronical Engineering, Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing 100081, China
| | - Jinglong Wu
- School of Mechatronical Engineering, Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing 100081, China.,Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, Graduate School of Interdisciplinary Science and Engineering in Health Systems, Okayama University, Okayama 700-0084, Japan
| | - Tianyi Yan
- School of Life Science, Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing 100081, China
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Trouilloud A, Rossel P, Faurite C, Roux-Sibilon A, Kauffmann L, Peyrin C. Influence of physical features from peripheral vision on scene categorization in central vision. VISUAL COGNITION 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2022.2087814] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Audrey Trouilloud
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, Grenoble, France
| | - Pauline Rossel
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, Grenoble, France
| | - Cynthia Faurite
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, Grenoble, France
| | - Alexia Roux-Sibilon
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute (IPSY), UC Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Louise Kauffmann
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, Grenoble, France
| | - Carole Peyrin
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, Grenoble, France
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Ribeiro FL, Bollmann S, Puckett AM. Predicting the retinotopic organization of human visual cortex from anatomy using geometric deep learning. Neuroimage 2021; 244:118624. [PMID: 34607019 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118624] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/04/2021] [Revised: 09/13/2021] [Accepted: 09/27/2021] [Indexed: 10/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Whether it be in a single neuron or a more complex biological system like the human brain, form and function are often directly related. The functional organization of human visual cortex, for instance, is tightly coupled with the underlying anatomy with cortical shape having been shown to be a useful predictor of the retinotopic organization in early visual cortex. Although the current state-of-the-art in predicting retinotopic maps is able to account for gross individual differences, such models are unable to account for any idiosyncratic differences in the structure-function relationship from anatomical information alone due to their initial assumption of a template. Here we developed a geometric deep learning model capable of exploiting the actual structure of the cortex to learn the complex relationship between brain function and anatomy in human visual cortex such that more realistic and idiosyncratic maps could be predicted. We show that our neural network was not only able to predict the functional organization throughout the visual cortical hierarchy, but that it was also able to predict nuanced variations across individuals. Although we demonstrate its utility for modeling the relationship between structure and function in human visual cortex, our approach is flexible and well-suited for a range of other applications involving data structured in non-Euclidean spaces.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fernanda L Ribeiro
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Saint Lucia, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia; Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia.
| | - Steffen Bollmann
- School of Information Technology and Electrical Engineering, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia
| | - Alexander M Puckett
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Saint Lucia, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia; Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Żołubak A, Garcia-Suarez L. Shape discrimination in peripheral vision: Addressing pragmatic limitations of M-scaling radial frequency patterns. Vision Res 2021; 188:115-125. [PMID: 34315091 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2021.06.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2020] [Revised: 06/24/2021] [Accepted: 06/27/2021] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Peripheral worsening in shape discrimination (SD) can be compensated by size-scaling of peripheral stimuli. However, such scaling results in production of large stimuli that occupy a vast range of eccentricities. We used six proportionally decreasing spatial scales to address this pragmatic limitation and to explore how shape discrimination varies with radius in the nasal visual field. Five participants with normal vision discriminated circles and radial frequency (RF) patterns presented nasally to the fixation point at 5°, 10°, 15° and 20°. Stimuli were scaled with the nasal cortical magnification factor (nCMF) from a central stimulus in six spatial scales, which varied from 0.125 to 1, where 1 corresponded to 1.2° radius. Thresholds expressed in Weber fractions remained constant at eccentricities up to 20° regardless of the spatial scale. Weber fractions for the smaller spatial scales (0.125-0.5) were higher and more variable than for the larger spatial scales (0.75-1), yet still constant across periphery. The results provide evidence that peripheral shape discrimination is constrained by low-level properties, such as eccentricity, and can be predicted by the cortical magnification theory. However, above the peripheral modulation resolution limits, RF shape discrimination is based on the proportion between the modulation amplitude and the radius for larger scales (0.75-1), and demonstrates peripheral scale invariance for these stimuli. For eccentric shape discrimination tests, stimuli with low spatial frequency, high contrast, and radii corresponding to SS 0.75-0.875 should be used to ensure constant Weber fractions, small variability, and peripheral stimuli that are not excessively magnified.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna Żołubak
- School of Health Professions, University of Plymouth, Derriford Road, Plymouth PL6 8BH, United Kingdom.
| | - Luis Garcia-Suarez
- School of Health Professions, University of Plymouth, Derriford Road, Plymouth PL6 8BH, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Blom JD, Nanuashvili N, Waters F. Time Distortions: A Systematic Review of Cases Characteristic of Alice in Wonderland Syndrome. Front Psychiatry 2021; 12:668633. [PMID: 34025485 PMCID: PMC8138562 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.668633] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2021] [Accepted: 04/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Of the perceptual distortions characteristic of Alice in Wonderland syndrome, substantial alterations in the immediate experience of time are probably the least known and the most fascinating. We reviewed original case reports to examine the phenomenology and associated pathology of these time distortions in this syndrome. A systematic search in PubMed, Ovid Medline, and the historical literature yielded 59 publications that described 168 people experiencing time distortions, including 84 detailed individual case reports. We distinguished five different types of time distortion. The most common category comprises slow-motion and quick-motion phenomena. In 39% of all cases, time distortions were unimodal in nature, while in 61% there was additional involvement of the visual (49%), kinaesthetic (18%), and auditory modalities (14%). In all, 40% of all time distortions described were bimodal in nature and 19% trimodal, with 1% involving four modalities. Underlying neurological mechanisms are varied and may be triggered by intoxications, infectious diseases, metabolic disorders, CNS lesions, paroxysmal neurological disorders, and psychiatric disorders. Bizarre sensations of time alteration-such as time going backwards or moving in circles-were mostly associated with psychosis. Pathophysiologically, mainly occipital areas appear to be involved, although the temporal network is widely disseminated, with separate component timing mechanisms not always functioning synchronously, thus occasionally creating temporal mismatches within and across sensory modalities (desynchronization). Based on our findings, we propose a classification of time distortions and formulate implications for research and clinical practice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jan Dirk Blom
- Outpatient Clinic for Uncommon Psychiatric Syndromes, Parnassia Psychiatric Institute, The Hague, Netherlands.,Faculty of Social Sciences, Leiden University, Leiden, Netherlands.,Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
| | - Nutsa Nanuashvili
- Amsterdam Brain and Cognition Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Flavie Waters
- Clinical Research Centre, Graylands Hospital, North Metro Health Service Mental Health, Perth, WA, Australia.,School of Psychological Sciences, University of Western Australia, Perth, WA, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Rutland JW, Delman BN, Huang KH, Verma G, Benson NC, Villavisanis DF, Lin HM, Bederson JB, Chelnis J, Shrivastava RK, Balchandani P. Primary visual cortical thickness in correlation with visual field defects in patients with pituitary macroadenomas: a structural 7-Tesla retinotopic analysis. J Neurosurg 2020; 133:1371-1381. [PMID: 31628280 PMCID: PMC7205160 DOI: 10.3171/2019.7.jns191712] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2019] [Accepted: 07/26/2019] [Indexed: 01/22/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Vision loss remains a debilitating complication of pituitary adenomas, although there is considerable variability in visual impairment before and after decompression surgery. Growing evidence suggests secondary damage to remote visual structures may contribute to vision loss in patients with chiasmatic compression. The present study leverages ultrahigh field 7-T MRI to study the retinotopic organization of the primary visual cortex (V1), and correlates visual defects with cortical thinning in V1 to characterize consequences of pituitary adenomas on the posterior visual system. METHODS Eight patients (4 males and 4 females, mean age 44.3 years) with pituitary adenomas who exhibited chiasmatic compression and visual field defects, as well as 8 matched healthy controls (4 males and 4 females, mean age 43.3 years), were scanned at 7-T MRI for this prospective study. Whole-brain cortical thickness was calculated using an automated algorithm. A previously published surface-based algorithm was applied to associate the eccentricity and polar angle with each position in V1. Cortical thickness was calculated at each point in the retinotopic organization, and a cortical thickness ratio was generated against matched controls for each point in the visual fields. Patients with adenoma additionally underwent neuroophthalmological examination including 24-2 Humphrey automated visual field perimetry. Pattern deviation (PD) of each point in the visual field, i.e., the deviation in point detection compared with neurologically healthy controls, was correlated with cortical thickness at corresponding polar and eccentricity angles in V1. RESULTS Whole-brain cortical thickness was successfully derived for all patients and controls. The mean tumor volume was 19.4 cm3. The median global thickness of V1 did not differ between patients (mean ± SD 2.21 ± 0.12 cm), compared with controls (2.06 ± 0.13 cm, p > 0.05). Surface morphometry-based retinotopic maps revealed that all 8 patients with adenoma showed a significant positive correlation between PD and V1 thickness ratios (r values ranged from 0.31 to 0.53, p < 0.05). Mixed-procedure analysis revealed that PD = -8.0719 + 5.5873*[Median V1 Thickness Ratio]. CONCLUSIONS All 8 patients showed significant positive correlations between V1 thickness and visual defect. These findings provide retinotopic maps of localized V1 cortical neurodegeneration spatially corresponding to impairments in the visual field. These results further characterize changes in the posterior visual pathway associated with chiasmatic compression, and may prove useful in the neuroophthalmological workup for patients with pituitary macroadenoma.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John W Rutland
- 1Translational and Molecular Imaging Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai
- 2Department of Neurosurgery, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai
| | - Bradley N Delman
- 1Translational and Molecular Imaging Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai
- 3Department of Diagnostic, Molecular and Interventional Radiology, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai
| | - Kuang-Han Huang
- 1Translational and Molecular Imaging Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai
| | - Gaurav Verma
- 1Translational and Molecular Imaging Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai
| | | | - Dillan F Villavisanis
- 1Translational and Molecular Imaging Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai
| | - Hung-Mo Lin
- 5Department of Population Health Science and Policy, Mount Sinai Hospital; and
| | - Joshua B Bederson
- 2Department of Neurosurgery, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai
| | - James Chelnis
- 6Department of Ophthalmology, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York
| | - Raj K Shrivastava
- 2Department of Neurosurgery, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai
| | - Priti Balchandani
- 1Translational and Molecular Imaging Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Trouilloud A, Kauffmann L, Roux-Sibilon A, Rossel P, Boucart M, Mermillod M, Peyrin C. Rapid scene categorization: From coarse peripheral vision to fine central vision. Vision Res 2020; 170:60-72. [PMID: 32259648 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2020.02.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2019] [Revised: 12/12/2019] [Accepted: 02/17/2020] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Studies on scene perception have shown that the rapid extraction of low spatial frequencies (LSF) allows a coarse parsing of the scene, prior to the analysis of high spatial frequencies (HSF) containing details. Many studies suggest that scene gist recognition can be achieved with only the low resolution of peripheral vision. Our study investigated the advantage of peripheral vision on central vision during a scene categorization task (indoor vs. outdoor). In Experiment 1, we used large scene photographs from which we built one central disk and four circular rings of different eccentricities. The central disk either contained or not an object semantically related to the scene category. Results showed better categorization performances for the peripheral rings, despite the presence of an object in central vision that was semantically related to the scene category that significantly improved categorization performances. In Experiment 2, the central disk and rings were assembled from Central to Peripheral vision (CtP sequence) or from Peripheral to Central vision (PtC sequence). Results revealed better performances for PtC than CtP sequences, except when no central object was present under rapid categorization constraints. As Experiment 3 suggested that the PtC advantage was not explained by a reduction of the visibility of the object in the central disk by the surrounding peripheral rings (CtP sequence), results are interpreted in the context of a predominant coarse-to-fine processing during scene categorization, with greater efficiency and utility of coarse peripheral vision relative to fine central vision during rapid scene categorization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Audrey Trouilloud
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France
| | - Louise Kauffmann
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France; Univ. Grenoble Alpes, CNRS, Grenoble INP, GIPSA-lab, 38000 Grenoble, France
| | - Alexia Roux-Sibilon
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France
| | - Pauline Rossel
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France
| | - Muriel Boucart
- SCALab, University of Lille, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Lille, France
| | - Martial Mermillod
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France
| | - Carole Peyrin
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France.
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Nasr S, LaPierre C, Vaughn CE, Witzel T, Stockmann JP, Polimeni JR. In vivo functional localization of the temporal monocular crescent representation in human primary visual cortex. Neuroimage 2020; 209:116516. [PMID: 31904490 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116516] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/22/2019] [Revised: 12/02/2019] [Accepted: 01/01/2020] [Indexed: 12/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The temporal monocular crescent (TMC) is the most peripheral portion of the visual field whose perception relies solely on input from the ipsilateral eye. According to a handful of post-mortem histological studies in humans and non-human primates, the TMC is represented visuotopically within the most anterior portion of the primary visual cortical area (V1). However, functional evidence of the TMC visuotopic representation in human visual cortex is rare, mostly due to the small size of the TMC representation (~6% of V1) and due to the technical challenges of stimulating the most peripheral portion of the visual field inside the MRI scanner. In this study, by taking advantage of custom-built MRI-compatible visual stimulation goggles with curved displays, we successfully stimulated the TMC region of the visual field in eight human subjects, half of them right-eye dominant, inside a 3 T MRI scanner. This enabled us to localize the representation of TMC, along with the blind spot representation (another visuotopic landmark in V1), in all volunteers, which match the expected spatial pattern based on prior anatomical studies. In all hemispheres, the TMC visuotopic representation was localized along the peripheral border of V1, within the most anterior portion of the calcarine sulcus, without any apparent extension into the second visual area (V2). We further demonstrate the reliability of this localization within/across experimental sessions, and consistency in the spatial location of TMC across individuals after accounting for inter-subject structural differences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shahin Nasr
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129, United States; Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States.
| | - Cristen LaPierre
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129, United States
| | - Christopher E Vaughn
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129, United States
| | - Thomas Witzel
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129, United States; Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Jason P Stockmann
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129, United States; Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Jonathan R Polimeni
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129, United States; Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States; Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Division of Health Sciences and Technology, Cambridge, MA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Li D, Li T, Niu Y, Xiang J, Cao R, Liu B, Zhang H, Wang B. Reduced hemispheric asymmetry of brain anatomical networks in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Brain Imaging Behav 2019; 13:669-684. [PMID: 29752654 DOI: 10.1007/s11682-018-9881-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
Despite many studies reporting a variety of alterations in brain networks in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), alterations in hemispheric anatomical networks are still unclear. In this study, we investigated topology alterations in hemispheric white matter in patients with ADHD and the relationship between these alterations and clinical features of the illness. Weighted hemispheric brain anatomical networks were first constructed for each of 40 right-handed patients with ADHD and 53 matched normal controls. Then, graph theoretical approaches were utilized to compute hemispheric topological properties. The small-world property was preserved in the hemispheric network. Furthermore, a significant group-by-hemisphere interaction was revealed in global efficiency, local efficiency and characteristic path length, attributed to the significantly reduced hemispheric asymmetry of global and local integration in patients with ADHD compared with normal controls. Specifically, reduced asymmetric regional efficiency was found in three regions. Finally, we found that the abnormal asymmetry of hemispheric brain anatomical network topology and regional efficiency were both associated with clinical features (the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale and Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale) in patients. Our findings provide new insights into the lateralized nature of hemispheric dysconnectivity and highlight the potential for using brain network measures of hemispheric asymmetry as neural biomarkers for ADHD and its clinical features.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dandan Li
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, No. 79, Yingze West Street, Taiyuan, 030024, Shanxi, China
| | - Ting Li
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, No. 79, Yingze West Street, Taiyuan, 030024, Shanxi, China
| | - Yan Niu
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, No. 79, Yingze West Street, Taiyuan, 030024, Shanxi, China
| | - Jie Xiang
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, No. 79, Yingze West Street, Taiyuan, 030024, Shanxi, China
| | - Rui Cao
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, No. 79, Yingze West Street, Taiyuan, 030024, Shanxi, China
| | - Bo Liu
- Department of Radiology, First Hospital of Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan, 030001, Shanxi, China
| | - Hui Zhang
- Department of Radiology, First Hospital of Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan, 030001, Shanxi, China
| | - Bin Wang
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, No. 79, Yingze West Street, Taiyuan, 030024, Shanxi, China. .,Department of Radiology, First Hospital of Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan, 030001, Shanxi, China.
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Zhang J, Zhang Z, Go R, Li C, Wu J. Discrimination Thresholds for Passive Tactile Volume Perception by Fingertips. Perception 2019; 48:1252-1267. [PMID: 31558099 DOI: 10.1177/0301006619878560] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
Haptic object perception is still poorly understood up to now. This study investigated the ability of human fingers to discriminate the volume of objects by passive touch. Experiments measured the discrimination threshold of volume using three tasks: passive tactile volume perception, passive tactile area perception, and active tactile volume perception. In each trial, we utilized two plastic cubes to successively stimulate the fingers, and participants were instructed to make comparisons between the stimulus objects’ volume and area. Results showed that there was no significant difference in the discrimination thresholds of tactile volume perception between passive touch and active touch, whereas significant differences in the discrimination thresholds between fingertips, such as the thumb versus the pinky finger. In passive touch, the discrimination thresholds of volume perception were larger than that with surface area perception. We found that the discrimination of the volume of objects is more difficult than the discrimination of the area of the objects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jian Zhang
- Intelligent Robotics Institute, School of Mechatronical Engineering, Beijing Institute of Technology, China
| | - Zhilin Zhang
- Department of Psychiatry, Graduate School of Medicine, Kyoto University, Japan
| | - Ritsu Go
- Intelligent Robotics Institute, School of Mechatronical Engineering, Beijing Institute of Technology, China
| | - Chunlin Li
- School of Biomedical Engineering, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Jinglong Wu
- Intelligent Robotics Institute, School of Mechatronical Engineering, Beijing Institute of Technology, China
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Central and peripheral steady-state visual evoked potentials in children with optic pathway gliomas. Doc Ophthalmol 2019; 139:137-149. [PMID: 31243619 DOI: 10.1007/s10633-019-09703-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/06/2018] [Accepted: 06/19/2019] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Treatment of optic pathway gliomas is prompted by neuroradiological evidence of tumor growth, usually associated with progressive visual loss. Despite therapy, approximately 40% will show visual deterioration. Treatment outcome is largely based on the preservation of vision. However, current visual function assessment is often unreliable in children with optic pathway gliomas who have limited collaboration. Thus, there is a need for new clinical tools to evaluate visual functions in these children. The aim of the study was to assess the value of steady-state visual evoked potentials as a tool to assess function in the central and peripheral visual fields of children with optic pathway gliomas. METHOD Ten patients with optic pathway gliomas and 33 healthy controls (ages 3 to 18 years) were tested using steady-state visual evoked potentials. The dartboard stimulus consisted of one central circle alternating at 16 reversals/s and one peripheral hoop alternating at 14.4 reversals/s, separated by a hoop of gray space. It was presented monocularly at 30% and 96% contrasts. RESULTS Results indicated that central signal-to-noise ratios were significantly lower in children with optic pathway gliomas compared to controls. However, no significant group difference was detected in the peripheral visual field. CONCLUSION Steady-state visual evoked potentials could eventually be implemented in the clinical assessment and follow-up of central visual field deficits in uncooperative or nonverbal children but seem to have limited usefulness for evaluation of peripheral visual field deficits. Additional studies are needed to identify testing parameters for full visual field assessment.
Collapse
|
19
|
Wang M, Li C, Zhang W, Wang Y, Feng Y, Liang Y, Wei J, Zhang X, Li X, Chen R. Support Vector Machine for Analyzing Contributions of Brain Regions During Task-State fMRI. Front Neuroinform 2019; 13:10. [PMID: 30894812 PMCID: PMC6414418 DOI: 10.3389/fninf.2019.00010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/15/2018] [Accepted: 02/12/2019] [Indexed: 12/24/2022] Open
Abstract
The mainstream method used for the analysis of task functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) data, is to obtain task-related active brain regions based on generalized linear models. Machine learning as a data-driven technical method is increasingly used in fMRI data analysis. The language task data, including math task and story task, of the Human Connectome Project (HCP) was used in this work. We chose a linear support vector machine as a classifier to classify math and story tasks and compared them with the activated brain regions of a SPM statistical analysis. As a result, 13 of the 25 regions used for classification in SVM were activated regions, and 12 were non-activated regions. In particular, the right Paracentral Lobule and right Rolandic Operculum which belong to non-activated regions, contributed most to the classification. Therefore, the differences found in machine learning can provide a new understanding of the physiological mechanisms of brain regions under different tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mengyue Wang
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Fundamental Research on Biomechanics in Clinical Application, School of Biomedical Engineering, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Chunlin Li
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Fundamental Research on Biomechanics in Clinical Application, School of Biomedical Engineering, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Wenjing Zhang
- Beijing Stomatological Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | | | - Yuan Feng
- Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing, China
| | - Ying Liang
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Fundamental Research on Biomechanics in Clinical Application, School of Biomedical Engineering, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Jing Wei
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Fundamental Research on Biomechanics in Clinical Application, School of Biomedical Engineering, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Xu Zhang
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Fundamental Research on Biomechanics in Clinical Application, School of Biomedical Engineering, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Xia Li
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Fundamental Research on Biomechanics in Clinical Application, School of Biomedical Engineering, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Renji Chen
- Beijing Stomatological Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Abnormal intrinsic functional network hubs and connectivity following peripheral visual loss because of inherited retinal degeneration. Neuroreport 2019; 30:295-304. [DOI: 10.1097/wnr.0000000000001200] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
|
21
|
Abstract
Rapid shifts of involuntary attention have been shown to induce mislocalizations of nearby objects. One pattern of mislocalization, termed the Attentional Repulsion Effect (ARE), occurs when the onset of peripheral pre-cues lead to perceived shifts of subsequently presented stimuli away from the cued location. While the standard ARE configuration utilizes vernier lines, to date, all previous ARE studies have only assessed distortions along one direction and tested one spatial dimension (i.e., position or shape). The present study assessed the magnitude of the ARE using a novel stimulus configuration. Across three experiments participants judged which of two rectangles on the left or right side of the display appeared wider or taller. Pre-cues were used in Experiments 1 and 2. Results show equivalent perceived expansions in the width and height of the pre-cued rectangle in addition to baseline asymmetries in left/right relative size under no-cue conditions. Altering cue locations led to shifts in the perceived location of the same rectangles, demonstrating distortions in perceived shape and location using the same stimuli and cues. Experiment 3 demonstrates that rectangles are perceived as larger in the periphery compared to fixation, suggesting that eye movements cannot account for results from Experiments 1 and 2. The results support the hypothesis that the ARE reflects a localized, symmetrical warping of visual space that impacts multiple aspects of spatial and object perception.
Collapse
|
22
|
El Mohamad AR, Tatu L, Moulin T, Fadoul S, Vuillier F. Main anatomical features of the calcarine sulcus: a 3D magnetic resonance imaging at 3T study. Surg Radiol Anat 2018; 41:181-186. [PMID: 30430185 DOI: 10.1007/s00276-018-2118-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2018] [Accepted: 07/13/2018] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The purpose of our study was to determine the main anatomical features of the calcarine sulcus using a 3-T MRI. METHODS Fifty human brains have been explored using an MRI 3-T in Doctors Center in Beirut (Lebanon). RESULTS The calcarine sulcus was identified in 100% of cases. In most cases, it had a continuous aspect with several peaks. In all our specimens, the calcarine sulcus crosses the parieto-occipital fissure. The majority of their collateral branches and their connections with other sulci were located at the level of the calcarine sulcus properly. In the majority of specimens, the deepest part of the anterior calcarine sulcus forms a protrusion in the occipital horn of the lateral ventricle called calcar avis. CONCLUSION Our study emphasizes the fact that the course patterns of the calcarine sulcus are highly variable. The description of the main anatomical features of the calcarine sulcus obtained from our study can be used as a reference for fMRI exploration and is useful for brain surgery.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Laurent Tatu
- Department of Anatomy, UFR Sante, Besancon, France
- Department of Neurology, CHU, Besancon, France
| | | | - Sami Fadoul
- Department of Radiology, Doctors Center, Beirut, Lebanon
| | - Fabrice Vuillier
- Department of Anatomy, UFR Sante, Besancon, France
- Department of Neurology, CHU, Besancon, France
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Niu Y, Wang B, Zhou M, Xue J, Shapour H, Cao R, Cui X, Wu J, Xiang J. Dynamic Complexity of Spontaneous BOLD Activity in Alzheimer's Disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment Using Multiscale Entropy Analysis. Front Neurosci 2018; 12:677. [PMID: 30327587 PMCID: PMC6174248 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00677] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/27/2018] [Accepted: 09/07/2018] [Indexed: 12/31/2022] Open
Abstract
Alzheimer's disease (AD) is characterized by progressive deterioration of brain function among elderly people. Studies revealed aberrant correlations in spontaneous blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signals in resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) over a wide range of temporal scales. However, the study of the temporal dynamics of BOLD signals in subjects with AD and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) remains largely unexplored. Multiscale entropy (MSE) analysis is a method for estimating the complexity of finite time series over multiple time scales. In this research, we applied MSE analysis to investigate the abnormal complexity of BOLD signals using the rs-fMRI data from the Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging initiative (ADNI) database. There were 30 normal controls (NCs), 33 early MCI (EMCI), 32 late MCI (LMCI), and 29 AD patients. Following preprocessing of the BOLD signals, whole-brain MSE maps across six time scales were generated using the Complexity Toolbox. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) analysis on the MSE maps of four groups revealed significant differences in the thalamus, insula, lingual gyrus and inferior occipital gyrus, superior frontal gyrus and olfactory cortex, supramarginal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, and middle temporal gyrus on multiple time scales. Compared with the NC group, MCI and AD patients had significant reductions in the complexity of BOLD signals and AD patients demonstrated lower complexity than that of the MCI subjects. Additionally, the complexity of BOLD signals from the regions of interest (ROIs) was found to be significantly associated with cognitive decline in patient groups on multiple time scales. Consequently, the complexity or MSE of BOLD signals may provide an imaging biomarker of cognitive impairments in MCI and AD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yan Niu
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| | - Bin Wang
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
- Department of Radiology, First Hospital of Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan, China
| | - Mengni Zhou
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| | - Jiayue Xue
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| | - Habib Shapour
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| | - Rui Cao
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| | - Xiaohong Cui
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| | - Jinglong Wu
- Key Laboratory of Biomimetic Robots and Systems, Ministry of Education, Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing, China
- Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University, Okayama, Japan
| | - Jie Xiang
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Geuzebroek AC, van den Berg AV. Eccentricity scale independence for scene perception in the first tens of milliseconds. J Vis 2018; 18:9. [DOI: 10.1167/18.9.9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Anna C. Geuzebroek
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | - Albert V. van den Berg
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Wang B, Li T, Niu Y, Xiang J, Cheng J, Liu B, Zhang H, Yan T, Kanazawa S, Wu J. Differences in neural responses to ipsilateral stimuli in wide-view fields between face- and house-selective areas. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0192532. [PMID: 29451872 PMCID: PMC5815592 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192532] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/22/2017] [Accepted: 01/25/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Category-selective brain areas exhibit varying levels of neural activity to ipsilaterally presented stimuli. However, in face- and house-selective areas, the neural responses evoked by ipsilateral stimuli in the peripheral visual field remain unclear. In this study, we displayed face and house images using a wide-view visual presentation system while performing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The face-selective areas (fusiform face area (FFA) and occipital face area (OFA)) exhibited intense neural responses to ipsilaterally presented images, whereas the house-selective areas (parahippocampal place area (PPA) and transverse occipital sulcus (TOS)) exhibited substantially smaller and even negative neural responses to the ipsilaterally presented images. We also found that the category preferences of the contralateral and ipsilateral neural responses were similar. Interestingly, the face- and house-selective areas exhibited neural responses to ipsilateral images that were smaller than the responses to the contralateral images. Multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA) was implemented to evaluate the difference between the contralateral and ipsilateral responses. The classification accuracies were much greater than those expected by chance. The classification accuracies in the FFA were smaller than those in the PPA and TOS. The closer eccentricities elicited greater classification accuracies in the PPA and TOS. We propose that these ipsilateral neural responses might be interpreted by interhemispheric communication through intrahemispheric connectivity of white matter connection and interhemispheric connectivity via the corpus callosum and occipital white matter connection. Furthermore, the PPA and TOS likely have weaker interhemispheric communication than the FFA and OFA, particularly in the peripheral visual field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bin Wang
- College of Computer Science and Technology, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, Shanxi, China
- Department of Radiology, First Hospital of Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan, Shanxi, China
- * E-mail: (BW); (TY); (HZ); (JW)
| | - Ting Li
- College of Computer Science and Technology, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, Shanxi, China
| | - Yan Niu
- College of Computer Science and Technology, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, Shanxi, China
| | - Jie Xiang
- College of Computer Science and Technology, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, Shanxi, China
| | - Junjie Cheng
- College of Computer Science and Technology, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, Shanxi, China
| | - Bo Liu
- Department of Radiology, First Hospital of Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan, Shanxi, China
| | - Hui Zhang
- Department of Radiology, First Hospital of Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan, Shanxi, China
- * E-mail: (BW); (TY); (HZ); (JW)
| | - Tianyi Yan
- School of Life Science, Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing, China
- * E-mail: (BW); (TY); (HZ); (JW)
| | - Susumu Kanazawa
- Graduate School of Medicine, Dentistry, Pharmaceutical Sciences, Okayama University, Okayama, Japan
| | - Jinglong Wu
- Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University, Okayama, Japan
- Key Laboratory of Biomimetic Robots and Systems, Ministry of Education, Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing, China
- * E-mail: (BW); (TY); (HZ); (JW)
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Bergsma DP, Elshout JA, van den Berg AV. Segregation of Spontaneous and Training Induced Recovery from Visual Field Defects in Subacute Stroke Patients. Front Neurol 2017; 8:681. [PMID: 29326645 PMCID: PMC5736566 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00681] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/13/2017] [Accepted: 11/29/2017] [Indexed: 12/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Whether rehabilitation after stroke profits from an early start is difficult to establish as the contributions of spontaneous recovery and treatment are difficult to tease apart. Here, we use a novel training design to dissociate these components for visual rehabilitation of subacute stroke patients with visual field defects such as hemianopia. Visual discrimination training was started within 6 weeks after stroke in 17 patients. Spontaneous and training-induced recoveries were distinguished by training one-half of the defect for 8 weeks, while monitoring spontaneous recovery in the other (control) half of the defect. Next, trained and control regions were swapped, and training continued for another 8 weeks. The same paradigm was also applied to seven chronic patients for whom spontaneous recovery can be excluded and changes in the control half of the defect point to a spillover effect of training. In both groups, field stability was assessed during a no-intervention period. Defect reduction was significantly greater in the trained part of the defect than in the simultaneously untrained part of the defect irrespective of training onset (p = 0.001). In subacute patients, training contributed about twice as much to their defect reduction as the spontaneous recovery. Goal Attainment Scores were significantly and positively correlated with the total defect reduction (p = 0.01), percentage increase reading speed was significantly and positively correlated with the defect reduction induced by training (epoch 1: p = 0.0044; epoch 2: p = 0.023). Visual training adds significantly to the spontaneous recovery of visual field defects, both during training in the early and the chronic stroke phase. However, field recovery as a result of training in this subacute phase was as large as in the chronic phase. This suggests that patients benefited primarily of early onset training by gaining access to a larger visual field sooner.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Douwe P Bergsma
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Section of Biophysics, Donders Centre for Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Joris A Elshout
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Section of Biophysics, Donders Centre for Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Albert V van den Berg
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Section of Biophysics, Donders Centre for Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Mikellidou K, Kurzawski JW, Frijia F, Montanaro D, Greco V, Burr DC, Morrone MC. Area Prostriata in the Human Brain. Curr Biol 2017; 27:3056-3060.e3. [PMID: 28966090 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.065] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2017] [Revised: 05/31/2017] [Accepted: 08/25/2017] [Indexed: 01/28/2023]
Abstract
Area prostriata is a cortical area at the fundus of the calcarine sulcus, described anatomically in humans [1-5] and other primates [6-9]. It is lightly myelinated and lacks the clearly defined six-layer structure evident throughout the cerebral cortex, with a thinner layer 4 and thicker layer 2 [10], characteristic of limbic cortex [11]. In the marmoset and rhesus monkey, area prostriata has cortical connections with MT+ [12], the cingulate motor cortex [8], the auditory cortex [13], the orbitofrontal cortex, and the frontal polar cortices [14]. Here we use functional magnetic resonance together with a wide-field projection system to study its functional properties in humans. With population receptive field mapping [15], we show that area prostriata has a complete representation of the visual field, clearly distinct from the adjacent area V1. As in the marmoset, the caudal-dorsal border of human prostriata-abutting V1-represents the far peripheral visual field, with eccentricities decreasing toward its rostral boundary. Area prostriata responds strongly to very fast motion, greater than 500°/s. The functional properties of area prostriata suggest that it may serve to alert the brain quickly to fast visual events, particularly in the peripheral visual field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kyriaki Mikellidou
- Department of Translational Research on New Technologies in Medicine and Surgery, University of Pisa, via Savi 10, 56126 Pisa, Italy.
| | - Jan W Kurzawski
- Stella Maris Scientific Institute, Viale del Tirreno, 331, 56128 Calambrone, Italy; Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Pharmacology and Child Health, University of Florence, Via di San Salvi 12, 50139 Florence, Italy
| | - Francesca Frijia
- Unit of Neuroradiology, Fondazione CNR/Regione Toscana G. Monasterio, Via Giuseppe Moruzzi 1, 56124 Pisa, Italy
| | - Domenico Montanaro
- Unit of Neuroradiology, Fondazione CNR/Regione Toscana G. Monasterio, Via Giuseppe Moruzzi 1, 56124 Pisa, Italy
| | - Vincenzo Greco
- Istituto Nazionale di Ottica, CNR, Largo Enrico Fermi 6, 50125 Florence, Italy
| | - David C Burr
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Pharmacology and Child Health, University of Florence, Via di San Salvi 12, 50139 Florence, Italy; Neuroscience Institute, CNR, Via Giuseppe Moruzzi 1, 56124 Pisa, Italy
| | - Maria Concetta Morrone
- Department of Translational Research on New Technologies in Medicine and Surgery, University of Pisa, via Savi 10, 56126 Pisa, Italy; Stella Maris Scientific Institute, Viale del Tirreno, 331, 56128 Calambrone, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Xia J, Wang P, Chen Q. Neural Correlates underlying Size Constancy in Virtual Three-Dimensional Space. Sci Rep 2017; 7:3279. [PMID: 28607416 PMCID: PMC5468224 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-03652-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2016] [Accepted: 05/03/2017] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
The perceived size of an object remains relatively constant although its retinal size keeps decreasing as the object moves away along the depth dimension of the 3D space, i.e. size constancy. Neural mechanisms generating size constancy in virtual 3D space, however, remain poorly understood. By constructing a virtual 3D world in the MR scanner, we positioned the same 3D objects either near or far from the observers so that the near and far objects were perceived as having the same physical size despite their differences in retinal size. To control for the effect of differential retinal size, an additional 2D condition was introduced: a large and a small object, with matched retinal images as the near and far objects in the 3D condition, respectively, were presented on a 2D screen. Differences in retinal size activated overlapped areas in bilateral inferior occipital gyrus (IOG) in both experiments. The overlapped areas in IOG, however, showed different patterns of functional connectivity with different neural networks, depending on the perceived size of objects. In particular, IOG showed enhanced connectivity with bilateral superior parietal cortex in the 2D condition, but with inferior temporal and prefrontal cortex in the virtual 3D condition, i.e., size constancy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jing Xia
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China
| | - Pengfei Wang
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China.,Center for Psychology and Brain Science and Department of Psychology, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou, 510006, China
| | - Qi Chen
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China. .,Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China.
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Ide JS, Tung HC, Yang CT, Tseng YC, Li CSR. Barratt Impulsivity in Healthy Adults Is Associated with Higher Gray Matter Concentration in the Parietal Occipital Cortex that Represents Peripheral Visual Field. Front Hum Neurosci 2017; 11:222. [PMID: 28522966 PMCID: PMC5415556 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00222] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2017] [Accepted: 04/18/2017] [Indexed: 01/02/2023] Open
Abstract
Impulsivity is a personality trait of clinical importance. Extant research focuses on fronto-striatal mechanisms of impulsivity and how executive functions are compromised in impulsive individuals. Imaging studies employing voxel based morphometry highlighted impulsivity-related changes in gray matter concentrations in a wide array of cerebral structures. In particular, whereas prefrontal cortical areas appear to show structural alterations in individuals with a neuropsychiatric condition, the findings are less than consistent in the healthy population. Here, in a sample (n = 113) of young adults assessed for Barratt impulsivity, we controlled for age, gender and alcohol use, and showed that higher impulsivity score is associated with increased gray matter volume (GMV) in bilateral medial parietal and occipital cortices known to represent the peripheral visual field. When impulsivity components were assessed, we observed that this increase in parieto-occipital cortical volume is correlated with inattention and non-planning but not motor subscore. In a separate behavioral experiment of 10 young adults, we demonstrated that impulsive individuals are more vulnerable to the influence of a distractor on target detection in an attention task. If replicated, these findings together suggest aberrant visual attention as a neural correlate of an impulsive personality trait in neurotypical individuals and need to be reconciled with the literature that focuses on frontal dysfunctions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jaime S Ide
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of MedicineNew Haven, CT, USA
| | - Hsiang C Tung
- Department of Psychology, National Cheng Kung UniversityTainan, Taiwan
| | - Cheng-Ta Yang
- Department of Psychology, National Cheng Kung UniversityTainan, Taiwan
| | - Yuan-Chi Tseng
- Department of Industrial Design, National Cheng Kung UniversityTainan, Taiwan
| | - Chiang-Shan R Li
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of MedicineNew Haven, CT, USA.,Department of Neuroscience, Yale UniversityNew Haven, CT, USA.,Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Yale UniversityNew Haven, CT, USA.,Beijing Huilongguan HospitalBeijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Direct and indirect parieto-medial temporal pathways for spatial navigation in humans: evidence from resting-state functional connectivity. Brain Struct Funct 2016; 222:1945-1957. [DOI: 10.1007/s00429-016-1318-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2015] [Accepted: 09/24/2016] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
|
31
|
Vindras P, Blangero A, Ota H, Reilly KT, Rossetti Y, Pisella L. The Pointing Errors in Optic Ataxia Reveal the Role of "Peripheral Magnification" of the PPC. Front Integr Neurosci 2016; 10:27. [PMID: 27507938 PMCID: PMC4960242 DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2016.00027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2016] [Accepted: 07/11/2016] [Indexed: 12/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Interaction with visual objects in the environment requires an accurate correspondence between visual space and its internal representation within the brain. Many clinical conditions involve some impairment in visuo-motor control and the errors created by the lesion of a specific brain region are neither random nor uninformative. Modern approaches to studying the neuropsychology of action require powerful data-driven analyses and error modeling in order to understand the function of the lesioned areas. In the present paper we carried out mixed-effect analyses of the pointing errors of seven optic ataxia patients and seven control subjects. We found that a small parameter set is sufficient to explain the pointing errors produced by unilateral optic ataxia patients. In particular, the extremely stereotypical errors made when pointing toward the contralesional visual field can be fitted by mathematical models similar to those used to model central magnification in cortical or sub-cortical structure(s). Our interpretation is that visual areas that contain this footprint of central magnification guide pointing movements when the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) is damaged and that the functional role of the PPC is to actively compensate for the under-representation of peripheral vision that accompanies central magnification. Optic ataxia misreaching reveals what would be hand movement accuracy and precision if the human motor system did not include elaborated corrective processes for reaching and grasping to non-foveated targets.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Philippe Vindras
- ImpAct Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center CRNL, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292 and University Claude Bernard Lyon I Villeurbanne, France
| | | | - Hisaaki Ota
- Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Health Sciences, Sapporo Medical University Sapporo, Japan
| | - Karen T Reilly
- ImpAct Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center CRNL, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292 and University Claude Bernard Lyon I Villeurbanne, France
| | - Yves Rossetti
- ImpAct Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center CRNL, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292 and University Claude Bernard Lyon I Villeurbanne, France
| | - Laure Pisella
- ImpAct Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center CRNL, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292 and University Claude Bernard Lyon I Villeurbanne, France
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Capilla A, Melcón M, Kessel D, Calderón R, Pazo-Álvarez P, Carretié L. Retinotopic mapping of visual event-related potentials. Biol Psychol 2016; 118:114-125. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.05.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2016] [Revised: 05/05/2016] [Accepted: 05/24/2016] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
33
|
Elshout JA, van Asten F, Hoyng CB, Bergsma DP, van den Berg AV. Visual Rehabilitation in Chronic Cerebral Blindness: A Randomized Controlled Crossover Study. Front Neurol 2016; 7:92. [PMID: 27379011 PMCID: PMC4911356 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00092] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2016] [Accepted: 06/03/2016] [Indexed: 01/22/2023] Open
Abstract
The treatment of patients suffering from cerebral blindness following stroke is a topic of much recent interest. Several types of treatment are under investigation, such as substitution with prisms and compensation training of saccades. A third approach, aimed at vision restitution is controversial, as a proper controlled study design is missing. In the current study, 27 chronic stroke patients with homonymous visual field defects were trained at home with a visual training device. We used a discrimination task for two types of stimuli: a static point stimulus and a new optic flow-discontinuity stimulus. Using a randomized controlled crossover design, each patient received two successive training rounds, one with high contrast stimuli in their affected hemifield (test) and one round with low-contrast stimuli in their intact hemifield (control). Goldmann and Humphrey perimetry were performed at the start of the study and following each training round. In addition, reading performance was measured. Goldmann perimetry revealed a statistically significant reduction of the visual field defect after the test training, but not after the control training or after no intervention. For both training rounds combined, Humphrey perimetry revealed that the effect of a directed training (sensitivity change in trained hemifield) exceeded that of an undirected training (sensitivity change in untrained hemifield). The interaction between trained and tested hemifield was just above the threshold of significance (p = 0.058). Interestingly, reduction of the field defect assessed by Goldmann perimetry increases with the difference between defect size as measured by Humphrey and Goldmann perimetry prior to training. Moreover, improvement of visual sensitivity measured by Humphrey perimetry increases with the fraction of non-responsive elements (i.e., more relative field loss) in Humphrey perimetry prior to training. Reading speed revealed a significant improvement after training. Our findings demonstrate that our training can result in reduction of the visual field. Improved reading performance after defect training further supports the significance of our training for improvement in daily life activities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joris A Elshout
- Section of Biophysics, Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Centre for Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre , Nijmegen , Netherlands
| | - Freekje van Asten
- Department of Ophthalmology, Radboud University Medical Center , Nijmegen , Netherlands
| | - Carel B Hoyng
- Department of Ophthalmology, Radboud University Medical Center , Nijmegen , Netherlands
| | - Douwe P Bergsma
- Section of Biophysics, Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Centre for Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre , Nijmegen , Netherlands
| | - Albert V van den Berg
- Section of Biophysics, Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Centre for Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre , Nijmegen , Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Wang B, Guo J, Yan T, Ohno S, Kanazawa S, Huang Q, Wu J. Neural Responses to Central and Peripheral Objects in the Lateral Occipital Cortex. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:54. [PMID: 26924972 PMCID: PMC4759278 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2015] [Accepted: 02/04/2016] [Indexed: 01/30/2023] Open
Abstract
Human object recognition and classification depend on the retinal location where the object is presented and decrease as eccentricity increases. The lateral occipital complex (LOC) is thought to be preferentially involved in the processing of objects, and its neural responses exhibit category biases to objects presented in the central visual field. However, the nature of LOC neural responses to central and peripheral objects remains largely unclear. In the present study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and a wide-view presentation system to investigate neural responses to four categories of objects (faces, houses, animals, and cars) in the primary visual cortex (V1) and the lateral visual cortex, including the LOC and the retinotopic areas LO-1 and LO-2. In these regions, the neural responses to objects decreased as the distance between the location of presentation and center fixation increased, which is consistent with the diminished perceptual ability that was found for peripherally presented images. The LOC and LO-2 exhibited significantly positive neural responses to all eccentricities (0–55°), but LO-1 exhibited significantly positive responses only to central eccentricities (0–22°). By measuring the ratio relative to V1 (RRV1), we further demonstrated that eccentricity, category and the interaction between them significantly affected neural processing in these regions. LOC, LO-1, and LO-2 exhibited larger RRV1s when stimuli were presented at an eccentricity of 0° compared to when they were presented at the greater eccentricities. In LOC and LO-2, the RRV1s for images of faces, animals and cars showed an increasing trend when the images were presented at eccentricities of 11 to 33°. However, the RRV1s for houses showed a decreasing trend in LO-1 and no difference in the LOC and LO-2. We hypothesize, that when houses and the images in the other categories were presented in the peripheral visual field, they were processed via different strategies in the lateral visual cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bin Wang
- College of Computer Science and Technology, Taiyuan University of Technology Taiyuan, China
| | - Jiayue Guo
- Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University Okayama, Japan
| | - Tianyi Yan
- School of Life Science, Beijing Institute of TechnologyBeijing, China; Key Laboratory of Convergence Medical Engineering System and Healthcare Technology, The Ministry of Industry and Information Technology, Beijing Institute of TechnologyBeijing, China
| | - Seiichiro Ohno
- Department of Radiology, Okayama University Hospital, Okayama University Okayama, Japan
| | - Susumu Kanazawa
- Graduate School of Medicine, Dentistry, Pharmaceutical Sciences, Okayama University Okayama, Japan
| | - Qiang Huang
- Key Laboratory of Biomimetic Robots and Systems, Ministry of Education, Beijing Institute of Technology Beijing, China
| | - Jinglong Wu
- Key Laboratory of Biomimetic Robots and Systems, Ministry of Education, Beijing Institute of TechnologyBeijing, China; Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama UniversityOkayama, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Retinotopy and attention to the face and house images in the human visual cortex. Exp Brain Res 2016; 234:1623-35. [PMID: 26838358 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-016-4562-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2015] [Accepted: 01/13/2016] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Abstract
Attentional modulation of the neural activities in human visual areas has been well demonstrated. However, the retinotopic activities that are driven by face and house images and attention to face and house images remain unknown. In the present study, we used images of faces and houses to estimate the retinotopic activities that were driven by both the images and attention to the images, driven by attention to the images, and driven by the images. Generally, our results show that both face and house images produced similar retinotopic activities in visual areas, which were only observed in the attention + stimulus and the attention conditions, but not in the stimulus condition. The fusiform face area (FFA) responded to faces that were presented on the horizontal meridian, whereas parahippocampal place area (PPA) rarely responded to house at any visual field. We further analyzed the amplitudes of the neural responses to the target wedge. In V1, V2, V3, V3A, lateral occipital area 1 (LO-1), and hV4, the neural responses to the attended target wedge were significantly greater than those to the unattended target wedge. However, in LO-2, ventral occipital areas 1 and 2 (VO-1 and VO-2) and FFA and PPA, the differences were not significant. We proposed that these areas likely have large fields of attentional modulation for face and house images and exhibit responses to both the target wedge and the background stimuli. In addition, we proposed that the absence of retinotopic activity in the stimulus condition might imply no perceived difference between the target wedge and the background stimuli.
Collapse
|
36
|
Dundon NM, Bertini C, Làdavas E, Sabel BA, Gall C. Visual rehabilitation: visual scanning, multisensory stimulation and vision restoration trainings. Front Behav Neurosci 2015; 9:192. [PMID: 26283935 PMCID: PMC4515568 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00192] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2015] [Accepted: 07/09/2015] [Indexed: 12/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Neuropsychological training methods of visual rehabilitation for homonymous vision loss caused by postchiasmatic damage fall into two fundamental paradigms: “compensation” and “restoration”. Existing methods can be classified into three groups: Visual Scanning Training (VST), Audio-Visual Scanning Training (AViST) and Vision Restoration Training (VRT). VST and AViST aim at compensating vision loss by training eye scanning movements, whereas VRT aims at improving lost vision by activating residual visual functions by training light detection and discrimination of visual stimuli. This review discusses the rationale underlying these paradigms and summarizes the available evidence with respect to treatment efficacy. The issues raised in our review should help guide clinical care and stimulate new ideas for future research uncovering the underlying neural correlates of the different treatment paradigms. We propose that both local “within-system” interactions (i.e., relying on plasticity within peri-lesional spared tissue) and changes in more global “between-system” networks (i.e., recruiting alternative visual pathways) contribute to both vision restoration and compensatory rehabilitation, which ultimately have implications for the rehabilitation of cognitive functions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Neil M Dundon
- Department of Psychology, University of Bologna Bologna, Italy ; Centre for Studies and Research in Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Bologna Cesena, Italy
| | - Caterina Bertini
- Department of Psychology, University of Bologna Bologna, Italy ; Centre for Studies and Research in Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Bologna Cesena, Italy
| | - Elisabetta Làdavas
- Department of Psychology, University of Bologna Bologna, Italy ; Centre for Studies and Research in Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Bologna Cesena, Italy
| | - Bernhard A Sabel
- Medical Faculty, Institute of Medical Psychology, Otto-von-Guericke University of Magdeburg Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Carolin Gall
- Medical Faculty, Institute of Medical Psychology, Otto-von-Guericke University of Magdeburg Magdeburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Zhang S, Cate AD, Herron TJ, Kang X, Yund EW, Bao S, Woods DL. Functional and anatomical properties of human visual cortical fields. Vision Res 2015; 109:107-21. [PMID: 25661165 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.01.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/07/2014] [Revised: 12/29/2014] [Accepted: 01/26/2015] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
Human visual cortical fields (VCFs) vary in size and anatomical location across individual subjects. Here, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with retinotopic stimulation to identify VCFs on the cortical surface. We found that aligning and averaging VCF activations across the two hemispheres provided clear delineation of multiple retinotopic fields in visual cortex. The results show that VCFs have consistent locations and extents in different subjects that provide stable and accurate landmarks for functional and anatomical mapping. Interhemispheric comparisons revealed minor differences in polar angle and eccentricity tuning in comparable VCFs in the left and right hemisphere, and somewhat greater intersubject variability in the right than left hemisphere. We then used the functional boundaries to characterize the anatomical properties of VCFs, including fractional anisotropy (FA), magnetization transfer ratio (MTR) and the ratio of T1W and T2W images and found significant anatomical differences between VCFs and between hemispheres.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shouyu Zhang
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Medical Physics and Engineering, Peking University, Beijing 100871, PR China; Human Cognitive Neurophysiology Lab, VA Research Service, VA-NCHCS, 150 Muir Road, Martinez, CA 94553, USA; Department of Neurology and Center for Neuroscience, 4860 Y St., Suite 3700, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA
| | - Anthony D Cate
- Psychology Department, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg, VA 24061, USA
| | - Timothy J Herron
- Human Cognitive Neurophysiology Lab, VA Research Service, VA-NCHCS, 150 Muir Road, Martinez, CA 94553, USA
| | - Xiaojian Kang
- Human Cognitive Neurophysiology Lab, VA Research Service, VA-NCHCS, 150 Muir Road, Martinez, CA 94553, USA; Department of Neurology and Center for Neuroscience, 4860 Y St., Suite 3700, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA.
| | - E William Yund
- Human Cognitive Neurophysiology Lab, VA Research Service, VA-NCHCS, 150 Muir Road, Martinez, CA 94553, USA
| | - Shanglian Bao
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Medical Physics and Engineering, Peking University, Beijing 100871, PR China
| | - David L Woods
- Human Cognitive Neurophysiology Lab, VA Research Service, VA-NCHCS, 150 Muir Road, Martinez, CA 94553, USA; Department of Neurology and Center for Neuroscience, 4860 Y St., Suite 3700, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Yan T, Wang B, Geng Y, Yan Y, Mu N, Wu J, Guo Q, Tang X, Zeng Y, Peng Y. Contrast response functions with wide-view stimuli in the human visual cortex. Perception 2014; 43:677-93. [PMID: 25223111 DOI: 10.1068/p7640] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
In this manuscript, using a novel wide-view visual presentation system that we developed for vision research and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we studied contrast response functions in regions of the brain that are central and peripheral to the entire set of visual areas (V1, V2, V3, V3A, MT+), regions that have not been all investigated in previous vision research. Under the stimulus conditions which were 0-20 deg, 20-40 deg, and 40-60 deg eccentricity black-and-white checkerboard patterns, we measured the blood oxygenation level-dependent fMRI contrast response at five contrast levels (6, 12, 24, 48, and 96%) in the visual areas. On the basis of these data, the central and pericentral visual areas had low-contrast gain, whereas the peripheral visual areas had high-contrast gain. In addition, our results showed that the signals fundamentally shift during visual processing through posterior visual cortical areas (V1, V2, and V3) to superior visual cortical areas (V3A and MT+).
Collapse
|
39
|
Yu HH, Chaplin TA, Rosa MGP. Representation of central and peripheral vision in the primate cerebral cortex: Insights from studies of the marmoset brain. Neurosci Res 2014; 93:47-61. [PMID: 25242578 DOI: 10.1016/j.neures.2014.09.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2014] [Revised: 08/27/2014] [Accepted: 08/28/2014] [Indexed: 01/06/2023]
Abstract
How the visual field is represented by neurons in the cerebral cortex is one of the most basic questions in visual neuroscience. However, research to date has focused heavily on the small part of the visual field within, and immediately surrounding the fovea. Studies on the cortical representation of the full visual field in the primate brain are still scarce. We have been investigating this issue with electrophysiological and anatomical methods, taking advantage of the small and lissencephalic marmoset brain, which allows easy access to the representation of the full visual field in many cortical areas. This review summarizes our main findings to date, and relates the results to a broader question: is the peripheral visual field processed in a similar manner to the central visual field, but with lower spatial acuity? Given the organization of the visual cortex, the issue can be addressed by asking: (1) Is visual information processed in the same way within a single cortical area? and (2) Are different cortical areas specialized for different parts of the visual field? The electrophysiological data from the primary visual cortex indicate that many aspects of spatiotemporal computation are remarkably similar across the visual field, although subtle variations are detectable. Our anatomical and electrophysiological studies of the extrastriate cortex, on the other hand, suggest that visual processing in the far peripheral visual field is likely to involve a distinct network of specialized cortical areas, located in the depths of the calcarine sulcus and interhemispheric fissure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H-H Yu
- Department of Physiology, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia; Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for Integrative Brain Function, Monash University Node, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia.
| | - T A Chaplin
- Department of Physiology, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia; Monash Vision Group, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia
| | - M G P Rosa
- Department of Physiology, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia; Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for Integrative Brain Function, Monash University Node, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia; Monash Vision Group, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Carr SJ, Borreggine K, Heilman J, Griswold M, Walter BL. Novel magnetomechanical MR compatible vibrational device for producing kinesthetic illusion during fMRI. Med Phys 2014; 40:112303. [PMID: 24320459 DOI: 10.1118/1.4824695] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/22/2023] Open
Abstract
PURPOSE Functional MRI (fMRI) can provide insights into the functioning of the sensorimotor system, which is of particular interest in studying people with movement disorders or chronic pain conditions. This creates a demand for manipulanda that can fit and operate within the environment of a MRI scanner. Here, the authors present a magnetomechanical device that delivers a vibrotactile sensation to the skin with a force of approximately 9 N. METHODS MRI compatibility of the device was tested in a 3 T scanner using a phantom to simulate the head. Preliminary investigation into the effectiveness of the device at producing cortical and subcortical activity was also conducted with a group of seven healthy subjects. The vibration was applied to the right extensor carpi ulnaris tendon to induce a kinesthetic illusion of flexion and extension of the wrist. RESULTS The MRI compatibility tests showed the device did not produce image artifacts and the generated electromagnetic field did not disrupt the static magnetic field of the scanner or its operation. The subject group results showed activity in the contralateral putamen, premotor cortex, and dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex. Ipsilaterally, there was increased activity in the superior and inferior parietal lobules. Areas that activated bilaterally included the thalamus, anterior cingulate, secondary somatosensory areas (S2), temporal lobes, and visual association areas. CONCLUSIONS This device offers an effective tool with precise control over the vibratory stimulus, delivering higher forces than some other types of devices (e.g., piezoelectric actuators). It can be useful for investigating sensory systems and sensorimotor integration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah J Carr
- Department of Neurology, Case Western Reserve University, 10900 Euclid Avenue, Cleveland, Ohio 44106
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
A frequency-tagging electrophysiological method to identify central and peripheral visual field deficits. Doc Ophthalmol 2014; 129:17-26. [PMID: 24817488 DOI: 10.1007/s10633-014-9439-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/21/2014] [Accepted: 04/29/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The aim of this study was to develop a fast and efficient electrophysiological protocol to examine the visual field's integrity, which would be useful in pediatric testing. METHODS Steady-state visual-evoked potentials (ssVEPs) to field-specific radial checkerboards flickering at two cycle frequencies (7.5 and 6 Hz for central and peripheral stimulations, respectively) recorded at Oz were collected from 22 participants from 5 to 34 years old and from 5 visually impaired adolescents (12-16 years old). Responses from additional leads (POz, O1, O2), and the impact of gaze deviation on the signals, were also investigated in a subgroup of participants. RESULTS Steady-state visual-evoked potentials responses were similar at all electrode sites, although the signal from the central stimulation was significantly higher at Oz and was highly sensitive in detecting gaze deviation. No effect of age or sex was found, indicating similar ssVEP responses between adults and healthy children. Visual acuity was related to the central signal when comparing healthy participants with four central visual impaired adolescents. Clinical validation of our electrophysiological protocol was also achieved in a 15-year-old adolescent with a severe peripheral visual deficit, as assessed with Goldmann perimetry. CONCLUSIONS A single electrode over Oz is sufficient to gather both central and peripheral visual signals and also to control for gaze deviation. Our method presents several advantages in evaluating visual fields integrity, as it is fast, reliable, and efficient, and applicable in children as young as 5 years old. However, a larger sample of healthy children should be tested to establish clinical norms.
Collapse
|
42
|
Elvsåshagen T, Moberget T, Bøen E, Hol PK, Malt UF, Andersson S, Westlye LT. The surface area of early visual cortex predicts the amplitude of the visual evoked potential. Brain Struct Funct 2014; 220:1229-36. [DOI: 10.1007/s00429-013-0703-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/28/2013] [Accepted: 12/30/2013] [Indexed: 01/17/2023]
|
43
|
Huang RS, Sereno MI. Bottom-up Retinotopic Organization Supports Top-down Mental Imagery. Open Neuroimag J 2013; 7:58-67. [PMID: 24478813 PMCID: PMC3905356 DOI: 10.2174/1874440001307010058] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/28/2013] [Revised: 12/22/2013] [Accepted: 12/22/2013] [Indexed: 02/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Finding a path between locations is a routine task in daily life. Mental navigation is often used to plan a route
to a destination that is not visible from the current location. We first used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
and surface-based averaging methods to find high-level brain regions involved in imagined navigation between locations
in a building very familiar to each participant. This revealed a mental navigation network that includes the precuneus,
retrosplenial cortex (RSC), parahippocampal place area (PPA), occipital place area (OPA), supplementary motor area
(SMA), premotor cortex, and areas along the medial and anterior intraparietal sulcus. We then visualized retinotopic maps
in the entire cortex using wide-field, natural scene stimuli in a separate set of fMRI experiments. This revealed five
distinct visual streams or ‘fingers’ that extend anteriorly into middle temporal, superior parietal, medial parietal,
retrosplenial and ventral occipitotemporal cortex. By using spherical morphing to overlap these two data sets, we showed
that the mental navigation network primarily occupies areas that also contain retinotopic maps. Specifically, scene-selective
regions RSC, PPA and OPA have a common emphasis on the far periphery of the upper visual field. These
results suggest that bottom-up retinotopic organization may help to efficiently encode scene and location information in
an eye-centered reference frame for top-down, internally generated mental navigation. This study pushes the border of
visual cortex further anterior than was initially expected.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruey-Song Huang
- Institute for Neural Computation, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
| | - Martin I Sereno
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA ; Cognitive, Perceptual, and Brain Sciences, University College London, London WC1H 0AP, United Kingdom ; Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck College, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Wang B, Yan T, Wu J, Chen K, Imajyo S, Ohno S, Kanazawa S. Regional neural response differences in the determination of faces or houses positioned in a wide visual field. PLoS One 2013; 8:e72728. [PMID: 23991147 PMCID: PMC3749153 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0072728] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/13/2013] [Accepted: 07/12/2013] [Indexed: 01/07/2023] Open
Abstract
In human visual cortex, the primary visual cortex (V1) is considered to be essential for visual information processing; the fusiform face area (FFA) and parahippocampal place area (PPA) are considered as face-selective region and places-selective region, respectively. Recently, a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study showed that the neural activity ratios between V1 and FFA were constant as eccentricities increasing in central visual field. However, in wide visual field, the neural activity relationships between V1 and FFA or V1 and PPA are still unclear. In this work, using fMRI and wide-view present system, we tried to address this issue by measuring neural activities in V1, FFA and PPA for the images of faces and houses aligning in 4 eccentricities and 4 meridians. Then, we further calculated ratio relative to V1 (RRV1) as comparing the neural responses amplitudes in FFA or PPA with those in V1. We found V1, FFA, and PPA showed significant different neural activities to faces and houses in 3 dimensions of eccentricity, meridian, and region. Most importantly, the RRV1s in FFA and PPA also exhibited significant differences in 3 dimensions. In the dimension of eccentricity, both FFA and PPA showed smaller RRV1s at central position than those at peripheral positions. In meridian dimension, both FFA and PPA showed larger RRV1s at upper vertical positions than those at lower vertical positions. In the dimension of region, FFA had larger RRV1s than PPA. We proposed that these differential RRV1s indicated FFA and PPA might have different processing strategies for encoding the wide field visual information from V1. These different processing strategies might depend on the retinal position at which faces or houses are typically observed in daily life. We posited a role of experience in shaping the information processing strategies in the ventral visual cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bin Wang
- School of Life Science, Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing, China
- Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University, Okayama, Japan
| | - Tianyi Yan
- School of Life Science, Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing, China
| | - Jinglong Wu
- School of Life Science, Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing, China
- Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University, Okayama, Japan
- * E-mail:
| | - Kewei Chen
- Computational Image Analysis Program, Banner Alzheimer Institute and Banner Good Samaritan PET Center, Phoenix, Arizona, United States of America
| | - Satoshi Imajyo
- Department of Radiology, Okayama University Hospital, Okayama University, Okayama, Japan
| | - Seiichiro Ohno
- Department of Radiology, Okayama University Hospital, Okayama University, Okayama, Japan
| | - Susumu Kanazawa
- Graduate School of Medicine, Dentistry, Pharmaceutical Sciences, Okayama University, Okayama, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Wu J, Wang B, Yang J, Hikino Y, Takahashi S, Yan T, Ohno S, Kanazawa S. Development of a method to present wide-view visual stimuli in MRI for peripheral visual studies. J Neurosci Methods 2013; 214:126-36. [PMID: 23376498 DOI: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.01.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/17/2012] [Revised: 01/17/2013] [Accepted: 01/23/2013] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
We developed a novel wide-view visual presentation system for fMRI studies. Computer-generated images were projected onto a hemispheric, translucent screen inside the MRI bore and were then back-projected onto a 52mm diameter screen. To achieve a wide field view, a spherical screen with a curvature radius of 30mm was placed 30mm away from the subjects' eyes. The subjects wore contact lenses that enabled them to focus on the screen, and the resulting visual field reached 120°. To evaluate the clarity and quality of the MRI images, a signal-to-noise ratio valuation experiment was performed. In addition, we successfully applied this visual presentation system to studies of visual retinotopic mapping and object perception neural function in the peripheral visual field. Our study demonstrated that the system is compatible with the MRI environment. Based on the wide-field mapping results, this system was more effective at mapping a checkerboard stimuli in V1-V3 from the central to peripheral visual fields. In higher-level visual areas, we successfully located several classical category-selective areas, including the face-selective area (FFA), occipital face area (OFA), house-selective area (PPA), transverse occipital sulcus (TOS), lateral occipital complex (LOC) and posterior fusiform area (pFs). In these areas, we found that the response amplitudes exhibited different decreasing trends with increasing eccentricity. In conclusion, we developed a simple, effective method for presenting wide-view visual stimuli within the MRI environment that can be applied to many kinds of fMRI studies of peripheral vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jinglong Wu
- Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University, Okayama, Japan.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|