1
|
Horn RR, Paletta S, Sandri Heidner G, Lewinski WJ, Bartel L, Gwon D, Murray NP. Effects of reinforcement sensitivity, impulsivity, and working memory on shoot/don't shoot performance in law enforcement officers and civilians under normal and high cognitive load. ERGONOMICS 2025:1-18. [PMID: 40020723 DOI: 10.1080/00140139.2025.2466013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2024] [Accepted: 01/29/2025] [Indexed: 03/03/2025]
Abstract
We compared shoot/don't shoot task (SDST) performance in law enforcement officers (LEOs) and non-officers under normal cognitive load, and under higher load induced by processing a complex dispatch message. We also examined the effects of participants' behavioural activation (BAS) and inhibition (BIS) systems, impulsivity, and working memory on SDST performance. Stimuli were videos of shoot and don't shoot variations performed by three actors. Dependent measures were shoot errors, fail-to-shoot errors, response time, and shooting accuracy. Compared to non-officers, LEOs were more accurate and adapted to make fewer fail-to-shoot errors. They were not faster, and did not make fewer shoot errors. Compared to normal cognitive load, under higher load, participants were quicker and more accurate, but made more shoot errors. The higher cognitive load condition revealed effects that were not present under normal load. Participants making more shoot errors had higher BAS, and BIS than those making fewer or no errors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert R Horn
- Department of Kinesiology, Montclair State University, Montclair, NJ, USA
| | - Skylar Paletta
- Department of Kinesiology, Montclair State University, Montclair, NJ, USA
| | | | | | | | - Daniel Gwon
- Department of Kinesiology, Montclair State University, Montclair, NJ, USA
| | - Nicholas P Murray
- Department of Kinesiology, East Carolina University, Greenville, NC, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Omeroglu FB, Li Y, Zaloom V, Curry J, Marquez A. The effects of music mood and binaural beats on academic advertising. Physiol Behav 2024; 288:114720. [PMID: 39442593 DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2024.114720] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2024] [Revised: 09/26/2024] [Accepted: 10/18/2024] [Indexed: 10/25/2024]
Abstract
How music affects our mood, cognition, and feelings has been studied extensively. Since the effect of music on mood and cognition has been demonstrated many times, it has found significant applications, particularly in advertising. In recent years, the use of music in advertising has grown significantly, with 86 % of advertisements now incorporating some form of musical stimuli. Our study specifically analyzed the effect of music mood in advertising while introducing the new concept of binaural beats. Conducted in a lab setting, the study incorporated biometric measures such as electroencephalography (EEG) and eye-tracking to enhance the research. The results revealed that calming music combined with binaural beats led to the highest levels of information retention and engagement, as indicated by increased left frontal beta power, relative theta power, and area of interest (AOI) dwell time percentages. Left frontal beta power is associated with increased attention and cognitive engagement, while relative theta power is linked to enhanced memory encoding and relaxation. The area of interest (AOI) dwell time percentages reflects the time participants focused on key areas of the advertisement, indicating higher attention levels. Although the study found unique connections between music mood and binaural beats, calming music with binaural beats produced the most favorable conditions for attention and memory retention. These findings provide valuable guidelines for future marketing strategies, particularly in academic advertising.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fatih Baha Omeroglu
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, Lamar University, Beaumont, TX, USA.
| | - Yueqing Li
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, Lamar University, Beaumont, TX, USA.
| | - Victor Zaloom
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, Lamar University, Beaumont, TX, USA.
| | - James Curry
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, Lamar University, Beaumont, TX, USA.
| | - Alberto Marquez
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, Lamar University, Beaumont, TX, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Firth J, Standen B, Sumich A, Fino E, Heym N. The neural correlates of reinforcement sensitivity theory: A systematic review of the frontal asymmetry and spectral power literature. Psychophysiology 2024; 61:e14594. [PMID: 38693649 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14594] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2023] [Revised: 12/10/2023] [Accepted: 03/27/2024] [Indexed: 05/03/2024]
Abstract
The original Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (oRST) proposes two systems of approach (BAS) and avoidance (BIS) motivation to underpin personality and behavior. The revised-RST (rRST) model separates avoidance motivation into passive (BIS; anxiety) and active (FFFS; fear) systems. Prior research has attempted to map RST onto lateralized frontal asymmetry to provide a neurophysiological marker of RST. The main aim is to examine the relationships of the o/rRST scales with trait (baseline) and state (manipulated through experimental paradigms) frontal asymmetry. A systematic review was conducted, resulting in 158 studies designated to neuroimaging research. In total, 54 studies were included in this review using either frontal asymmetry or spectral power. The results were split into three main categories: resting frontal alpha asymmetry (N = 23), emotional induction and state-related frontal alpha asymmetry (N = 20), and spectral analysis (N = 16). Findings indicated that BAS was associated with enhanced left frontal asymmetry at baseline and during state-related paradigms. Findings for BIS were more inconsistent, especially at rest, suggesting that BIS, in particular, may require active engagement with the environment. Only 9 of the 54 papers included used the revised RST model, highlighting the need for more rRST research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer Firth
- Division of Psychology, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, UK
| | - Bradley Standen
- Division of Psychology, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, UK
| | - Alexander Sumich
- Division of Psychology, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, UK
| | - Emanuele Fino
- Division of Psychology, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, UK
| | - Nadja Heym
- Division of Psychology, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, UK
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Maruo Y, Masaki H. Monetary reward enhances response inhibition processes manifested in No-go P3. Int J Psychophysiol 2024; 203:112410. [PMID: 39102986 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2024.112410] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/12/2024] [Revised: 07/16/2024] [Accepted: 08/01/2024] [Indexed: 08/07/2024]
Abstract
This study investigated the impact of motivational valence on No-go P3 and N2 by incorporating monetary rewards based on response outcomes. We also investigated how personality differences in terms of the behavioral inhibition system (BIS) and behavioral approach system (BAS) influenced No-go N2 and No-go P3. Twenty-eight participants performed Go/No-go tasks (80 % Go and 20 % No-go) under two conditions. In the reward condition, each correct-rejection trial for the No-go stimulus was rewarded with 10 yen (∼6 cents), whereas in the neutral condition, neither monetary rewards nor punishments were contingent on response outcomes. Individual responsiveness to punishment and rewards was evaluated using the BIS and BAS scales. The error rate was significantly lower in the reward condition than in the neutral condition. P3 amplitude for correct-rejection trials (i.e., preceding erroneous muscular activity on the wrong hand) was larger in the reward condition than in the neutral condition; however, N2 amplitudes did not differ between the two conditions. These results suggest that monetary rewards may enhance motor inhibition control. Individuals with a higher BIS score exhibited a larger No-go N2 for correct-rejection in the neutral condition. We conclude that No-go N2 amplitude is modulated by avoidance motivation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuya Maruo
- Department of Physical Education, Tokyo Women's College of Physical Education, Japan; Institute for Sports and Brain Science, Waseda University, Japan
| | - Hiroaki Masaki
- Faculty of Sport Sciences, Waseda University, Japan; Institute for Sports and Brain Science, Waseda University, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Li W, Huang B, Song Y, Hou L, Shi W. Altered neural mechanisms of deception in individuals with autistic traits. Brain Cogn 2023; 170:106005. [PMID: 37320929 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2023.106005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2023] [Revised: 06/02/2023] [Accepted: 06/04/2023] [Indexed: 06/17/2023]
Abstract
A successful deception involves making a decision, acting on it, and evaluating results. Here, we investigated deception in a non-clinical sample (n = 36) with varying autism traits using a coin-toss paradigm of active deception. The subjects were asked to react to the instructions by clicking one of the two boxes that could mislead their opponents, followed by feedback on their success or failure. During this reaction, their EEG activity was recorded, and the results suggested that people with high autistic traits exhibited longer reaction times and lower amplitude of P3 in the decision-making stage compared to individuals with low autistic traits. The feedback evaluation stage in the high autistic trait group elicited lower amplitude of FRN and P3. Overall, these results indicated that people with high autistic traits experienced difficulties in deceiving, which could be related to atypical neural mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wenrui Li
- School of Education, Shanghai Normal University, Shanghai 200000, China
| | - Bowen Huang
- School of Education, Shanghai Normal University, Shanghai 200000, China
| | - Youming Song
- Department of Psychology, College of Education Science, Yan'an University, Yan'an 716000, China
| | - Lulu Hou
- School of Education, Shanghai Normal University, Shanghai 200000, China
| | - Wendian Shi
- School of Education, Shanghai Normal University, Shanghai 200000, China.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Huang K, Chen D, Wang F, Yang L. Prediction of dispositional dialectical thinking from resting-state electroencephalography. Brain Behav 2021; 11:e2327. [PMID: 34423595 PMCID: PMC8442598 DOI: 10.1002/brb3.2327] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2021] [Revised: 07/20/2021] [Accepted: 08/02/2021] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
This study aims to explore the possibility of predicting the dispositional level of dialectical thinking using resting-state electroencephalography signals. Thirty-four participants completed a self-reported measure of dialectical thinking, and their resting-state electroencephalography was recorded. After wave filtration and eye movement removal, time-frequency electroencephalography signals were converted into four frequency domains: delta (1-4 Hz), theta (4-7 Hz), alpha (7-13 Hz), and beta (13-30 Hz). Functional principal component analysis with B-spline approximation was then applied for feature reduction. Five machine learning methods (support vector regression, least absolute shrinkage and selection operator, K-nearest neighbors, random forest, and gradient boosting decision tree) were applied to the reduced features for prediction. The model ensemble technique was used to create the best performing final model. The results showed that the alpha wave of the electroencephalography signal in the early period (12-15 s) contributed most to the prediction of dialectical thinking. With data-driven electrode selection (FC1, FCz, Fz, FC3, Cz, AFz), the prediction model achieved an average coefficient of determination of 0.45 on 200 random test sets. Furthermore, a significant positive correlation was found between the alpha value of standardized low-resolution electromagnetic tomography activity in the right dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and dialectical self-scale score. The prefrontal and midline alpha oscillations of resting electroencephalography are good predictors of the dispositional level of dialectical thinking, possibly reflecting these brain structures' involvement in dialectical thinking.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kun Huang
- Center for Statistical Science and Department of Industrial Engineering, Tsinghua University, Beijing, China
| | - Dian Chen
- Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, Tsinghua University, Beijing, China
| | - Fei Wang
- Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, Tsinghua University, Beijing, China.,Laboratory of Brain and Intelligence, Tsinghua University, Beijing, China
| | - Lijian Yang
- Center for Statistical Science and Department of Industrial Engineering, Tsinghua University, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Huber KB, Marsolek CJ. Do cerebral motivational asymmetries mediate the relationship between handedness and personality? Laterality 2021; 27:21-56. [PMID: 34238115 DOI: 10.1080/1357650x.2021.1942483] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Handedness has long been tied to personality, but detailed explanations for the association are lacking. Importantly for purposes of theory development, measures of approach and withdrawal associated with Big Five personality traits have also been traced back to activity in brain areas that relate to handedness. Specifically, increased right-hemisphere frontal activity appears to be linked to both withdrawal motivation and left/inconsistent-handedness, while increased left-hemisphere frontal activity is associated with approach motivation and right/consistent-handedness. Cerebral motivational asymmetries therefore present one plausible mechanism by which approach and withdrawal motivation could mediate the relationship between handedness and personality. We tested this possibility in a large online study (N = 499) in which participants completed multiple survey measures. Results indicated that approach/withdrawal motivation partially accounts for relationships between handedness and personality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katie B Huber
- Psychological Sciences Department, University of Wisconsin-River Falls, River Falls, WI, USA.,Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota-Twin Cities, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Chad J Marsolek
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota-Twin Cities, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Rodrigues J, Allen JJB, Müller M, Hewig J. Methods matter: An examination of factors that moderate predictions of the capability model concerning the relationship of frontal asymmetry to trait measures. Biol Psychol 2020; 158:107993. [PMID: 33259911 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107993] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2020] [Revised: 11/10/2020] [Accepted: 11/16/2020] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
The capability model of anterior asymmetry integrates trait-related and state-related frontal asymmetry research by proposing that frontal asymmetry is dependent on relevant traits if they are activated by a situation. However, differences in experimental design and EEG recording methods haven't been fully explored. We investigated 56 participants under three different situational paradigms (virtual T-maze, mental imagery, movies), varying the stimulus and type of measurement concerning frontal asymmetry. We predicted that "strong" situational manipulations (virtual T-maze, frontal asymmetry measured as event-related desynchronization) would eclipse relationships between frontal asymmetry and relevant traits, whereas "weaker" task manipulations, measured during longer time periods, would enhance relationships to relevant traits compared to frontal asymmetry at rest. The results confirmed these expectations, stressing the importance of stimulus characteristics, trait measures and recording methods with respect to the capability model. Additionally, a revision of the capability model to an inverse U-shaped quadratic relationship might be appropriate.
Collapse
|
9
|
Zhang J, Hua Y, Xiu L, Oei TP, Hu P. Resting state frontal alpha asymmetry predicts emotion regulation difficulties in impulse control. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.109870] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/03/2023]
|
10
|
Increases in theta CSD power and coherence during a calibrated stop-signal task: implications for goal-conflict processing and the Behavioural Inhibition System. PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE 2020; 2:e10. [PMID: 32435745 PMCID: PMC7219682 DOI: 10.1017/pen.2019.10] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/12/2019] [Revised: 09/03/2019] [Accepted: 09/17/2019] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
Psychologists have identified multiple different forms of conflict, such as information processing conflict and goal conflict. As such, there is a need to examine the similarities and differences in neurology between each form of conflict. To address this, we conducted a comprehensive electroencephalogram (EEG) analysis of Shadli, Glue, McIntosh, and McNaughton’s calibrated stop-signal task (SST) goal-conflict task. Specifically, we examined changes in scalp-wide current source density (CSD) power and coherence across a wide range of frequency bands during the calibrated SST (n = 34). We assessed differences in EEG between the high and low goal-conflict conditions using hierarchical analyses of variance (ANOVAs). We also related goal-conflict EEG to trait anxiety, neuroticism, Behavioural Inhibition System (BIS)-anxiety and revised BIS (rBIS) using regression analyses. We found that changes in CSD power during goal conflict were limited to increased midfrontocentral theta. Conversely, coherence increased across 23 scalp-wide theta region pairs and one frontal delta region pair. Finally, scalp-wide theta significantly predicted trait neuroticism but not trait anxiety, BIS-anxiety or rBIS. We conclude that goal conflict involves increased midfrontocentral CSD theta power and scalp-wide theta-dominated coherence. Therefore, compared with information processing conflict, goal conflict displays a similar EEG power profile of midfrontocentral theta but a much wider coherence profile. Furthermore, the increases in theta during goal conflict are the characteristic of BIS-driven activity. Therefore, future research should confirm whether these goal-conflict effects are driven by the BIS by examining whether the effects are attenuated by anxiolytic drugs. Overall, we have identified a unique network of goal-conflict EEG during the calibrated SST.
Collapse
|
11
|
Altered Processing of Visual Food Stimuli in Adolescents with Loss of Control Eating. Nutrients 2019; 11:nu11020210. [PMID: 30678145 PMCID: PMC6412983 DOI: 10.3390/nu11020210] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2018] [Revised: 01/14/2019] [Accepted: 01/16/2019] [Indexed: 12/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Loss of control eating (LOC) constitutes a common eating pathology in childhood and adolescence. Models developed for adult patients stress a biased processing of food-related stimuli as an important maintaining factor. To our knowledge, however, no EEG study to date investigated the processing of visual food stimuli in children or adolescents with LOC. Adolescents with at least one self-reported episode of LOC in the last four weeks and a matched control group completed a modified Go/NoGo task, with a numerical target or non-target stimulus being presented on one side of the screen and an irrelevant high-calorie food or neutral stimulus being presented on the opposite side. Mean P3 amplitudes were analyzed. In Go trials, the LOC group’s mean P3 amplitudes were comparable irrespective of distractor category, while for NoGo trials, mean P3 amplitudes were significantly higher when the distractor was a high-calorie food stimulus. This pattern was reversed in the control group. Results are interpreted in light of Gray’s reinforcement sensitivity theory. They might reflect altered processes of behavioral inhibition in adolescents with LOC upon confrontation with visual food stimuli.
Collapse
|
12
|
Käckenmester W, Kroencke L, Wacker J. Frontal asymmetry predicts the incentive value of perceptual information. Int J Psychophysiol 2018; 134:22-29. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.10.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/09/2018] [Revised: 09/24/2018] [Accepted: 10/02/2018] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
13
|
Geraldo A, Azeredo A, Pasion R, Dores AR, Barbosa F. Fostering advances to neuropsychological assessment based on the Research Domain Criteria: The bridge between cognitive functioning and physiology. Clin Neuropsychol 2018; 33:327-356. [DOI: 10.1080/13854046.2018.1523467] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Andreia Geraldo
- Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
- School of Health, Institute Polytechnic of Porto, Porto, Portugal
| | - Andreia Azeredo
- Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
| | - Rita Pasion
- Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
| | - Artemisa Rocha Dores
- Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
- School of Health, Institute Polytechnic of Porto, Porto, Portugal
| | - Fernando Barbosa
- Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Shadli SM, Kawe T, Martin D, McNaughton N, Neehoff S, Glue P. Ketamine Effects on EEG during Therapy of Treatment-Resistant Generalized Anxiety and Social Anxiety. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2018; 21:717-724. [PMID: 29718262 PMCID: PMC6070106 DOI: 10.1093/ijnp/pyy032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2018] [Accepted: 03/15/2018] [Indexed: 12/29/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Ketamine is swiftly effective in a range of neurotic disorders that are resistant to conventional antidepressant and anxiolytic drugs. The neural basis for its therapeutic action is unknown. Here we report the effects of ketamine on the EEG of patients with treatment-resistant generalized anxiety and social anxiety disorders. METHODS Twelve patients with refractory DSM-IV generalized anxiety disorder and/or social anxiety disorder provided EEG during 10 minutes of relaxation before and 2 hours after receiving double-blind drug administration. Three ascending ketamine dose levels (0.25, 0.5, and 1 mg/kg) and midazolam (0.01 mg/kg) were given at 1-week intervals to each patient, with the midazolam counterbalanced in dosing position across patients. Anxiety was assessed pre- and postdose with the Fear Questionnaire and HAM-A. RESULTS Ketamine dose-dependently improved Fear Questionnaire but not HAM-A scores, decreased EEG power most at low (delta) frequency, and increased it most at high (gamma) frequency. Only the decrease in medium-low (theta) frequency at right frontal sites predicted the effect of ketamine on the Fear Questionnaire. Ketamine produced no improvement in Higuchi's fractal dimension at any dose or systematic changes in frontal alpha asymmetry. CONCLUSIONS Ketamine may achieve its effects on treatment-resistant generalized anxiety disorder and social anxiety disorder through related mechanisms to the common reduction by conventional anxiolytic drugs in right frontal theta. However, in the current study midazolam did not have such an effect, and it remains to be determined whether, unlike conventional anxiolytics, ketamine changes right frontal theta when it is effective in treatment-resistant depression.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Tame Kawe
- Department of Psychology University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | - Daniel Martin
- Department of Psychology University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | - Neil McNaughton
- Department of Psychology University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | - Shona Neehoff
- Department of Psychological Medicine University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | - Paul Glue
- Department of Psychological Medicine University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Landes I, Bakos S, Kohls G, Bartling J, Schulte-Körne G, Greimel E. Altered neural processing of reward and punishment in adolescents with Major Depressive Disorder. J Affect Disord 2018; 232:23-33. [PMID: 29475180 DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.01.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/24/2016] [Revised: 01/02/2018] [Accepted: 01/29/2018] [Indexed: 01/16/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Altered reward and punishment function has been suggested as an important vulnerability factor for the development of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD). Prior ERP studies found evidence for neurophysiological dysfunctions in reinforcement processes in adults with MDD. To date, only few ERP studies have examined the neural underpinnings of reinforcement processing in adolescents diagnosed with MDD. The present event-related potential (ERP) study aimed to investigate neurophysiological mechanisms of anticipation and consumption of reward and punishment in adolescents with MDD in one comprehensive paradigm. METHOD During ERP recording, 25 adolescents with MDD and 29 healthy controls (12-17 years) completed a Monetary Incentive Delay Task comprising both a monetary reward and a monetary punishment condition. During anticipation, the cue-P3 signaling attentional allocation was recorded. During consumption, the feedback-P3 and Reward Positivity (RewP) were recorded to capture attentional allocation and outcome evaluation, respectively. RESULTS Compared to controls, adolescents with MDD showed prolonged cue-P3 latencies to reward cues. Furthermore, unlike controls, adolescents with MDD displayed shorter feedback-P3 latencies in the reward versus punishment condition. RewPs did not differ between groups. LIMITATIONS It remains unanswered whether the observed alterations in adolescent MDD represent a state or trait. CONCLUSIONS Delayed neural processing of reward cues corresponds to the clinical presentation of adolescent MDD with reduced motivational tendencies to obtain rewards. Relatively shorter feedback-P3 latencies in the reward versus punishment condition could indicate a high salience of performance-contingent reward. Frequent exposure of negatively biased adolescents with MDD to performance-contingent rewards might constitute a promising intervention approach.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- I Landes
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Munich, Munich, Germany
| | - S Bakos
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Munich, Munich, Germany
| | - G Kohls
- Child Neuropsychology Section, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics, and Psychotherapy, Medical Faculty, RWTH Aachen University Hospital, Aachen, Germany
| | - J Bartling
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Munich, Munich, Germany
| | - G Schulte-Körne
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Munich, Munich, Germany
| | - E Greimel
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Munich, Munich, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Kelley NJ, Hortensius R, Schutter DJ, Harmon-Jones E. The relationship of approach/avoidance motivation and asymmetric frontal cortical activity: A review of studies manipulating frontal asymmetry. Int J Psychophysiol 2017; 119:19-30. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.03.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2016] [Revised: 03/08/2017] [Accepted: 03/09/2017] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
|
17
|
Rodrigues J, Müller M, Mühlberger A, Hewig J. Mind the movement: Frontal asymmetry stands for behavioral motivation, bilateral frontal activation for behavior. Psychophysiology 2017; 55. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12908] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/26/2016] [Revised: 05/11/2017] [Accepted: 05/17/2017] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Johannes Rodrigues
- Institute of Psychology, Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg; Würzburg Germany
| | - Mathias Müller
- Institute of Psychology, Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg; Würzburg Germany
| | | | - Johannes Hewig
- Institute of Psychology, Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg; Würzburg Germany
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Mennella R, Sarlo M, Messerotti Benvenuti S, Buodo G, Mento G, Palomba D. The two faces of avoidance: Time-frequency correlates of motivational disposition in blood phobia. Psychophysiology 2017; 54:1606-1620. [PMID: 28580599 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12904] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2016] [Revised: 05/05/2017] [Accepted: 05/06/2017] [Indexed: 01/24/2023]
Abstract
Contrary to other phobias, individuals with blood phobia do not show a clear-cut withdrawal disposition from the feared stimulus. The study of response inhibition provides insights into reduced action disposition in blood phobia. Twenty individuals with and 20 without blood phobia completed an emotional go/no-go task including phobia-related pictures, as well as phobia-unrelated unpleasant, neutral, and pleasant stimuli. Behavioral results did not indicate a phobia-specific reduced action disposition in the phobic group. Time-frequency decomposition of event-related EEG data showed a reduction of right prefrontal activity, as indexed by an increase in alpha power (200 ms), for no-go mutilation trials in the phobic group but not in controls. Moreover, theta power (300 ms) increased specifically for phobia-related pictures in individuals with, but not without, blood phobia, irrespective of go or no-go trial types. Passive avoidance of phobia-related stimuli subtended by the increased alpha in the right prefrontal cortex, associated with increased emotional salience indexed by theta synchronization, represents a possible neurophysiological correlate of the conflicting motivational response in blood phobia. Through the novel use of time-frequency decomposition in an emotional go/no-go task, the present study contributed to clarifying the neurophysiological correlates of the overlapping motivational tendencies in blood phobia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rocco Mennella
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy.,Laboratoire de neurosciences cognitives, Département d'études cognitives, École normale supérieure, INSERM, PSL Research University, Paris, France
| | - Michela Sarlo
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy.,Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | | | - Giulia Buodo
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy.,Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | - Giovanni Mento
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | - Daniela Palomba
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy.,Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Appetitive and aversive motivation in dysphoria: A time-domain and time-frequency study of response inhibition. Biol Psychol 2017; 125:12-27. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.02.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/30/2016] [Revised: 01/25/2017] [Accepted: 02/22/2017] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
20
|
Hewig J. Intentionality in frontal asymmetry research. Psychophysiology 2017; 55. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12852] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2016] [Revised: 12/12/2016] [Accepted: 12/27/2016] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Johannes Hewig
- Institute of Psychology at the University of Würzburg; Würzburg Germany
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Agroskin D, Jonas E, Klackl J, Prentice M. Inhibition Underlies the Effect of High Need for Closure on Cultural Closed-Mindedness under Mortality Salience. Front Psychol 2016; 7:1583. [PMID: 27826261 PMCID: PMC5078785 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01583] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/13/2016] [Accepted: 09/29/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The hypothesis that people respond to reminders of mortality with closed-minded, ethnocentric attitudes has received extensive empirical support, largely from research in the Terror Management Theory (TMT) tradition. However, the basic motivational and neural processes that underlie this effect remain largely hypothetical. According to recent neuropsychological theorizing, mortality salience (MS) effects on cultural closed-mindedness may be mediated by activity in the behavioral inhibition system (BIS), which leads to passive avoidance and decreased approach motivation. This should be especially true for people motivated to avoid unfamiliar and potentially threatening stimuli as reflected in a high need for closure (NFC). In two studies involving moderated mediation analyses, people high on trait NFC responded to MS with increased BIS activity (as indicated by EEG and the line bisection task), which is characteristic of inhibited approach motivation. BIS activity, in turn, predicted a reluctance to explore foreign cultures (Study 1) and generalized ethnocentric attitudes (Study 2). In a third study, inhibition was induced directly and caused an increase in ethnocentrism for people high on NFC. Moreover, the effect of the inhibition manipulation × NFC interaction on ethnocentrism was explained by increases in BIS-related affect (i.e., anxious inhibition) at high NFC. To our knowledge, this research is the first to establish an empirical link between very basic, neurally-instantiated inhibitory processes and rather complex, higher-order manifestations of intergroup negativity in response to MS. Our findings contribute to a fuller understanding of the cultural worldview defense phenomenon by illuminating the motivational underpinnings of cultural closed-mindedness in the wake of existential threat.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dmitrij Agroskin
- Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg Salzburg, Austria
| | - Eva Jonas
- Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg Salzburg, Austria
| | - Johannes Klackl
- Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg Salzburg, Austria
| | - Mike Prentice
- Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg Salzburg, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Pidgeon LM, Grealy M, Duffy AHB, Hay L, McTeague C, Vuletic T, Coyle D, Gilbert SJ. Functional neuroimaging of visual creativity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Behav 2016; 6:e00540. [PMID: 27781148 PMCID: PMC5064346 DOI: 10.1002/brb3.540] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2016] [Accepted: 07/03/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION The generation of creative visual imagery contributes to technological and scientific innovation and production of visual art. The underlying cognitive and neural processes are, however, poorly understood. METHODS This review synthesizes functional neuroimaging studies of visual creativity. Seven functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and 19 electroencephalography (EEG) studies were included, comprising 27 experiments and around 800 participants. RESULTS Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of the fMRI studies comparing visual creativity to non-rest control tasks yielded significant clusters in thalamus, left fusiform gyrus, and right middle and inferior frontal gyri. The EEG studies revealed a tendency for decreased alpha power during visual creativity compared to baseline, but comparisons of visual creativity to non-rest control tasks revealed inconsistent findings. CONCLUSIONS The findings are consistent with suggested contributions to visual creativity of prefrontally mediated inhibition, evaluation, and working memory, as well as visual imagery processes. Findings are discussed in relation to prominent theories of the neural basis of creativity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura M. Pidgeon
- School of Psychological Sciences and HealthUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowUK
- Department of Design, Manufacture and Engineering ManagementUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowUK
| | - Madeleine Grealy
- School of Psychological Sciences and HealthUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowUK
| | - Alex H. B. Duffy
- Department of Design, Manufacture and Engineering ManagementUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowUK
| | - Laura Hay
- Department of Design, Manufacture and Engineering ManagementUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowUK
| | - Chris McTeague
- School of Psychological Sciences and HealthUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowUK
- Department of Design, Manufacture and Engineering ManagementUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowUK
| | - Tijana Vuletic
- School of Psychological Sciences and HealthUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowUK
- Department of Design, Manufacture and Engineering ManagementUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowUK
| | - Damien Coyle
- Intelligent Systems Research CentreUniversity of UlsterDerryNorthern Ireland
| | - Sam J. Gilbert
- Institute of Cognitive NeuroscienceUniversity College LondonLondonUK
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Shadli SM, Glue P, McIntosh J, McNaughton N. An improved human anxiety process biomarker: characterization of frequency band, personality and pharmacology. Transl Psychiatry 2015; 5:e699. [PMID: 26670284 PMCID: PMC5068587 DOI: 10.1038/tp.2015.188] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2015] [Revised: 10/21/2015] [Accepted: 10/26/2015] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Anxiety disorders are among the most common mental illness in the western world with a major impact on disability. But their diagnosis has lacked objective biomarkers. We previously demonstrated a human anxiety process biomarker, goal-conflict-specific electroencephalography (EEG) rhythmicity (GCSR) in the stop-signal task (SST). Here we have developed and characterized an improved test appropriate for clinical group testing. We modified the SST to produce balanced numbers of trials in clearly separated stop-signal delay groups. As previously, right frontal (F8) GCSR was extracted as the difference in EEG log Fourier power between matching stop and go trials (that is, stop-signal-specific power) of a quadratic contrast of the three delay values (that is, power when stopping and going are in balanced conflict compared with the average of when stopping or going is greater). Separate experiments assessed drug sensitivity (n=34) and personality relations (n=59). GCSR in this new SST was reduced by three chemically distinct anxiolytic drugs (administered double-blind): buspirone (10 mg), triazolam (0.25 mg) and pregabalin (75 mg); had a frequency range (4-12 Hz) consistent with rodent model data; and positively correlated significantly with neuroticism and nonsignificantly with trait anxiety scores. GCSR, measured in our new form of the SST, should be suitable as a biomarker for one specific anxiety process in the testing of clinical groups and novel drugs and in the development of measures suitable for individual diagnosis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S M Shadli
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | - P Glue
- Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | - J McIntosh
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | - N McNaughton
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand,Department of Psychology, University of Otago, PO Box 56, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand. E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Hecht D. Cerebral lateralization of pro- and anti-social tendencies. Exp Neurobiol 2014; 23:1-27. [PMID: 24737936 PMCID: PMC3984952 DOI: 10.5607/en.2014.23.1.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/02/2013] [Revised: 01/18/2014] [Accepted: 02/17/2014] [Indexed: 12/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Mounting evidence suggest that the right-hemisphere (RH) has a relative advantage, over the left-hemisphere (LH), in mediating social intelligence - identifying social stimuli, understanding the intentions of other people, awareness of the dynamics in social relationships, and successful handling of social interactions. Furthermore, a review and synthesis of the literature suggest that pro-social attitudes and behaviors are associated with physiological activity in the RH, whereas unsocial and anti-social tendencies are mediated primarily by the LH. This hemispheric asymmetry is rooted in several neurobiological and functional differences between the two hemispheres. (I) Positive social interactions often require inhibiting one's immediate desires and considering the perspectives and needs of others. Given that self-control is mediated by the RH, pro-social emotions and behaviors are, therefore, inherently associated with the RH as it subserves the brain's self-restraint mechanisms. (II) The RH mediates experiences of vulnerability. It registers the relative clumsiness and motor weakness of the left limbs, and it is involved, more than the LH, in processing threats and mediating fear. Emotional states of vulnerability trigger the need for affiliation and sociality, therefore the RH has a greater role in mediating pro-social attitudes and behaviors. (III) The RH mediates a holistic mode of representing the world. Holistic perception emphasizes similarities rather than differences, takes a long-term perspective, is associated with divergent thinking and seeing other points-of-view, and it mediates a personal mode of relating to people. All these features of holistic perception facilitate a more empathetic attitude toward others and pro-social behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Hecht
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Keough MT, O'Connor RM. Clarifying the measurement and the role of the behavioral inhibition system in alcohol misuse. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2014; 38:1470-9. [PMID: 24588401 DOI: 10.1111/acer.12387] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2013] [Accepted: 01/12/2014] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND In response to conflicting reward (Behavioral Approach System [BAS]) and/or punishment cues (Fight-Flight-Freeze System [FFFS]) the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) inhibits behavior, leading to increased attention to threat, high anxiety, and behavioral ambivalence. The role of BIS in alcohol misuse is complex, as anxiety promotes self-medication drinking, while attention to threat (e.g., negative outcomes of heavy drinking) may reduce risk. Theory suggests that a concurrent strong BAS may bias BIS-conflict in favor of alcohol approach, while a concurrent strong FFFS may increase the likelihood of alcohol avoidance. However, few studies measure BIS as a conflict system, and no studies incorporate such a measure into examinations of alcohol misuse. Our study goals were to (i) test the Motivational Flanker Task (MFT) as a new laboratory measure of the BIS, BAS, and FFFS; and (ii) use the MFT, in conjunction with self-report measures, to test BAS and FFFS as moderators of the BIS-alcohol misuse relation. We hypothesized that an elevated BIS would predict heavy drinking and alcohol-related problems, but only when BAS was high. Further, we expected an elevated BIS to be associated with reduced alcohol misuse, but only when FFFS was high. METHODS Students (N = 198) completed self-reports of BIS/BAS/FFFS and drinking behavior, and 2 reaction time tasks: MFT and Point Scoring Reaction Time Task (PSRTT). The PSRTT is a published measure of the revised BIS. RESULTS MFT BIS conflict was associated with self-report and PSRTT measures. MFT BAS, but not FFFS, was associated with self-reports. As expected, elevated BIS was associated with heavy drinking, but only when BAS-Drive and BAS-Fun Seeking was also high. FFFS was not supported as a moderator of the BIS-alcohol misuse association. CONCLUSIONS Results support the MFT as a promising measure of the revised BIS. Considering the joint effects of BIS and BAS clarified risk for alcohol misuse.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew T Keough
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, Québec, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
26
|
Gardiner E, Jackson CJ, Loxton NJ. Left Hemispheric Lateral Preference and High Neuroticism Predict Disinhibition in Two Go/No-Go Experiments. J Pers 2014; 83:84-96. [DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12084] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
27
|
A comparison of phenylketonuria with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: do markedly different aetiologies deliver common phenotypes? Brain Res Bull 2013; 99:63-83. [PMID: 24140048 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2013.10.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/17/2013] [Revised: 09/04/2013] [Accepted: 10/07/2013] [Indexed: 01/21/2023]
Abstract
Phenylketonuria (PKU) is a well-defined metabolic disorder arising from a mutation that disrupts phenylalanine metabolism and so produces a variety of neural changes indirectly. Severe cognitive impairment can be prevented by dietary treatment; however, residual symptoms may be reported. These residual symptoms appear to overlap a more prevalent childhood disorder: Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). However, the aetiology of ADHD is a vast contrast to PKU: it seems to arise from a complex combination of genes; and it has a substantial environmental component. We ask whether these two disorders result from two vastly different genotypes that converge on a specific core phenotype that includes similar dysfunctions of Gray's (Gray, 1982) Behavioural Inhibition System (BIS), coupled with other disorder-specific dysfunctions. If so, we believe comparison of the commonalities will allow greater understanding of the neuropsychology of both disorders. We review in detail the aetiology, treatment, neural pathology, cognitive deficits and electrophysiological abnormalities of PKU; and compare this with selected directly matching aspects of ADHD. The biochemical and neural pathologies of PKU and ADHD are quite distinct in their causes and detail; but they result in the disorder in the brain of large amino acid levels, dopamine and white matter that are very similar and could explain the overlap of symptoms within and between the PKU and ADHD spectra. The common deficits affect visual function, motor function, attention, working memory, planning, and inhibition. For each of PKU and ADHD separately, a subset of deficits has been attributed to a primary dysfunction of behavioural inhibition. In the case of ADHD (excluding the inattentive subtype) this has been proposed to involve a specific failure of the BIS; and we suggest that this is also true of PKU. This accounts for a substantial proportion of the parallels in the superficial symptoms of both disorders and we see this as linked to prefrontal, rather than more general, dysfunction of the BIS.
Collapse
|
28
|
McNaughton N, Swart C, Neo P, Bates V, Glue P. Anti-anxiety drugs reduce conflict-specific "theta"--a possible human anxiety-specific biomarker. J Affect Disord 2013; 148:104-11. [PMID: 23261140 DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.057] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/04/2012] [Revised: 11/22/2012] [Accepted: 11/24/2012] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Syndromes of fear/anxiety are currently ill-defined, with no accepted human biomarkers for anxiety-specific processes. A unique common neural action of different classes of anxiolytic drugs may provide such a biomarker. In rodents, a reduction in low frequency (4-12 Hz; "theta") brain rhythmicity is produced by all anxiolytics (even those lacking panicolytic or antidepressant action) and not by any non-anxiolytics. This rhythmicity is a key property of the Behavioural Inhibition System (BIS) postulated to be one neural substrate of anxiety. We sought homologous anxiolytic-sensitive changes in human surface EEG rhythmicity. METHOD Thirty-four healthy volunteers in parallel groups were administered double blind single doses of triazolam 0.25mg, buspirone 10mg or placebo 1 hour prior to completing the stop-signal task. Right frontal conflict-specific EEG power (previously shown to correlate with trait anxiety and neuroticism in this task) was extracted as a contrast between trials with balanced approach-avoidance (stop-go) conflict and the average of trials with net approach and net avoidance. RESULTS Compared with placebo, both triazolam and buspirone decreased right-frontal, 9-10 Hz, conflict-specific-power. LIMITATIONS Only one dose of each of only two classes of anxiolytic and no non-anxiolytics were tested, so additional tests are needed to determine generality. CONCLUSIONS There is a distinct rhythmic system in humans that is sensitive to both classical/GABAergic and novel/serotonergic anxiolytics. This conflict-specific rhythmicity should provide a biomarker, with a strong pre-clinical neuropsychology, for a novel approach to classifying anxiety disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Neil McNaughton
- Department of Psychology and Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Otago, POB56, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Ehlis AC, Pauli P, Herrmann MJ, Plichta MM, Zielasek J, Pfuhlmann B, Stöber G, Ringel T, Jabs B, Fallgatter AJ. Hypofrontality in schizophrenic patients and its relevance for the choice of antipsychotic medication: an event-related potential study. World J Biol Psychiatry 2012; 13:188-99. [PMID: 21517702 DOI: 10.3109/15622975.2011.566354] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES One of the neurobiological core features of schizophrenic illnesses is a hypo-functionality of the frontal cortex ("cerebral hypofrontality"). The two major classes of antipsychotic medication differ regarding their impact on frontal lobe function and metabolism, with a presumably more positive effect of "atypical" compared to "typical" agents. To date, neurobiological markers reliably predicting the treatment response to different antipsychotics are lacking. The present study, therefore, aimed at establishing a neurophysiological marker of frontal lobe function (NoGo-Anteriorization, NGA) as a predictor of the treatment response to first- and second-generation antipsychotics. METHODS Seventy-six schizophrenic patients were examined three times over a 6-week study period. Patients were treated with first- or second-generation antipsychotics, and NGA, neurocognitive performance, and symptomatology were assessed on admission as well as during two follow-up measurements. RESULTS Baseline NGA values significantly predicted the treatment response to typical and atypical antipsychotics; however, the direction of this prediction was dependent on the antipsychotic drug regimen. Moreover, atypical antipsychotics had a superior impact on neurocognitive performance and self-reported quality of life. CONCLUSIONS The NGA might be a useful tool in developing individualized treatment strategies based on pathophysiological aspects of schizophrenic illnesses that can be easily determined in clinical routine settings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ann-Christine Ehlis
- Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatics, and Psychotherapy, University of Wuerzburg , Germany.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Rutherford HJV, Lindell AK. Thriving and Surviving: Approach and Avoidance Motivation and Lateralization. EMOTION REVIEW 2011. [DOI: 10.1177/1754073911402392] [Citation(s) in RCA: 90] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Two core motivational systems have been conceptualized as underlying emotion and behavior. The approach system drives the organism toward stimuli or events in the environment, and the avoidance system instead deters the organism away from these stimuli or events. This approach—avoidance dichotomy has been central to theories of emotion. Advances in neuroscience complementing well-designed behavioral experiments have begun to elucidate the neural mechanisms underlying approach—avoidance motivation, suggesting that these two systems exist in parallel and are lateralized in the brain. This review explores the notion of approach—avoidance and the cerebral lateralization of these motivational tendencies.
Collapse
|
31
|
Stopping, goal-conflict, trait anxiety and frontal rhythmic power in the stop-signal task. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2011; 11:485-93. [DOI: 10.3758/s13415-011-0046-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
32
|
Frontal theta power linked to neuroticism and avoidance. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2011; 11:396-403. [DOI: 10.3758/s13415-011-0038-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/31/2023]
|
33
|
Abstract
AbstractThe set of 30 stimulating commentaries on our target article helps to define the areas of our initial position that should be reiterated or else made clearer and, more importantly, the ways in which moderators of and extensions to the SIMS can be imagined. In our response, we divide the areas of discussion into (1) a clarification of our meaning of “functional,” (2) a consideration of our proposed categories of smiles, (3) a reminder about the role of top-down processes in the interpretation of smile meaning in SIMS, (4) an evaluation of the role of eye contact in the interpretation of facial expression of emotion, and (5) an assessment of the possible moderators of the core SIMS model. We end with an appreciation of the proposed extensions to the model, and note that the future of research on the problem of the smile appears to us to be assured.
Collapse
|
34
|
Trait anxiety and dynamic adjustments in conflict processing. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2010; 10:372-81. [DOI: 10.3758/cabn.10.3.372] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|