1
|
Litvak ND, Tal A, Mudrik L. Semantic priming modulates the strength and direction of the Kanizsa illusion. COMMUNICATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 2025; 3:86. [PMID: 40425840 PMCID: PMC12117148 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-025-00268-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/29/2024] [Accepted: 05/15/2025] [Indexed: 05/29/2025]
Abstract
Visual illusions are considered key examples for cognitive impenetrability, as they are held not to be affected by non-perceptual processes. We revisit this claim in five experiments (N = 1148; four preregistered) focused on the Kanizsa illusion, where a nonexistent shape is experienced within illusory contours. Pac-Man-like shapes inducing the illusion were presented after primes that were either semantically related to the Pac-Man game or not. We hypothesized that semantic primes would promote interpreting the shapes as individual Pac-Man characters, thus biasing participants away from the holistic Kanizsa illusion. Indeed, we found that the Kanizsa shape was detected less when participants were primed with Pac-Man-related stimuli. We then also demonstrated the opposite effect: a prime indexing the illusory shape ("Triangle") enhanced the probability of seeing the illusion. Together, our results suggest that semantic priming can both reduce and increase the probability of experiencing the Kanizsa illusion, thus supporting claims of cognitive penetrability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Amir Tal
- School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
- Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
| | - Liad Mudrik
- School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel.
- Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel.
- Brain, Mind, and Consciousness Program, Canadian Institute for Advanced Research (CIFAR), Toronto, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Warrington O, Graedel NN, Callaghan MF, Kok P. Communication of perceptual predictions from the hippocampus to the deep layers of the parahippocampal cortex. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2025; 11:eads4970. [PMID: 40397746 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.ads4970] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2024] [Accepted: 04/17/2025] [Indexed: 05/23/2025]
Abstract
Current evidence suggests that the hippocampus is essential for exploiting predictive relationships during perception. However, it remains unclear whether the hippocampus drives the communication of predictions to sensory cortex or receives prediction signals from elsewhere. We collected 7-tesla fMRI data in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) while auditory cues predicted abstract shapes. Strikingly, neural patterns evoked by predicted shapes in CA2/3, pre/parasubiculum, and the parahippocampal cortex (PHC) were negatively correlated to patterns evoked by the same shapes when actually presented. Using layer-specific analyses, we ask: In which direction are predictions communicated between the hippocampus and neocortex? Superficial layers of the MTL cortex project to the hippocampus, while the deep layers receive feedback projections. Informational connectivity analyses revealed that communication between CA2/3 and PHC was specific to the deep layers of PHC. These findings suggest that the hippocampus generates predictions through pattern completion in CA2/3 and feeds these predictions back to the neocortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Oliver Warrington
- Department of Imaging Neuroscience, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK
- Department of Brain Sciences, Imperial College London, London, UK
| | - Nadine N Graedel
- Department of Imaging Neuroscience, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Martina F Callaghan
- Department of Imaging Neuroscience, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Peter Kok
- Department of Imaging Neuroscience, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Carricarte T, Xie S, Singer J, Trampel R, Huber L, Weiskopf N, Cichy RM. Layer-specific spatiotemporal dynamics of feedforward and feedback in human visual object perception. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2025:2025.05.13.653501. [PMID: 40462954 PMCID: PMC12132538 DOI: 10.1101/2025.05.13.653501] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/11/2025]
Abstract
Visual object perception is mediated by information flow between regions of the ventral visual stream along feedforward and feedback anatomical connections. However, feedforward and feedback signals during naturalistic vision are rapid and overlapping, complicating their identification and precise functional specification. Here we recorded human layer-specific fMRI responses to naturalistic object images in early visual cortex (EVC) and lateral occipital complex (LOC) to isolate feedforward and feedback information signals spatially by their cortical layer specific termination pattern. We combined these layer-specific fMRI responses with electroencephalography (EEG) responses for the same images to segregate feedforward and feedback signals in both time and space. Feedforward signals emerge early in the middle layers of EVC and LOC, followed by feedback signals in the superficial layer of both regions, and the deep layer of EVC. Comparing the identified dynamics in LOC to a visual deep neural network (DNN), revealed that early feedforward signals in LOC encode medium complexity features, whereas later feedback signals increase the representational format to high complexity features. Together this specifies the spatiotemporal dynamics and functional role of feedforward and feedback information flow mediating visual object perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tony Carricarte
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, 14195 Berlin, Germany
- Einstein Center for Neurosciences Berlin, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany
- Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience Berlin, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany
| | - Siying Xie
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, 14195 Berlin, Germany
| | - Johannes Singer
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, 14195 Berlin, Germany
| | - Robert Trampel
- Department of Neurophysics, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | | | - Nikolaus Weiskopf
- Department of Neurophysics, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
- Felix Bloch Institute for Solid State Physics, Faculty of Physics and Earth Sciences, Universität Leipzig, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, WC1N 3AR London, United Kingdom
| | - Radoslaw M. Cichy
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, 14195 Berlin, Germany
- Einstein Center for Neurosciences Berlin, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany
- Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience Berlin, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany
- Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Hashim IC, Senden M, Goebel R. PrediRep: Modeling hierarchical predictive coding with an unsupervised deep learning network. Neural Netw 2025; 185:107246. [PMID: 39946763 DOI: 10.1016/j.neunet.2025.107246] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/02/2024] [Revised: 10/02/2024] [Accepted: 01/31/2025] [Indexed: 03/09/2025]
Abstract
Hierarchical predictive coding (hPC) provides a compelling framework for understanding how the cortex predicts future sensory inputs by minimizing prediction errors through an internal generative model of the external world. Existing deep learning models inspired by hPC incorporate architectural choices that deviate from core hPC principles, potentially limiting their utility for neuroscientific investigations. We introduce PrediRep (Predicting Representations), a novel deep learning network that adheres more closely to architectural principles of hPC. We validate PrediRep by comparing its functional alignment with hPC to that of existing models after being trained on a next-frame prediction task. Our findings demonstrate that PrediRep, particularly when trained with an all-level loss function (PrediRepAll), exhibits high functional alignment with hPC. In contrast to other contemporary deep learning networks inspired by hPC, it consistently processes input-relevant information at higher hierarchical levels and maintains active representations and accurate predictions across all hierarchical levels. Although PrediRep was designed primarily to serve as a model suitable for neuroscientific research rather than to optimize performance, it nevertheless achieves competitive performance in next-frame prediction while utilizing significantly fewer trainable parameters than alternative models. Our results underscore that even minor architectural deviations from neuroscientific theories like hPC can lead to significant functional discrepancies. By faithfully adhering to hPC principles, PrediRep provides a more accurate tool for in silico exploration of cortical phenomena. PrediRep's lightweight and biologically plausible design makes it well-suited for future studies aiming to investigate the neural underpinnings of predictive coding and to derive empirically testable predictions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ibrahim C Hashim
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands; Maastricht Brain Imaging Centre, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands.
| | - Mario Senden
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands; Maastricht Brain Imaging Centre, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands.
| | - Rainer Goebel
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands; Maastricht Brain Imaging Centre, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Noorman S, Stein T, Fahrenfort JJ, van Gaal S. Perceptual and attentional impairments of conscious access involve distinct neural mechanisms despite equal task performance. eLife 2025; 13:RP97900. [PMID: 40310881 PMCID: PMC12045619 DOI: 10.7554/elife.97900] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/03/2025] Open
Abstract
This study investigates failures in conscious access resulting from either weak sensory input (perceptual impairments) or unattended input (attentional impairments). Participants viewed a Kanizsa stimulus with or without an illusory triangle within a rapid serial visual presentation of distractor stimuli. We designed a novel Kanizsa stimulus that contained additional ancillary features of different complexity (local contrast and collinearity) that were independently manipulated. Perceptual performance on the Kanizsa stimulus (presence vs. absence of an illusion) was equated between the perceptual (masking) and attentional (attentional blink) manipulation to circumvent common confounds related to conditional differences in task performance. We trained and tested classifiers on electroencephalogram (EEG) data to reflect the processing of specific stimulus features, with increasing levels of complexity. We show that late stages of processing (~200-250 ms), reflecting the integration of complex stimulus features (collinearity, illusory triangle), were impaired by masking but spared by the attentional blink. In contrast, decoding of local contrast (the spatial arrangement of stimulus features) was observed early in time (~80 ms) and was left largely unaffected by either manipulation. These results replicate previous work showing that feedforward processing is largely preserved under both perceptual and attentional impairments. Crucially, however, under matched levels of performance, only attentional impairments left the processing of more complex visual features relatively intact, likely related to spared lateral and local feedback processes during inattention. These findings reveal distinct neural mechanisms associated with perceptual and attentional impairments and thus contribute to a comprehensive understanding of distinct neural stages leading to conscious access.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel Noorman
- Department of Psychology, University of AmsterdamAmsterdamNetherlands
- Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of AmsterdamAmsterdamNetherlands
| | - Timo Stein
- Department of Psychology, University of AmsterdamAmsterdamNetherlands
- Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of AmsterdamAmsterdamNetherlands
| | - Johannes Jacobus Fahrenfort
- Department of Psychology, University of AmsterdamAmsterdamNetherlands
- Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of AmsterdamAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of Applied and Experimental Psychology, Vrije Universiteit AmsterdamAmsterdamNetherlands
| | - Simon van Gaal
- Department of Psychology, University of AmsterdamAmsterdamNetherlands
- Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of AmsterdamAmsterdamNetherlands
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Pizzuti A, Gulban OF, Huber LR, Peters JC, Goebel R. In the brain of the beholder: bi-stable motion reveals mesoscopic-scale feedback modulation in V1. Brain Struct Funct 2025; 230:47. [PMID: 40186769 PMCID: PMC11972204 DOI: 10.1007/s00429-025-02906-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/18/2024] [Accepted: 03/06/2025] [Indexed: 04/07/2025]
Abstract
Understanding the neural processes underlying conscious perception remains a central goal in neuroscience. Visual illusions, whether static or dynamic, provide an effective ecological paradigm for studying conscious perception, as they induce subjective experiences from constant visual inputs. While previous neuroimaging studies have dissociated perceptual interpretation of visual motion from sensory input within the motion-sensitive area (hMT+) in humans, less is known about the role of the primary visual area (V1) and its relationship to hMT+ during a bistable perception. To address this, we conducted a layer-fMRI study at 7 T with human participants exposed to a bistable motion quartet stimulus. Despite a constant sensory input, the bistable motion quartet elicits switching horizontal and vertical apparent motion percepts likely due to lateral and feedback connections across low and high-level brain regions (feedback processing). As control, we used an "unambiguous" version of the motion quartet, hereafter referred to as "physical" motion stimulus, where horizontal and vertical motion is physically presented as visual stimulus in an alternated fashion (feedforward processing). With the advantage of a sub-millimeter resolution gained at ultra-high magnetic field (7 Tesla), we aimed to unveil the differential laminar modulation of V1 (early visual area) and hMT+ (high-order visual area) during the physical and bistable condition. Our results indicate that: (1) hMT+ functional activity correlates with conscious perception during both physical and ambiguous stimuli with similar strength. There is no evidence of differential laminar profiles in hMT+ between the two experimental conditions. (2) Between inducer squares, V1 shows a significantly reduced functional response to the ambiguous stimulus compared to the physical stimulus, as it primarily reflects feedback signals with diminished feedforward input. Distinct V1 laminar profiles differentiate the two experimental conditions. (3) The temporal dynamics of V1 and hMT+ become more similar during the ambiguous condition. (4) V1 exhibits reduced specificity to horizontal and vertical motion perception during the ambiguous condition at the retinotopic locations corresponding to the perceived motion. Our findings demonstrate that during the ambiguous condition, there is a stronger temporal coupling between hMT+ and V1 due to feedback signals from hMT+ to V1. Such feedback to V1 might be contributing to the stabilization of the vivid perception of directed motion at the face of constant ambiguous stimulation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alessandra Pizzuti
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands.
- Brain Innovation B.V., Maastricht, The Netherlands.
| | - Omer Faruk Gulban
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
- Brain Innovation B.V., Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | | | - Judith Carolien Peters
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Rainer Goebel
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands.
- Brain Innovation B.V., Maastricht, The Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Moreni G, Dorcioman RA, Pennartz CMA, Mejias JF. Cell-type-specific firing patterns in a V1 cortical column model depend on feedforward and feedback-driven states. PLoS Comput Biol 2025; 21:e1012036. [PMID: 40267074 PMCID: PMC12017539 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1012036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2024] [Accepted: 03/27/2025] [Indexed: 04/25/2025] Open
Abstract
Stimulation of specific cell groups under different network regimes (e.g., spontaneous activity or sensory-evoked activity) can provide insights into the neural dynamics of cortical columns. While these protocols are challenging to perform experimentally, modelling can serve as a powerful tool for such explorations. Using detailed electrophysiological and anatomical data from mouse V1, we built a novel spiking network model of a cortical column, which incorporates pyramidal cells and three distinct interneuron types (PV, SST, and VIP cells, specified per lamina), as well as the dynamic and voltage-dependent properties of AMPA, GABA, and NMDA receptors. We first demonstrate that thalamocortical feedforward (FF) and feedback (FB) stimuli arriving in the column have opposite effects, leading to net columnar excitation and inhibition respectively and revealing translaminar gain control via full-column inhibition by layer 6. We then perturb one cell group (i.e., a cell type in a specific layer) at a time and observe the effects on other cell groups under distinct network states: spontaneous, feedforward-driven, feedback-driven, and a combination of feedforward and feedback. Our findings reveal that when a given group is perturbed, the columnar response varies significantly based on its state, with strong sensory feedforward input decreasing columnar sensitivity to all perturbations and feedback input serving as modulator of intra columnar interactions. Given that activity changes within specific neuronal populations are difficult to predict a priori in experiments, our model may constitute a useful tool to predict outcomes of perturbations and assist in experimental design.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Giulia Moreni
- Cognitive and Systems Neuroscience Group, Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, Faculty of Science, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
- Research Priority Area Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Rares A. Dorcioman
- Cognitive and Systems Neuroscience Group, Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, Faculty of Science, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
- Research Priority Area Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Cyriel M. A. Pennartz
- Cognitive and Systems Neuroscience Group, Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, Faculty of Science, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
- Research Priority Area Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Jorge F. Mejias
- Cognitive and Systems Neuroscience Group, Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, Faculty of Science, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
- Research Priority Area Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Ahmed AA, Alegret N, Almeida B, Alvarez-Puebla R, Andrews AM, Ballerini L, Barrios-Capuchino JJ, Becker C, Blick RH, Bonakdar S, Chakraborty I, Chen X, Cheon J, Chilla G, Coelho Conceicao AL, Delehanty J, Dulle M, Efros AL, Epple M, Fedyk M, Feliu N, Feng M, Fernández-Chacón R, Fernandez-Cuesta I, Fertig N, Förster S, Garrido JA, George M, Guse AH, Hampp N, Harberts J, Han J, Heekeren HR, Hofmann UG, Holzapfel M, Hosseinkazemi H, Huang Y, Huber P, Hyeon T, Ingebrandt S, Ienca M, Iske A, Kang Y, Kasieczka G, Kim DH, Kostarelos K, Lee JH, Lin KW, Liu S, Liu X, Liu Y, Lohr C, Mailänder V, Maffongelli L, Megahed S, Mews A, Mutas M, Nack L, Nakatsuka N, Oertner TG, Offenhäusser A, Oheim M, Otange B, Otto F, Patrono E, Peng B, Picchiotti A, Pierini F, Pötter-Nerger M, Pozzi M, Pralle A, Prato M, Qi B, Ramos-Cabrer P, Genger UR, Ritter N, Rittner M, Roy S, Santoro F, Schuck NW, Schulz F, Şeker E, Skiba M, Sosniok M, Stephan H, Wang R, Wang T, Wegner KD, Weiss PS, Xu M, Yang C, Zargarian SS, Zeng Y, Zhou Y, Zhu D, Zierold R, Parak WJ. Interfacing with the Brain: How Nanotechnology Can Contribute. ACS NANO 2025; 19:10630-10717. [PMID: 40063703 PMCID: PMC11948619 DOI: 10.1021/acsnano.4c10525] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2024] [Revised: 12/19/2024] [Accepted: 12/24/2024] [Indexed: 03/26/2025]
Abstract
Interfacing artificial devices with the human brain is the central goal of neurotechnology. Yet, our imaginations are often limited by currently available paradigms and technologies. Suggestions for brain-machine interfaces have changed over time, along with the available technology. Mechanical levers and cable winches were used to move parts of the brain during the mechanical age. Sophisticated electronic wiring and remote control have arisen during the electronic age, ultimately leading to plug-and-play computer interfaces. Nonetheless, our brains are so complex that these visions, until recently, largely remained unreachable dreams. The general problem, thus far, is that most of our technology is mechanically and/or electrically engineered, whereas the brain is a living, dynamic entity. As a result, these worlds are difficult to interface with one another. Nanotechnology, which encompasses engineered solid-state objects and integrated circuits, excels at small length scales of single to a few hundred nanometers and, thus, matches the sizes of biomolecules, biomolecular assemblies, and parts of cells. Consequently, we envision nanomaterials and nanotools as opportunities to interface with the brain in alternative ways. Here, we review the existing literature on the use of nanotechnology in brain-machine interfaces and look forward in discussing perspectives and limitations based on the authors' expertise across a range of complementary disciplines─from neuroscience, engineering, physics, and chemistry to biology and medicine, computer science and mathematics, and social science and jurisprudence. We focus on nanotechnology but also include information from related fields when useful and complementary.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Abdullah
A. A. Ahmed
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
- Department
of Physics, Faculty of Applied Science, Thamar University, Dhamar 87246, Yemen
| | - Nuria Alegret
- Biogipuzkoa
HRI, Paseo Dr. Begiristain
s/n, 20014 Donostia-San
Sebastián, Spain
- Basque
Foundation for Science, Ikerbasque, 48013 Bilbao, Spain
| | - Bethany Almeida
- Department
of Chemical and Biomolecular Engineering, Clarkson University, Potsdam, New York 13699, United States
| | - Ramón Alvarez-Puebla
- Universitat
Rovira i Virgili, 43007 Tarragona, Spain
- ICREA, 08010 Barcelona, Spain
| | - Anne M. Andrews
- Department
of Chemistry and Biochemistry, University
of California, Los Angeles, Los
Angeles, California 90095, United States
- Neuroscience
Interdepartmental Program, University of
California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California 90095, United States
- Department
of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Semel Institute for Neuroscience
& Human Behavior, and Hatos Center for Neuropharmacology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California 90095, United States
- California
Nanosystems Institute, University of California,
Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California 90095, United States
| | - Laura Ballerini
- Neuroscience
Area, International School for Advanced
Studies (SISSA/ISAS), Trieste 34136, Italy
| | | | - Charline Becker
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Robert H. Blick
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Shahin Bonakdar
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
- National
Cell Bank Department, Pasteur Institute
of Iran, P.O. Box 1316943551, Tehran, Iran
| | - Indranath Chakraborty
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
- School
of Nano Science and Technology, Indian Institute
of Technology Kharagpur, Kharagpur 721302, India
| | - Xiaodong Chen
- Innovative
Center for Flexible Devices (iFLEX), Max Planck − NTU Joint
Lab for Artificial Senses, School of Materials Science and Engineering, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore 639798, Singapore
| | - Jinwoo Cheon
- Institute
for Basic Science Center for Nanomedicine, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 03722, Korea
- Advanced
Science Institute, Yonsei University, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 03722, Korea
- Department
of Chemistry, Yonsei University, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 03722, Korea
| | - Gerwin Chilla
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | | | - James Delehanty
- U.S. Naval
Research Laboratory, Washington, D.C. 20375, United States
| | - Martin Dulle
- JCNS-1, Forschungszentrum
Jülich, 52428 Jülich, Germany
| | | | - Matthias Epple
- Inorganic
Chemistry and Center for Nanointegration Duisburg-Essen (CeNIDE), University of Duisburg-Essen, 45117 Essen, Germany
| | - Mark Fedyk
- Center
for Neuroengineering and Medicine, UC Davis, Sacramento, California 95817, United States
| | - Neus Feliu
- Zentrum
für Angewandte Nanotechnologie CAN, Fraunhofer-Institut für Angewandte Polymerforschung IAP, 20146 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Miao Feng
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Rafael Fernández-Chacón
- Instituto
de Biomedicina de Sevilla (IBiS), Hospital
Universitario Virgen del Rocío/Consejo Superior de Investigaciones
Científicas/Universidad de Sevilla, 41013 Seville, Spain
- Departamento
de Fisiología Médica y Biofísica, Facultad de
Medicina, Universidad de Sevilla, CIBERNED,
ISCIII, 41013 Seville, Spain
| | | | - Niels Fertig
- Nanion
Technologies GmbH, 80339 München, Germany
| | | | - Jose A. Garrido
- ICREA, 08010 Barcelona, Spain
- Catalan
Institute of Nanoscience and Nanotechnology (ICN2), CSIC and BIST, 08193 Bellaterra, Spain
| | | | - Andreas H. Guse
- The Calcium
Signaling Group, Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Cell Biology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Norbert Hampp
- Fachbereich
Chemie, Universität Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany
| | - Jann Harberts
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
- Drug Delivery,
Disposition and Dynamics, Monash Institute of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Monash University, Parkville, Victoria 3052, Australia
- Melbourne
Centre for Nanofabrication, Victorian Node
of the Australian National Fabrication Facility, Clayton, Victoria 3168, Australia
| | - Jili Han
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Hauke R. Heekeren
- Executive
University Board, Universität Hamburg, 20148 Hamburg Germany
| | - Ulrich G. Hofmann
- Section
for Neuroelectronic Systems, Department for Neurosurgery, University Medical Center Freiburg, 79108 Freiburg, Germany
- Faculty
of Medicine, University of Freiburg, 79110 Freiburg, Germany
| | - Malte Holzapfel
- Zentrum
für Angewandte Nanotechnologie CAN, Fraunhofer-Institut für Angewandte Polymerforschung IAP, 20146 Hamburg, Germany
| | | | - Yalan Huang
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Patrick Huber
- Institute
for Materials and X-ray Physics, Hamburg
University of Technology, 21073 Hamburg, Germany
- Center
for X-ray and Nano Science CXNS, Deutsches
Elektronen-Synchrotron DESY, 22607 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Taeghwan Hyeon
- Center
for Nanoparticle Research, Institute for Basic Science (IBS), Seoul 08826, Republic of Korea
- School
of Chemical and Biological Engineering, and Institute of Chemical
Processes, Seoul National University, Seoul 08826, Republic of Korea
| | - Sven Ingebrandt
- Institute
of Materials in Electrical Engineering 1, RWTH Aachen University, 52074 Aachen, Germany
| | - Marcello Ienca
- Institute
for Ethics and History of Medicine, School of Medicine and Health, Technische Universität München (TUM), 81675 München, Germany
| | - Armin Iske
- Fachbereich
Mathematik, Universität Hamburg, 20146 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Yanan Kang
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | | | - Dae-Hyeong Kim
- Center
for Nanoparticle Research, Institute for Basic Science (IBS), Seoul 08826, Republic of Korea
- School
of Chemical and Biological Engineering, and Institute of Chemical
Processes, Seoul National University, Seoul 08826, Republic of Korea
| | - Kostas Kostarelos
- Catalan
Institute of Nanoscience and Nanotechnology (ICN2), CSIC and BIST, 08193 Bellaterra, Spain
- Centre
for Nanotechnology in Medicine, Faculty of Biology, Medicine &
Health and The National Graphene Institute, University of Manchester, Manchester M13 9PL, United
Kingdom
| | - Jae-Hyun Lee
- Institute
for Basic Science Center for Nanomedicine, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 03722, Korea
- Advanced
Science Institute, Yonsei University, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 03722, Korea
| | - Kai-Wei Lin
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Sijin Liu
- State Key
Laboratory of Environmental Chemistry and Ecotoxicology, Research Center for Eco-Environmental Sciences, Chinese
Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100085, China
- University
of the Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
| | - Xin Liu
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Yang Liu
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Christian Lohr
- Fachbereich
Biologie, Universität Hamburg, 20146 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Volker Mailänder
- Department
of Dermatology, Center for Translational Nanomedicine, Universitätsmedizin der Johannes-Gutenberg,
Universität Mainz, 55131 Mainz, Germany
- Max Planck
Institute for Polymer Research, Ackermannweg 10, 55129 Mainz, Germany
| | - Laura Maffongelli
- Institute
of Medical Psychology, University of Lübeck, 23562 Lübeck, Germany
| | - Saad Megahed
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
- Physics
Department, Faculty of Science, Al-Azhar
University, 4434104 Cairo, Egypt
| | - Alf Mews
- Fachbereich
Chemie, Universität Hamburg, 20146 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Marina Mutas
- Zentrum
für Angewandte Nanotechnologie CAN, Fraunhofer-Institut für Angewandte Polymerforschung IAP, 20146 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Leroy Nack
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Nako Nakatsuka
- Laboratory
of Chemical Nanotechnology (CHEMINA), Neuro-X
Institute, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne
(EPFL), Geneva CH-1202, Switzerland
| | - Thomas G. Oertner
- Institute
for Synaptic Neuroscience, University Medical
Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Andreas Offenhäusser
- Institute
of Biological Information Processing - Bioelectronics, Forschungszentrum Jülich, 52425 Jülich, Germany
| | - Martin Oheim
- Université
Paris Cité, CNRS, Saints Pères
Paris Institute for the Neurosciences, 75006 Paris, France
| | - Ben Otange
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Ferdinand Otto
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Enrico Patrono
- Institute
of Physiology, Czech Academy of Sciences, Prague 12000, Czech Republic
| | - Bo Peng
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | | | - Filippo Pierini
- Department
of Biosystems and Soft Matter, Institute
of Fundamental Technological Research, Polish Academy of Sciences, 02-106 Warsaw, Poland
| | - Monika Pötter-Nerger
- Head and
Neurocenter, Department of Neurology, University
Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, 20246 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Maria Pozzi
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Arnd Pralle
- University
at Buffalo, Department of Physics, Buffalo, New York 14260, United States
| | - Maurizio Prato
- CIC biomaGUNE, Basque Research and Technology
Alliance (BRTA), 20014 Donostia-San
Sebastián, Spain
- Department
of Chemical and Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of Trieste, 34127 Trieste, Italy
- Basque
Foundation for Science, Ikerbasque, 48013 Bilbao, Spain
| | - Bing Qi
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
- School
of Life Sciences, Southern University of
Science and Technology, Shenzhen, 518055, China
| | - Pedro Ramos-Cabrer
- CIC biomaGUNE, Basque Research and Technology
Alliance (BRTA), 20014 Donostia-San
Sebastián, Spain
- Basque
Foundation for Science, Ikerbasque, 48013 Bilbao, Spain
| | - Ute Resch Genger
- Division
Biophotonics, Federal Institute for Materials Research and Testing
(BAM), 12489 Berlin, Germany
| | - Norbert Ritter
- Executive
Faculty Board, Faculty for Mathematics, Informatics and Natural Sciences, Universität Hamburg, 20345 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Marten Rittner
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Sathi Roy
- Zentrum
für Angewandte Nanotechnologie CAN, Fraunhofer-Institut für Angewandte Polymerforschung IAP, 20146 Hamburg, Germany
- Department
of Mechanical Engineering, Indian Institute
of Technology Kharagpur, Kharagpur 721302, India
| | - Francesca Santoro
- Institute
of Biological Information Processing - Bioelectronics, Forschungszentrum Jülich, 52425 Jülich, Germany
- Faculty
of Electrical Engineering and Information Technology, RWTH Aachen, 52074 Aachen, Germany
| | - Nicolas W. Schuck
- Institute
of Psychology, Universität Hamburg, 20146 Hamburg, Germany
- Max Planck
Research Group NeuroCode, Max Planck Institute
for Human Development, 14195 Berlin, Germany
- Max Planck
UCL Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research, 14195 Berlin, Germany
| | - Florian Schulz
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Erkin Şeker
- University
of California, Davis, Davis, California 95616, United States
| | - Marvin Skiba
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Martin Sosniok
- Zentrum
für Angewandte Nanotechnologie CAN, Fraunhofer-Institut für Angewandte Polymerforschung IAP, 20146 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Holger Stephan
- Helmholtz-Zentrum
Dresden-Rossendorf, Institute of Radiopharmaceutical
Cancer Research, 01328 Dresden, Germany
| | - Ruixia Wang
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
- Deutsches
Elektronen-Synchrotron DESY, 22607 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Ting Wang
- State Key
Laboratory of Organic Electronics and Information Displays & Jiangsu
Key Laboratory for Biosensors, Institute of Advanced Materials (IAM),
Jiangsu National Synergetic Innovation Center for Advanced Materials
(SICAM), Nanjing University of Posts and
Telecommunications, Nanjing 210023, China
| | - K. David Wegner
- Division
Biophotonics, Federal Institute for Materials Research and Testing
(BAM), 12489 Berlin, Germany
| | - Paul S. Weiss
- Department
of Chemistry and Biochemistry, University
of California, Los Angeles, Los
Angeles, California 90095, United States
- California
Nanosystems Institute, University of California,
Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California 90095, United States
- Department
of Bioengineering, University of California,
Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California 90095, United States
- Department
of Materials Science and Engineering, University
of California, Los Angeles, Los
Angeles, California 90095, United States
| | - Ming Xu
- State Key
Laboratory of Environmental Chemistry and Ecotoxicology, Research Center for Eco-Environmental Sciences, Chinese
Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100085, China
- University
of the Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
| | - Chenxi Yang
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Seyed Shahrooz Zargarian
- Department
of Biosystems and Soft Matter, Institute
of Fundamental Technological Research, Polish Academy of Sciences, 02-106 Warsaw, Poland
| | - Yuan Zeng
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Yaofeng Zhou
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | - Dingcheng Zhu
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
- College
of Material, Chemistry and Chemical Engineering, Key Laboratory of
Organosilicon Chemistry and Material Technology, Ministry of Education,
Key Laboratory of Organosilicon Material Technology, Hangzhou Normal University, Hangzhou 311121, China
| | - Robert Zierold
- Fachbereich
Physik, Universität Hamburg, 22761 Hamburg, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Yang Z, Arabinda M, Wang F, Chen LM, Gore JC. Layer-specific BOLD effects in gradient and spin-echo acquisitions in somatosensory cortex. Magn Reson Med 2025; 93:1314-1328. [PMID: 39370926 PMCID: PMC11680728 DOI: 10.1002/mrm.30326] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/17/2024] [Revised: 09/10/2024] [Accepted: 09/14/2024] [Indexed: 10/08/2024]
Abstract
PURPOSE Previous studies have shown varied BOLD signals with gradient echo (GE) across cortical depth. To interpret these variations, and understand the effects of vascular geometry and size, the magnitudes and layer distributions of GE and spin-echo (SE) BOLD functional MRI signals were compared in the somatosensory cortex of squirrel monkeys during tactile stimulation and in a resting state at high spatial resolution and high field. METHODS A block-design stimulation was used to identify tactile-evoked activation signals in somatosensory Areas 3b and 1. Layer-specific connectivities were calculated using resting-state data. Signal power spectra were compared by depth and pulse sequence. The measured ratios of transverse relaxation rate changes were compared with Anderson and Weiss's model. RESULTS SE signals showed a 26% lower percentage signal change during tactile stimulation compared with GE, along with a slower time course. SE signals remained consistent but weaker in lower layers, whereas GE signals decreased with cortical depth. This pattern extended to resting-state power spectra. Resting-state functional connectivity indicated larger connectivity between the top layers of Area 3b and Area 1 for GE, with minimal changes for SE. Comparisons with theory suggest vessel diameters ranging from 19.4 to 9 microns are responsible for BOLD effects across cortical layers at 9.4 T. CONCLUSION These results provide further evidence that at high field, SE BOLD signals are relatively free of contributions from sources other than microvascular changes in response to neural activity, whereas GE signals, even in the superficial layers, are not dominated by very large vessels.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhangyan Yang
- Institute of Imaging ScienceVanderbilt University Medical Center
NashvilleTennesseeUSA
- Department of Biomedical EngineeringVanderbilt UniversityNashvilleTennesseeUSA
| | - Mishra Arabinda
- Institute of Imaging ScienceVanderbilt University Medical Center
NashvilleTennesseeUSA
- Department of Radiology and Radiological SciencesVanderbilt University Medical CenterNashvilleTennesseeUSA
| | - Feng Wang
- Institute of Imaging ScienceVanderbilt University Medical Center
NashvilleTennesseeUSA
- Department of Radiology and Radiological SciencesVanderbilt University Medical CenterNashvilleTennesseeUSA
| | - Li Min Chen
- Institute of Imaging ScienceVanderbilt University Medical Center
NashvilleTennesseeUSA
- Department of Radiology and Radiological SciencesVanderbilt University Medical CenterNashvilleTennesseeUSA
| | - John C. Gore
- Institute of Imaging ScienceVanderbilt University Medical Center
NashvilleTennesseeUSA
- Department of Biomedical EngineeringVanderbilt UniversityNashvilleTennesseeUSA
- Department of Radiology and Radiological SciencesVanderbilt University Medical CenterNashvilleTennesseeUSA
- Department of Physics and AstronomyVanderbilt UniversityNashvilleTennesseeUSA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
George D, Lázaro-Gredilla M, Lehrach W, Dedieu A, Zhou G, Marino J. A detailed theory of thalamic and cortical microcircuits for predictive visual inference. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2025; 11:eadr6698. [PMID: 39908384 PMCID: PMC11800772 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.adr6698] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/12/2024] [Accepted: 01/06/2025] [Indexed: 02/07/2025]
Abstract
Understanding cortical microcircuitry requires theoretical models that can tease apart their computational logic from biological details. Although Bayesian inference serves as an abstract framework of cortical computation, precisely mapping concrete instantiations of computational models to biology under real-world tasks is necessary to produce falsifiable neural models. On the basis of a recent generative model, recursive cortical networks, that demonstrated excellent performance on vision benchmarks, we derive a theoretical cortical microcircuit by placing the requirements of the computational model within biological constraints. The derived model suggests precise algorithmic roles for the columnar and laminar feed-forward, feedback, and lateral connections, the thalamic pathway, blobs and interblobs, and the innate lineage-specific interlaminar connectivity within cortical columns. The model also explains several visual phenomena, including the subjective contour effect and neon-color spreading effect, with circuit-level precision. Our model and methodology provides a path forward in understanding cortical and thalamic computations.
Collapse
|
11
|
Faes LK, Zulfiqar I, Vizioli L, Yu Z, Wu YH, Shin J, Cloos MA, Auksztulewicz R, Melloni L, Uludag K, Yacoub E, De Martino F. Predictive acoustical processing in human cortical layers. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2025:2025.01.09.632099. [PMID: 39829870 PMCID: PMC11741426 DOI: 10.1101/2025.01.09.632099] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/22/2025]
Abstract
In our dynamic environments, predictive processing is vital for auditory perception and its associated behaviors. Predictive coding formalizes inferential processes by implementing them as information exchange across cortical layers and areas. With laminar-specific blood oxygenation level dependent we measured responses to a cascading oddball paradigm, to ground predictive auditory processes on the mesoscopic human cortical architecture. We show that the violation of predictions are potentially hierarchically organized and associated with responses in superficial layers of the planum polare and middle layers of the lateral temporal cortex. Moreover, we relate the updating of the brain's internal model to changes in deep layers. Using a modeling approach, we derive putative changes in neural dynamics while accounting for draining effects. Our results support the role of temporal cortical architecture in the implementation of predictive coding and highlight the ability of laminar fMRI to investigate mesoscopic processes in a large extent of temporal areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lonike K Faes
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Isma Zulfiqar
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Brain Sciences, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Luca Vizioli
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, Minneapolis, USA
| | - Zidan Yu
- Center for Advanced Imaging Innovation and Research (CAI2R), Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
- Bernard and Irene Schwartz Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
- MRI Research Center, University of Hawaii, USA
| | - Yuan-Hao Wu
- Department of Neurology, New York University Grossman, New York, New York, USA
| | - Jiyun Shin
- Department of Neurology, New York University Grossman, New York, New York, USA
| | - Martijn A Cloos
- Center for Advanced Imaging Innovation and Research (CAI2R), Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
- Bernard and Irene Schwartz Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
- Australian Institute for Bioengineering and Nanotechnology, University of Queensland, St Lucia 4066, Australia
- Donders Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | - Ryszard Auksztulewicz
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience Berlin, Free University Berlin, Germany
| | - Lucia Melloni
- Department of Neurology, New York University Grossman, New York, New York, USA
- Max Planck For Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
- Predictive Brain Department, Research Center One Health Ruhr, University Alliance Ruhr, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany
| | | | - Essa Yacoub
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, Minneapolis, USA
| | - Federico De Martino
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, Minneapolis, USA
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Chowdhury A, Bianciardi M, Chapdelaine E, Riaz OS, Timmermann C, van Lutterveld R, Sparby T, Sacchet MD. Multimodal neurophenomenology of advanced concentration absorption meditation: An intensively sampled case study of Jhana. Neuroimage 2025; 305:120973. [PMID: 39681243 PMCID: PMC11770875 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2024.120973] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2023] [Revised: 12/01/2024] [Accepted: 12/10/2024] [Indexed: 12/18/2024] Open
Abstract
Using a combination of fMRI, EEG, and phenomenology ratings, we examined the neurophenomenology of advanced concentrative absorption meditation, namely jhanas (ACAM-J), in a practitioner with over 23,000 h of meditation practice. Our study shows that ACAM-J states induce reliable changes in conscious experience and that these experiences are related to neural activity. Using resting-state fMRI functional connectivity, we found that ACAM-J is associated with decreased within-network modularity, increased global functional connectivity (GFC), and desegregation of the default mode and visual networks. Compared to control tasks, the ACAM-J were also related to widespread decreases in broadband EEG oscillatory power and increases in Lempel-Ziv complexity (LZ, a measure of brain entropy). Some fMRI findings varied by the control task used, while EEG results remained consistent, emphasizing both shared and unique neural features of ACAM-J. These differences in fMRI and EEG-measured neurophysiological properties correlated with specific changes in phenomenology - and especially with ACAM-J-induced states of bliss - enriching our understanding of these advanced meditative states. Our results show that advanced meditation practices markedly dysregulate high-level brain systems via practices of enhanced attention to sensations, corroborating recent neurocognitive theories of meditation as the deconstruction of the brain's cortical hierarchy. Overall, our results suggest that ACAM-J is associated with the modulation of large-scale brain networks in both fMRI and EEG, with potential implications for understanding the mechanisms of deep concentration practices and their effects on subjective experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Avijit Chowdhury
- Meditation Research Program, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA; Depression and Anxiety Centre for Discovery and Treatment, Icahn School of Medicine, Mount Sinai Hospital, New York, NY, USA.
| | - Marta Bianciardi
- Brainstem Imaging Lab, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Eric Chapdelaine
- Meditation Research Program, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Omar S Riaz
- Meditation Research Program, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Christopher Timmermann
- Centre for Psychedelic Research, Division of Psychiatry, Department of Brain Sciences, Imperial College London, London, UK
| | - Remko van Lutterveld
- Brain Research and Innovation Centre, Dutch Ministry of Defence; Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center, Utrecht, the Netherlands
| | - Terje Sparby
- Rudolf Steiner University College, Oslo, Norway; Department of Psychology and Psychotherapy, Witten/Herdecke University, Witten, Germany; Integrated Curriculum for Anthroposophic Psychology, Witten/Herdecke University, Witten, Germany
| | - Matthew D Sacchet
- Meditation Research Program, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Phillips WA, Bachmann T, Spratling MW, Muckli L, Petro LS, Zolnik T. Cellular psychology: relating cognition to context-sensitive pyramidal cells. Trends Cogn Sci 2025; 29:28-40. [PMID: 39353837 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2024.09.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/12/2024] [Revised: 09/05/2024] [Accepted: 09/06/2024] [Indexed: 10/04/2024]
Abstract
'Cellular psychology' is a new field of inquiry that studies dendritic mechanisms for adapting mental events to the current context, thus increasing their coherence, flexibility, effectiveness, and comprehensibility. Apical dendrites of neocortical pyramidal cells have a crucial role in cognition - those dendrites receive input from diverse sources, including feedback, and can amplify the cell's feedforward transmission if relevant in that context. Specialized subsets of inhibitory interneurons regulate this cooperative context-sensitive processing by increasing or decreasing amplification. Apical input has different effects on cellular output depending on whether we are awake, deeply asleep, or dreaming. Furthermore, wakeful thought and imagery may depend on apical input. High-resolution neuroimaging in humans supports and complements evidence on these cellular mechanisms from other mammals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- William A Phillips
- Psychology, Faculty of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling, FK9 4LA, UK.
| | - Talis Bachmann
- Institute of Psychology, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia.
| | - Michael W Spratling
- Department of Behavioral and Cognitive Sciences, University of Luxembourg, L-4366 Esch-Belval, Luxembourg
| | - Lars Muckli
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, College of Medical, Veterinary and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, G12 8QB, UK; Imaging Centre of Excellence, College of Medical, Veterinary and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow and Queen Elizabeth University Hospital, Glasgow, UK
| | - Lucy S Petro
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, College of Medical, Veterinary and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, G12 8QB, UK; Imaging Centre of Excellence, College of Medical, Veterinary and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow and Queen Elizabeth University Hospital, Glasgow, UK
| | - Timothy Zolnik
- Department of Biochemistry, Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin 10117, Germany; Department of Biology, Humboldt Universität zu Berlin, Berlin 10117, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Kotlarz P, Lankinen K, Hakonen M, Turpin T, Polimeni JR, Ahveninen J. Multilayer Network Analysis across Cortical Depths in Resting-State 7T fMRI. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2024:2023.12.23.573208. [PMID: 38187540 PMCID: PMC10769454 DOI: 10.1101/2023.12.23.573208] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/09/2024]
Abstract
In graph theory, "multilayer networks" represent systems involving several interconnected topological levels. One example in neuroscience is the stratification of connections between different cortical depths or "laminae", which is becoming non-invasively accessible in humans using ultra-high-resolution functional MRI (fMRI). Here, we applied multilayer graph theory to examine functional connectivity across different cortical depths in humans, using 7T fMRI (1-mm3 voxels; 30 participants). Blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) signals were derived from five depths between the white matter and pial surface. We compared networks where the inter-regional connections were limited to a single cortical depth only ("layer-by-layer matrices") to those considering all possible connections between areas and cortical depths ("multilayer matrix"). We utilized global and local graph theory features that quantitatively characterize network attributes including network composition, nodal centrality, path-based measures, and hub segregation. Detecting functional differences between cortical depths was improved using multilayer connectomics compared to the layer-by-layer versions. Superficial depths of the cortex dominated information transfer and deeper depths drove clustering. These differences were largest in frontotemporal and limbic regions. fMRI functional connectivity across different cortical depths may contain neurophysiologically relevant information; thus, multilayer connectomics could provide a methodological framework for studies on how information flows across this stratification.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Parker Kotlarz
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
- Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Kaisu Lankinen
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
- Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Maria Hakonen
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
- Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | | | - Jonathan R Polimeni
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
- Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
- Division of Health Sciences and Technology, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Jyrki Ahveninen
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
- Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Sohoglu E, Beckers L, Davis MH. Convergent neural signatures of speech prediction error are a biological marker for spoken word recognition. Nat Commun 2024; 15:9984. [PMID: 39557848 PMCID: PMC11574182 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-53782-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/17/2023] [Accepted: 10/17/2024] [Indexed: 11/20/2024] Open
Abstract
We use MEG and fMRI to determine how predictions are combined with speech input in superior temporal cortex. We compare neural responses to words in which first syllables strongly or weakly predict second syllables (e.g., "bingo", "snigger" versus "tango", "meagre"). We further compare neural responses to the same second syllables when predictions mismatch with input during pseudoword perception (e.g., "snigo" and "meago"). Neural representations of second syllables are suppressed by strong predictions when predictions match sensory input but show the opposite effect when predictions mismatch. Computational simulations show that this interaction is consistent with prediction error but not alternative (sharpened signal) computations. Neural signatures of prediction error are observed 200 ms after second syllable onset and in early auditory regions (bilateral Heschl's gyrus and STG). These findings demonstrate prediction error computations during the identification of familiar spoken words and perception of unfamiliar pseudowords.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ediz Sohoglu
- School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, UK.
| | - Loes Beckers
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Cochlear Ltd., Mechelen, Belgium
| | - Matthew H Davis
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Banaraki AK, Toghi A, Mohammadzadeh A. RDoC Framework Through the Lens of Predictive Processing: Focusing on Cognitive Systems Domain. COMPUTATIONAL PSYCHIATRY (CAMBRIDGE, MASS.) 2024; 8:178-201. [PMID: 39478691 PMCID: PMC11523845 DOI: 10.5334/cpsy.119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2024] [Accepted: 10/11/2024] [Indexed: 11/02/2024]
Abstract
In response to shortcomings of the current classification system in translating discoveries from basic science to clinical applications, NIMH offers a new framework for studying mental health disorders called Research Domain Criteria (RDoC). This framework holds a multidimensional outlook on psychopathologies focusing on functional domains of behavior and their implementing neural circuits. In parallel, the Predictive Processing (PP) framework stands as a leading theory of human brain function, offering a unified explanation for various types of information processing in the brain. While both frameworks share an interest in studying psychopathologies based on pathophysiology, their integration still needs to be explored. Here, we argued in favor of the explanatory power of PP to be a groundwork for the RDoC matrix in validating its constructs and creating testable hypotheses about mechanistic interactions between molecular biomarkers and clinical traits. Together, predictive processing may serve as a foundation for achieving the goals of the RDoC framework.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Armin Toghi
- Institute for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran
| | - Azar Mohammadzadeh
- Research Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Studies, Tehran University of Medical Science, Tehran, Iran
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Degutis JK, Chaimow D, Haenelt D, Assem M, Duncan J, Haynes JD, Weiskopf N, Lorenz R. Dynamic layer-specific processing in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Commun Biol 2024; 7:1140. [PMID: 39277694 PMCID: PMC11401931 DOI: 10.1038/s42003-024-06780-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2023] [Accepted: 08/26/2024] [Indexed: 09/17/2024] Open
Abstract
The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) is reliably engaged in working memory (WM) and comprises different cytoarchitectonic layers, yet their functional role in human WM is unclear. Here, participants completed a delayed-match-to-sample task while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) at ultra-high resolution. We examine layer-specific activity to manipulations in WM load and motor response. Superficial layers exhibit a preferential response to WM load during the delay and retrieval periods of a WM task, indicating a lamina-specific activation of the frontoparietal network. Multivariate patterns encoding WM load in the superficial layer dynamically change across the three periods of the task. Last, superficial and deep layers are non-differentially involved in the motor response, challenging earlier findings of a preferential deep layer activation. Taken together, our results provide new insights into the functional laminar circuitry of the dlPFC during WM and support a dynamic account of dlPFC coding.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonas Karolis Degutis
- Department of Psychology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany.
- Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience Berlin and Berlin Center for Advanced Neuroimaging, Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, corporate member of the Freie Universität Berlin, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, and Berlin Institute of Health, Berlin, Germany.
- Max Planck School of Cognition, Leipzig, Germany.
| | - Denis Chaimow
- Department of Neurophysics, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Daniel Haenelt
- Department of Neurophysics, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Moataz Assem
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, Cambridgeshire, UK
| | - John Duncan
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, Cambridgeshire, UK
| | - John-Dylan Haynes
- Department of Psychology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience Berlin and Berlin Center for Advanced Neuroimaging, Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, corporate member of the Freie Universität Berlin, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, and Berlin Institute of Health, Berlin, Germany
- Max Planck School of Cognition, Leipzig, Germany
- Research Training Group "Extrospection" and Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Research Cluster of Excellence "Science of Intelligence", Technische Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Collaborative Research Center "Volition and Cognitive Control", Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
| | - Nikolaus Weiskopf
- Department of Psychology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Department of Neurophysics, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
- Felix Bloch Institute for Solid State Physics, Faculty of Physics and Earth Sciences, Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Romy Lorenz
- Department of Neurophysics, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.
- Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Cavanagh P. Using Illusions to Track the Emergence of Visual Perception. Annu Rev Vis Sci 2024; 10:1-22. [PMID: 38871345 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-vision-103023-012730] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/15/2024]
Abstract
Everybody loves illusions. At times, the content on the internet seems to be mostly about illusions-shoes, dresses, straight lines looking bent. This attraction has a long history. Almost 2,000 years ago, Ptolemy marveled at how the sail of a distant boat could appear convex or concave. This sense of marvel continues to drive our fascination with illusions; indeed, few other corners of science can boast of such a large reach. However, illusions not only draw in the crowds; they also offer insights into visual processes. This review starts with a simple definition of illusions as conflicts between perception and cognition, where what we see does not agree with what we believe we should see. This mismatch can be either because cognition has misunderstood how perception works or because perception has misjudged the visual input. It is the perceptual errors that offer the chance to track the development of perception across visual regions. Unfortunately, the effects of illusions in different brain regions cannot be isolated in any simple way: Top-down projections from attention broadcast the expected perceptual properties everywhere, obscuring the critical evidence of where the illusion and perception emerge. The second part of this review then highlights the roadblocks to research raised by attention and describes current solutions for accessing what illusions can offer.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patrick Cavanagh
- Department of Psychology, Glendon College; and Center for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada;
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Faes LK, Lage-Castellanos A, Valente G, Yu Z, Cloos MA, Vizioli L, Moeller S, Yacoub E, De Martino F. Evaluating the effect of denoising submillimeter auditory fMRI data with NORDIC. IMAGING NEUROSCIENCE (CAMBRIDGE, MASS.) 2024; 2:1-18. [PMID: 39810817 PMCID: PMC11726685 DOI: 10.1162/imag_a_00270] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/24/2024] [Revised: 07/22/2024] [Accepted: 07/22/2024] [Indexed: 01/16/2025]
Abstract
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has emerged as an essential tool for exploring human brain function. Submillimeter fMRI, in particular, has emerged as a tool to study mesoscopic computations. The inherently low signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) at submillimeter resolutions warrants the use of denoising approaches tailored at reducing thermal noise-the dominant contributing noise component in high-resolution fMRI. NOise Reduction with DIstribution Corrected Principal Component Analysis (NORDIC PCA) is one of such approaches, and has been benchmarked against other approaches in several applications. Here, we investigate the effects that two versions of NORDIC denoising have on auditory submillimeter data. While investigating auditory functional responses poses unique challenges, we anticipated NORDIC to have a positive impact on the data on the basis of previous applications. Our results show that NORDIC denoising improves the detection sensitivity and the reliability of estimates in submillimeter auditory fMRI data. These effects can be explained by the reduction of the noise-induced signal variability. However, we did observe a reduction in the average response amplitude (percent signal change) within regions of interest, which may suggest that a portion of the signal of interest, which could not be distinguished from general i.i.d. noise, was also removed. We conclude that, while evaluating the effects of the signal reduction induced by NORDIC may be necessary for each application, using NORDIC in high-resolution auditory fMRI studies may be advantageous because of the large reduction in variability of the estimated responses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lonike K. Faes
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Agustin Lage-Castellanos
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
- Department of Neuroinformatics, Cuban Neuroscience Center, Havana City, Cuba
| | - Giancarlo Valente
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Zidan Yu
- Center for Advanced Imaging Innovation and Research (CAI2R), Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, United States
- Bernard and Irene Schwartz Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, United States
- MRI Research Center, University of Hawaii, Honolulu, HI, United States
| | - Martijn A. Cloos
- Center for Advanced Imaging Innovation and Research (CAI2R), Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, United States
- Bernard and Irene Schwartz Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, United States
- Australian Institute for Bioengineering and Nanotechnology, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Australia
| | - Luca Vizioli
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, Department of Radiology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
| | - Steen Moeller
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, Department of Radiology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
| | - Essa Yacoub
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, Department of Radiology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
| | - Federico De Martino
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, Department of Radiology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Carricarte T, Iamshchinina P, Trampel R, Chaimow D, Weiskopf N, Cichy RM. Laminar dissociation of feedforward and feedback in high-level ventral visual cortex during imagery and perception. iScience 2024; 27:110229. [PMID: 39006482 PMCID: PMC11246059 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2024.110229] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2023] [Revised: 01/26/2024] [Accepted: 06/06/2024] [Indexed: 07/16/2024] Open
Abstract
Visual imagery and perception share neural machinery but rely on different information flow. While perception is driven by the integration of sensory feedforward and internally generated feedback information, imagery relies on feedback only. This suggests that although imagery and perception may activate overlapping brain regions, they do so in informationally distinctive ways. Using lamina-resolved MRI at 7 T, we measured the neural activity during imagery and perception of faces and scenes in high-level ventral visual cortex at the mesoscale of laminar organization that distinguishes feedforward from feedback signals. We found distinctive laminar profiles for imagery and perception of scenes and faces in the parahippocampal place area and the fusiform face area, respectively. Our findings provide insight into the neural basis of the phenomenology of visual imagery versus perception and shed new light into the mesoscale organization of feedforward and feedback information flow in high-level ventral visual cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tony Carricarte
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, 14195 Berlin, Germany
- Einstein Center for Neurosciences Berlin, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany
- Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience Berlin, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany
| | - Polina Iamshchinina
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, New Jersey 08544, USA
| | - Robert Trampel
- Department of Neurophysics, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Denis Chaimow
- Department of Neurophysics, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Nikolaus Weiskopf
- Department of Neurophysics, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
- Felix Bloch Institute for Solid State Physics, Faculty of Physics and Earth Sciences, Universität Leipzig, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Radoslaw M. Cichy
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, 14195 Berlin, Germany
- Einstein Center for Neurosciences Berlin, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany
- Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience Berlin, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany
- Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Center EG, Federmeier KD, Beck DM. The Brain's Sensitivity to Real-world Statistical Regularity Does Not Require Full Attention. J Cogn Neurosci 2024; 36:1715-1740. [PMID: 38739561 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02181] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/16/2024]
Abstract
Predictive coding accounts of perception state that the brain generates perceptual predictions in the service of processing incoming sensory data. These predictions are hypothesized to be afforded by the brain's ability to internalize useful patterns, that is, statistical regularities, from the environment. We have previously argued that the N300 ERP component serves as an index of the brain's use of representations of (real-world) statistical regularities. However, we do not yet know whether overt attention is necessary in order for this process to engage. We addressed this question by presenting stimuli of either high or low real-world statistical regularity in terms of their representativeness (good/bad exemplars of natural scene categories) to participants who either fully attended the stimuli or were distracted by another task (attended/distracted conditions). Replicating past work, N300 responses were larger to bad than to good scene exemplars, and furthermore, we demonstrate minimal impacts of distraction on N300 effects. Thus, it seems that overtly focused attention is not required to maintain the brain's sensitivity to real-world statistical regularity. Furthermore, in an exploratory analysis, we showed that providing additional, artificial regularities, formed by altering the proportions of good and bad exemplars within blocks, further enhanced the N300 effect in both attended and distracted conditions, shedding light on the relationship between statistical regularities learned in the real world and those learned within the context of an experiment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Evan G Center
- University of Oulu
- University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Báez-Yáñez MG, Siero JCW, Curcic V, van Osch MJP, Petridou N. On the influence of the vascular architecture on Gradient Echo and Spin Echo BOLD fMRI signals across cortical depth: a simulation approach based on realistic 3D vascular networks. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2024:2024.05.30.596593. [PMID: 38853905 PMCID: PMC11160811 DOI: 10.1101/2024.05.30.596593] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/11/2024]
Abstract
GE-BOLD contrast stands out as the predominant technique in functional MRI experiments for its high sensitivity and straightforward implementation. GE-BOLD exhibits rather similar sensitivity to vessels independent of their size at submillimeter resolution studies like those examining cortical columns and laminae. However, the presence of nonspecific macrovascular contributions poses a challenge to accurately isolate neuronal activity. SE-BOLD increases specificity towards small vessels, thereby enhancing its specificity to neuronal activity, due to the effective suppression of extravascular contributions caused by macrovessels with its refocusing pulse. However, even SE-BOLD measurements may not completely remove these macrovascular contributions. By simulating hemodynamic signals across cortical depth, we gain insights into vascular contributions to the laminar BOLD signal. In this study, we employed four realistic 3D vascular models to simulate oxygen saturation states in various vascular compartments, aiming to characterize both intravascular and extravascular contributions to GE and SE signals, and corresponding BOLD signal changes, across cortical depth at 7T. Simulations suggest that SE-BOLD cannot completely reduce the macrovascular contribution near the pial surface. Simulations also show that both the specificity and signal amplitude of BOLD signals at 7T depend on the spatial arrangement of large vessels throughout cortical depth and on the pial surface.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mario Gilberto Báez-Yáñez
- Translational Neuroimaging Group, Center for Image Sciences, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, the Netherlands
| | - Jeroen C W Siero
- Translational Neuroimaging Group, Center for Image Sciences, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, the Netherlands
- Spinoza Centre for Neuroimaging Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Vanja Curcic
- Translational Neuroimaging Group, Center for Image Sciences, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, the Netherlands
| | - Matthias J P van Osch
- C.J. Gorter MRI Center, Department of Radiology, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Natalia Petridou
- Translational Neuroimaging Group, Center for Image Sciences, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Wang X, Krieger-Redwood K, Lyu B, Lowndes R, Wu G, Souter NE, Wang X, Kong R, Shafiei G, Bernhardt BC, Cui Z, Smallwood J, Du Y, Jefferies E. The Brain's Topographical Organization Shapes Dynamic Interaction Patterns That Support Flexible Behavior Based on Rules and Long-Term Knowledge. J Neurosci 2024; 44:e2223232024. [PMID: 38527807 PMCID: PMC11140685 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2223-23.2024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/15/2023] [Revised: 03/14/2024] [Accepted: 03/17/2024] [Indexed: 03/27/2024] Open
Abstract
Adaptive behavior relies both on specific rules that vary across situations and stable long-term knowledge gained from experience. The frontoparietal control network (FPCN) is implicated in the brain's ability to balance these different influences on action. Here, we investigate how the topographical organization of the cortex supports behavioral flexibility within the FPCN. Functional properties of this network might reflect its juxtaposition between the dorsal attention network (DAN) and the default mode network (DMN), two large-scale systems implicated in top-down attention and memory-guided cognition, respectively. Our study tests whether subnetworks of FPCN are topographically proximal to the DAN and the DMN, respectively, and how these topographical differences relate to functional differences: the proximity of each subnetwork is anticipated to play a pivotal role in generating distinct cognitive modes relevant to working memory and long-term memory. We show that FPCN subsystems share multiple anatomical and functional similarities with their neighboring systems (DAN and DMN) and that this topographical architecture supports distinct interaction patterns that give rise to different patterns of functional behavior. The FPCN acts as a unified system when long-term knowledge supports behavior but becomes segregated into discrete subsystems with different patterns of interaction when long-term memory is less relevant. In this way, our study suggests that the topographical organization of the FPCN and the connections it forms with distant regions of cortex are important influences on how this system supports flexible behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiuyi Wang
- CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
- Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington, York YO10 5DD, United Kingdom
| | - Katya Krieger-Redwood
- Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington, York YO10 5DD, United Kingdom
| | - Baihan Lyu
- CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
| | - Rebecca Lowndes
- Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington, York YO10 5DD, United Kingdom
| | - Guowei Wu
- CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
| | - Nicholas E Souter
- Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington, York YO10 5DD, United Kingdom
| | - Xiaokang Wang
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of California, Davis, California 95616
| | - Ru Kong
- Centre for Sleep and Cognition (CSC) & Centre for Translational Magnetic Resonance Research (TMR), Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, Singapore 117549, Singapore
| | - Golia Shafiei
- Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104
| | - Boris C Bernhardt
- McConnell Brain Imaging Centre, Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec H3A 2B4, Canada
| | - Zaixu Cui
- Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, China
| | - Jonathan Smallwood
- Department of Psychology, Queens University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
| | - Yi Du
- CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
- Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, China
- CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Shanghai 200031, China
| | - Elizabeth Jefferies
- Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington, York YO10 5DD, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Naccache L, Munoz-Musat E. A global neuronal workspace model of functional neurological disorders. DIALOGUES IN CLINICAL NEUROSCIENCE 2024; 26:1-23. [PMID: 38767966 PMCID: PMC11107854 DOI: 10.1080/19585969.2024.2340131] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/06/2023] [Accepted: 04/03/2024] [Indexed: 05/22/2024]
Abstract
We introduce here a general model of Functional Neurological Disorders based on the following hypothesis: a Functional Neurological Disorder could correspond to a consciously initiated voluntary top-down process causing involuntary lasting consequences that are consciously experienced and subjectively interpreted by the patient as involuntary. We develop this central hypothesis according to Global Neuronal Workspace theory of consciousness, that is particularly suited to describe interactions between conscious and non-conscious cognitive processes. We then present a list of predictions defining a research program aimed at empirically testing their validity. Finally, this general model leads us to reinterpret the long-debated links between hypnotic suggestion and functional neurological disorders. Driven by both scientific and therapeutic goals, this theoretical paper aims at bringing closer the psychiatric and neurological worlds of functional neurological disorders with the latest developments of cognitive neuroscience of consciousness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lionel Naccache
- Institut du Cerveau - Paris Brain Institute - ICM, Inserm, CNRS, Paris, France- Sorbonne Université, UPMC Univ Paris 06, Faculté de Médecine Pitié-Salpêtrière, Sorbonne Université, Paris, France
- Department of Neurology, AP-HP, Hôpital Groupe hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière, DMU Neurosciences, Paris, France
- Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, AP-HP, Hôpital Groupe hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière, DMU Neurosciences, Paris, France
| | - Esteban Munoz-Musat
- Institut du Cerveau - Paris Brain Institute - ICM, Inserm, CNRS, Paris, France- Sorbonne Université, UPMC Univ Paris 06, Faculté de Médecine Pitié-Salpêtrière, Sorbonne Université, Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Thomas ER, Haarsma J, Nicholson J, Yon D, Kok P, Press C. Predictions and errors are distinctly represented across V1 layers. Curr Biol 2024; 34:2265-2271.e4. [PMID: 38697110 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2024.04.036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/22/2024] [Revised: 04/09/2024] [Accepted: 04/13/2024] [Indexed: 05/04/2024]
Abstract
Popular accounts of mind and brain propose that the brain continuously forms predictions about future sensory inputs and combines predictions with inputs to determine what we perceive.1,2,3,4,5,6 Under "predictive processing" schemes, such integration is supported by the hierarchical organization of the cortex, whereby feedback connections communicate predictions from higher-level deep layers to agranular (superficial and deep) lower-level layers.7,8,9,10 Predictions are compared with input to compute the "prediction error," which is transmitted up the hierarchy from superficial layers of lower cortical regions to the middle layers of higher areas, to update higher-level predictions until errors are reconciled.11,12,13,14,15 In the primary visual cortex (V1), predictions have thereby been proposed to influence representations in deep layers while error signals may be computed in superficial layers. Despite the framework's popularity, there is little evidence for these functional distinctions because, to our knowledge, unexpected sensory events have not previously been presented in human laminar paradigms to contrast against expected events. To this end, this 7T fMRI study contrasted V1 responses to expected (75% likely) and unexpected (25%) Gabor orientations. Multivariate decoding analyses revealed an interaction between expectation and layer, such that expected events could be decoded with comparable accuracy across layers, while unexpected events could only be decoded in superficial laminae. Although these results are in line with these accounts that have been popular for decades, such distinctions have not previously been demonstrated in humans. We discuss how both prediction and error processes may operate together to shape our unitary perceptual experiences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily R Thomas
- Neuroscience Institute, New York University Medical Center, 435 East 30(th) Street, New York 10016, USA; Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London WC1E 7HX, UK.
| | - Joost Haarsma
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK
| | - Jessica Nicholson
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London WC1E 7HX, UK
| | - Daniel Yon
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London WC1E 7HX, UK
| | - Peter Kok
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK
| | - Clare Press
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London WC1E 7HX, UK; Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK; Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London WC1H 0AP, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Storm JF, Klink PC, Aru J, Senn W, Goebel R, Pigorini A, Avanzini P, Vanduffel W, Roelfsema PR, Massimini M, Larkum ME, Pennartz CMA. An integrative, multiscale view on neural theories of consciousness. Neuron 2024; 112:1531-1552. [PMID: 38447578 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2024.02.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 18.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/08/2023] [Revised: 12/20/2023] [Accepted: 02/05/2024] [Indexed: 03/08/2024]
Abstract
How is conscious experience related to material brain processes? A variety of theories aiming to answer this age-old question have emerged from the recent surge in consciousness research, and some are now hotly debated. Although most researchers have so far focused on the development and validation of their preferred theory in relative isolation, this article, written by a group of scientists representing different theories, takes an alternative approach. Noting that various theories often try to explain different aspects or mechanistic levels of consciousness, we argue that the theories do not necessarily contradict each other. Instead, several of them may converge on fundamental neuronal mechanisms and be partly compatible and complementary, so that multiple theories can simultaneously contribute to our understanding. Here, we consider unifying, integration-oriented approaches that have so far been largely neglected, seeking to combine valuable elements from various theories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Johan F Storm
- The Brain Signaling Group, Division of Physiology, IMB, Faculty of Medicine, University of Oslo, Domus Medica, Sognsvannsveien 9, Blindern, 0317 Oslo, Norway.
| | - P Christiaan Klink
- Department of Vision and Cognition, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, 1105 BA Amsterdam, the Netherlands; Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, 3584 CS Utrecht, the Netherlands; Laboratory of Visual Brain Therapy, Sorbonne Université, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Institut de la Vision, Paris 75012, France
| | - Jaan Aru
- Institute of Computer Science, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia
| | - Walter Senn
- Department of Physiology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| | - Rainer Goebel
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Oxfordlaan 55, 6229 EV Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Andrea Pigorini
- Department of Biomedical, Surgical and Dental Sciences, Università degli Studi di Milano, Milan 20122, Italy
| | - Pietro Avanzini
- Istituto di Neuroscienze, Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, 43125 Parma, Italy
| | - Wim Vanduffel
- Department of Neurosciences, Laboratory of Neuro and Psychophysiology, KU Leuven Medical School, 3000 Leuven, Belgium; Leuven Brain Institute, KU Leuven, 3000 Leuven, Belgium; Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA; Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02144, USA
| | - Pieter R Roelfsema
- Department of Vision and Cognition, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, 1105 BA Amsterdam, the Netherlands; Laboratory of Visual Brain Therapy, Sorbonne Université, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Institut de la Vision, Paris 75012, France; Department of Integrative Neurophysiology, VU University, De Boelelaan 1085, 1081 HV Amsterdam, the Netherlands; Department of Neurosurgery, Academisch Medisch Centrum, Postbus 22660, 1100 DD Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Marcello Massimini
- Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences "L. Sacco", Università degli Studi di Milano, Milan 20157, Italy; Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico, Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi, Milan 20122, Italy; Azrieli Program in Brain, Mind and Consciousness, Canadian Institute for Advanced Research (CIFAR), Toronto, ON M5G 1M1, Canada
| | - Matthew E Larkum
- Institute of Biology, Humboldt University Berlin, Berlin, Germany; Neurocure Center for Excellence, Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Cyriel M A Pennartz
- Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, Center for Neuroscience, Faculty of Science, University of Amsterdam, Sciencepark 904, Amsterdam 1098 XH, the Netherlands; Research Priority Program Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Ishida T, Nittono H. Visual omitted stimulus potentials are not retinotopic. Neurosci Lett 2024; 830:137777. [PMID: 38621505 DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2024.137777] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2024] [Revised: 04/03/2024] [Accepted: 04/12/2024] [Indexed: 04/17/2024]
Abstract
Omitted stimulus potentials (OSPs) are elicited in response to the omission of expected stimuli and are thought to reflect prediction errors. If prediction errors are signaled in the sensory cortex, OSPs are expected to be generated in the sensory cortex. The present study investigated the involvement of the early visual cortex in the generation of OSPs by testing whether omitted visual stimuli elicit brain responses in a spatially specific manner. Checkerboard pattern stimuli were presented alternately in the upper and lower visual fields, and the stimuli were omitted in 10 % of the trials. Event-related potentials were recorded from 33 participants. While a retinotopic C1 component was evoked by real visual stimuli, omitted stimuli did not produce any response reflecting retinotopy but did elicit a visual mismatch negativity, which was larger for omitted stimuli expected in the lower visual field than for those in the upper visual field. These results suggest that omitted visual stimuli are processed in a different pathway than actual stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tomomi Ishida
- Graduate School of Human Sciences, Osaka University, Osaka, Japan.
| | - Hiroshi Nittono
- Graduate School of Human Sciences, Osaka University, Osaka, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Milne GA, Lisi M, McLean A, Zheng R, Groen II, Dekker TM. Perceptual reorganization from prior knowledge emerges late in childhood. iScience 2024; 27:108787. [PMID: 38303715 PMCID: PMC10831247 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2024.108787] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/06/2023] [Revised: 09/05/2023] [Accepted: 01/02/2024] [Indexed: 02/03/2024] Open
Abstract
Human vision relies heavily on prior knowledge. Here, we show for the first time that prior-knowledge-induced reshaping of visual inputs emerges gradually in late childhood. To isolate the effects of prior knowledge on perception, we presented 4- to 12-year-olds and adults with two-tone images - hard-to-recognize degraded photos. In adults, seeing the original photo triggers perceptual reorganization, causing mandatory recognition of the two-tone version. This involves top-down signaling from higher-order brain areas to early visual cortex. We show that children younger than 7-9 years do not experience this knowledge-guided shift, despite viewing the original photo immediately before each two-tone. To assess computations underlying this development, we compared human performance to three neural networks with varying architectures. The best-performing model behaved much like 4- to 5-year-olds, displaying feature-based rather than holistic processing strategies. The reconciliation of prior knowledge with sensory input undergoes a striking age-related shift, which may underpin the development of many perceptual abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Georgia A. Milne
- Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, EC1V 9EL London, UK
- Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, WC1H 0AP London, UK
| | - Matteo Lisi
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, TW20 0EX London, UK
| | - Aisha McLean
- Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, EC1V 9EL London, UK
| | - Rosie Zheng
- Informatics Institute, University of Amsterdam, 1098 XH Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Iris I.A. Groen
- Informatics Institute, University of Amsterdam, 1098 XH Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Tessa M. Dekker
- Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, EC1V 9EL London, UK
- Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, WC1H 0AP London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Lankinen K, Ahveninen J, Jas M, Raij T, Ahlfors SP. Neuronal modeling of magnetoencephalography responses in auditory cortex to auditory and visual stimuli. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2024:2023.06.16.545371. [PMID: 37398025 PMCID: PMC10312796 DOI: 10.1101/2023.06.16.545371] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/04/2023]
Abstract
Previous studies have demonstrated that auditory cortex activity can be influenced by crosssensory visual inputs. Intracortical recordings in non-human primates (NHP) have suggested a bottom-up feedforward (FF) type laminar profile for auditory evoked but top-down feedback (FB) type for cross-sensory visual evoked activity in the auditory cortex. To test whether this principle applies also to humans, we analyzed magnetoencephalography (MEG) responses from eight human subjects (six females) evoked by simple auditory or visual stimuli. In the estimated MEG source waveforms for auditory cortex region of interest, auditory evoked responses showed peaks at 37 and 90 ms and cross-sensory visual responses at 125 ms. The inputs to the auditory cortex were then modeled through FF and FB type connections targeting different cortical layers using the Human Neocortical Neurosolver (HNN), which consists of a neocortical circuit model linking the cellular- and circuit-level mechanisms to MEG. The HNN models suggested that the measured auditory response could be explained by an FF input followed by an FB input, and the crosssensory visual response by an FB input. Thus, the combined MEG and HNN results support the hypothesis that cross-sensory visual input in the auditory cortex is of FB type. The results also illustrate how the dynamic patterns of the estimated MEG/EEG source activity can provide information about the characteristics of the input into a cortical area in terms of the hierarchical organization among areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kaisu Lankinen
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129
- Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115
| | - Jyrki Ahveninen
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129
- Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115
| | - Mainak Jas
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129
- Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115
| | - Tommi Raij
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129
- Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115
| | - Seppo P. Ahlfors
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129
- Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Bergmann J, Petro LS, Abbatecola C, Li MS, Morgan AT, Muckli L. Cortical depth profiles in primary visual cortex for illusory and imaginary experiences. Nat Commun 2024; 15:1002. [PMID: 38307834 PMCID: PMC10837448 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-45065-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2022] [Accepted: 01/12/2024] [Indexed: 02/04/2024] Open
Abstract
Visual illusions and mental imagery are non-physical sensory experiences that involve cortical feedback processing in the primary visual cortex. Using laminar functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in two studies, we investigate if information about these internal experiences is visible in the activation patterns of different layers of primary visual cortex (V1). We find that imagery content is decodable mainly from deep layers of V1, whereas seemingly 'real' illusory content is decodable mainly from superficial layers. Furthermore, illusory content shares information with perceptual content, whilst imagery content does not generalise to illusory or perceptual information. Together, our results suggest that illusions and imagery, which differ immensely in their subjective experiences, also involve partially distinct early visual microcircuits. However, overlapping microcircuit recruitment might emerge based on the nuanced nature of subjective conscious experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Johanna Bergmann
- Imaging Centre of Excellence (ICE), Queen Elizabeth University Hospital, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK.
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (CCNi), School of Psychology and Neuroscience, College of Medical, Veterinary and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK.
- Department of Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.
| | - Lucy S Petro
- Imaging Centre of Excellence (ICE), Queen Elizabeth University Hospital, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (CCNi), School of Psychology and Neuroscience, College of Medical, Veterinary and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
| | - Clement Abbatecola
- Imaging Centre of Excellence (ICE), Queen Elizabeth University Hospital, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (CCNi), School of Psychology and Neuroscience, College of Medical, Veterinary and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
| | - Min S Li
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (CCNi), School of Psychology and Neuroscience, College of Medical, Veterinary and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
- Centre for Computational Neuroscience and Cognitive Robotics, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
| | - A Tyler Morgan
- Imaging Centre of Excellence (ICE), Queen Elizabeth University Hospital, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (CCNi), School of Psychology and Neuroscience, College of Medical, Veterinary and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
- Functional MRI Core Facility, National Institute of Mental Health, NIH, Bethesda, MD, 20817, USA
| | - Lars Muckli
- Imaging Centre of Excellence (ICE), Queen Elizabeth University Hospital, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK.
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (CCNi), School of Psychology and Neuroscience, College of Medical, Veterinary and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Hodson R, Mehta M, Smith R. The empirical status of predictive coding and active inference. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2024; 157:105473. [PMID: 38030100 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105473] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2023] [Revised: 10/27/2023] [Accepted: 11/16/2023] [Indexed: 12/01/2023]
Abstract
Research on predictive processing models has focused largely on two specific algorithmic theories: Predictive Coding for perception and Active Inference for decision-making. While these interconnected theories possess broad explanatory potential, they have only recently begun to receive direct empirical evaluation. Here, we review recent studies of Predictive Coding and Active Inference with a focus on evaluating the degree to which they are empirically supported. For Predictive Coding, we find that existing empirical evidence offers modest support. However, some positive results can also be explained by alternative feedforward (e.g., feature detection-based) models. For Active Inference, most empirical studies have focused on fitting these models to behavior as a means of identifying and explaining individual or group differences. While Active Inference models tend to explain behavioral data reasonably well, there has not been a focus on testing empirical validity of active inference theory per se, which would require formal comparison to other models (e.g., non-Bayesian or model-free reinforcement learning models). This review suggests that, while promising, a number of specific research directions are still necessary to evaluate the empirical adequacy and explanatory power of these algorithms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Ryan Smith
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Faes LK, Lage-Castellanos A, Valente G, Yu Z, Cloos MA, Vizioli L, Moeller S, Yacoub E, De Martino F. Evaluating the effect of denoising submillimeter auditory fMRI data with NORDIC. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2024:2024.01.24.577070. [PMID: 38328173 PMCID: PMC10849717 DOI: 10.1101/2024.01.24.577070] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/09/2024]
Abstract
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has emerged as an essential tool for exploring human brain function. Submillimeter fMRI, in particular, has emerged as a tool to study mesoscopic computations. The inherently low signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) at submillimeter resolutions warrants the use of denoising approaches tailored at reducing thermal noise - the dominant contributing noise component in high resolution fMRI. NORDIC PCA is one of such approaches, and has been benchmarked against other approaches in several applications. Here, we investigate the effects that two versions of NORDIC denoising have on auditory submillimeter data. As investigating auditory functional responses poses unique challenges, we anticipated that the benefit of this technique would be especially pronounced. Our results show that NORDIC denoising improves the detection sensitivity and the reliability of estimates in submillimeter auditory fMRI data. These effects can be explained by the reduction of the noise-induced signal variability. However, we also observed a reduction in the average response amplitude (percent signal), which may suggest that a small amount of signal was also removed. We conclude that, while evaluating the effects of the signal reduction induced by NORDIC may be necessary for each application, using NORDIC in high resolution auditory fMRI studies may be advantageous because of the large reduction in variability of the estimated responses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lonike K. Faes
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, 6200 MD, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Agustin Lage-Castellanos
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, 6200 MD, Maastricht, The Netherlands
- Department of Neuroinformatics, Cuban Neuroscience Center, Havana City 11600, Cuba
| | - Giancarlo Valente
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, 6200 MD, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Zidan Yu
- Center for Advanced Imaging Innovation and Research (CAI2R), Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
- Bernard and Irene Schwartz Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
- MRI Research Center, University of Hawaii, United States
| | - Martijn A. Cloos
- Center for Advanced Imaging Innovation and Research (CAI2R), Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
- Bernard and Irene Schwartz Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
- Australian Institute for Bioengineering and Nanotechnology, University of Queensland, St Lucia 4066, Australia
| | - Luca Vizioli
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, Department of Radiology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States
| | - Steen Moeller
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, Department of Radiology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States
| | - Essa Yacoub
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, Department of Radiology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States
| | - Federico De Martino
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, 6200 MD, Maastricht, The Netherlands
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, Department of Radiology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Nakashima Y, Kanazawa S, Yamaguchi MK. Metacontrast masking is ineffective in the first 6 months of life. Cognition 2024; 242:105666. [PMID: 37984131 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105666] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/26/2023] [Revised: 11/09/2023] [Accepted: 11/11/2023] [Indexed: 11/22/2023]
Abstract
Metacontrast masking is one of the most widely studied types of visual masking, in which a visual stimulus is rendered invisible by a subsequent mask that does not spatially overlap with the target. Metacontrast has been used for many decades as a tool to study visual processing and conscious perception in adults. However, there are so far no infant studies on metacontrast and it remains unknown even whether it occurs in infants. The present study examined metacontrast masking in 3- to 8-month-old infants (N = 168) using a habituation paradigm. We found that metacontrast is ineffective for infants under 7 months and that younger infants can perceive a masked stimulus that older infants cannot. Our results suggest that metacontrast is distinct from other simple types of masking that occur in early infancy, and would be consistent with the idea that metacontrast results from the disruption of recurrent processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yusuke Nakashima
- Research and Development Initiative, Chuo University, 742-1 Higashinakano, Hachioji-shi, Tokyo 192-0393, Japan.
| | - So Kanazawa
- Department of Psychology, Japan Women's University, 2-8-1 Mejirodai, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo 112-8681, Japan
| | - Masami K Yamaguchi
- Department of Psychology, Chuo University, 742-1 Higashinakano, Hachioji-shi, Tokyo 192-0393, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Peelen MV, Berlot E, de Lange FP. Predictive processing of scenes and objects. NATURE REVIEWS PSYCHOLOGY 2024; 3:13-26. [PMID: 38989004 PMCID: PMC7616164 DOI: 10.1038/s44159-023-00254-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/25/2023] [Indexed: 07/12/2024]
Abstract
Real-world visual input consists of rich scenes that are meaningfully composed of multiple objects which interact in complex, but predictable, ways. Despite this complexity, we recognize scenes, and objects within these scenes, from a brief glance at an image. In this review, we synthesize recent behavioral and neural findings that elucidate the mechanisms underlying this impressive ability. First, we review evidence that visual object and scene processing is partly implemented in parallel, allowing for a rapid initial gist of both objects and scenes concurrently. Next, we discuss recent evidence for bidirectional interactions between object and scene processing, with scene information modulating the visual processing of objects, and object information modulating the visual processing of scenes. Finally, we review evidence that objects also combine with each other to form object constellations, modulating the processing of individual objects within the object pathway. Altogether, these findings can be understood by conceptualizing object and scene perception as the outcome of a joint probabilistic inference, in which "best guesses" about objects act as priors for scene perception and vice versa, in order to concurrently optimize visual inference of objects and scenes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marius V Peelen
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Eva Berlot
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Floris P de Lange
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Feinberg DA, Beckett AJS, Vu AT, Stockmann J, Huber L, Ma S, Ahn S, Setsompop K, Cao X, Park S, Liu C, Wald LL, Polimeni JR, Mareyam A, Gruber B, Stirnberg R, Liao C, Yacoub E, Davids M, Bell P, Rummert E, Koehler M, Potthast A, Gonzalez-Insua I, Stocker S, Gunamony S, Dietz P. Next-generation MRI scanner designed for ultra-high-resolution human brain imaging at 7 Tesla. Nat Methods 2023; 20:2048-2057. [PMID: 38012321 PMCID: PMC10703687 DOI: 10.1038/s41592-023-02068-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 23.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2022] [Accepted: 10/09/2023] [Indexed: 11/29/2023]
Abstract
To increase granularity in human neuroimaging science, we designed and built a next-generation 7 Tesla magnetic resonance imaging scanner to reach ultra-high resolution by implementing several advances in hardware. To improve spatial encoding and increase the image signal-to-noise ratio, we developed a head-only asymmetric gradient coil (200 mT m-1, 900 T m-1s-1) with an additional third layer of windings. We integrated a 128-channel receiver system with 64- and 96-channel receiver coil arrays to boost signal in the cerebral cortex while reducing g-factor noise to enable higher accelerations. A 16-channel transmit system reduced power deposition and improved image uniformity. The scanner routinely performs functional imaging studies at 0.35-0.45 mm isotropic spatial resolution to reveal cortical layer functional activity, achieves high angular resolution in diffusion imaging and reduces acquisition time for both functional and structural imaging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David A Feinberg
- Erwin Hahn 7T MRI Laboratory, Henry H. Wheeler Brain Imaging Center, Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, USA.
- Advanced MRI Technologies, Sebastopol, CA, USA.
| | - Alexander J S Beckett
- Erwin Hahn 7T MRI Laboratory, Henry H. Wheeler Brain Imaging Center, Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, USA
- Advanced MRI Technologies, Sebastopol, CA, USA
| | - An T Vu
- Radiology and Biomedical Imaging, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
- San Francisco Veteran Affairs Health Care System, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Jason Stockmann
- A. A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
- Harvard-MIT Health Sciences and Technology, MIT, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Laurentius Huber
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands
| | | | | | - Kawin Setsompop
- Radiological Sciences Laboratory, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Xiaozhi Cao
- Radiological Sciences Laboratory, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Suhyung Park
- Erwin Hahn 7T MRI Laboratory, Henry H. Wheeler Brain Imaging Center, Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, USA
- Department of Computer Engineering, Chonnam National University, Gwangju, Republic of Korea
- Department of ICT Convergence System Engineering, Chonnam National University, Gwangju, Republic of Korea
| | - Chunlei Liu
- Erwin Hahn 7T MRI Laboratory, Henry H. Wheeler Brain Imaging Center, Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, USA
| | - Lawrence L Wald
- A. A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
- Harvard-MIT Health Sciences and Technology, MIT, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Jonathan R Polimeni
- A. A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
- Harvard-MIT Health Sciences and Technology, MIT, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Azma Mareyam
- A. A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
- Harvard-MIT Health Sciences and Technology, MIT, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Bernhard Gruber
- A. A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
- Harvard-MIT Health Sciences and Technology, MIT, Cambridge, MA, USA
- BARNLabs, Muenzkirchen, Austria
| | | | - Congyu Liao
- Radiological Sciences Laboratory, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Essa Yacoub
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Mathias Davids
- A. A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
- Harvard-MIT Health Sciences and Technology, MIT, Cambridge, MA, USA
- Computer Assisted Clinical Medicine, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Heidelberg, Germany
| | - Paul Bell
- Siemens Medical Solutions, Malvern, PA, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | | - Shajan Gunamony
- Imaging Centre of Excellence, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
- MR CoilTech Limited, Glasgow, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Haarsma J, Deveci N, Corbin N, Callaghan MF, Kok P. Expectation Cues and False Percepts Generate Stimulus-Specific Activity in Distinct Layers of the Early Visual Cortex. J Neurosci 2023; 43:7946-7957. [PMID: 37739797 PMCID: PMC10669763 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0998-23.2023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/30/2023] [Revised: 09/10/2023] [Accepted: 09/13/2023] [Indexed: 09/24/2023] Open
Abstract
Perception has been proposed to result from the integration of feedforward sensory signals with internally generated feedback signals. Feedback signals are believed to play an important role in driving false percepts, that is, seeing things that are not actually there. Feedforward and feedback influences on perception can be studied using layer-specific fMRI, which we used here to interrogate neural activity underlying high-confidence false percepts while healthy human participants (N = 25, male and female) performed a perceptual orientation discrimination task. Auditory cues implicitly signaled the most likely upcoming orientation (referred to here as expectations). These expectations induced orientation-specific templates in the deep and superficial layers of V2, without affecting perception. In contrast, the orientation of falsely perceived stimuli with high confidence was reflected in the middle input layers of V2, suggesting a feedforward signal contributing to false percepts. The prevalence of high-confidence false percepts was related to everyday hallucination severity in a separate online sample (N = 100), suggesting a possible link with abnormal perceptual experiences. These results reveal a potential feedforward mechanism underlying false percepts, reflected by spontaneous stimulus-like activity in the input layers of the visual cortex, independent of top-down signals reflecting cued orientations.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT False percepts have been suggested to arise through excessive feedback signals. However, feedforward contributions to false percepts have remained largely understudied. Laminar fMRI has been shown to be useful in distinguishing feedforward from feedback activity as it allows the imaging of different cortical layers. In the present study we demonstrate that although cued orientations are encoded in the feedback layers of the visual cortex, the content of the false percepts are encoded in the feedforward layers and did not rely on these cued orientations. This shows that false percepts can in principle emerge from random feedforward signals in the visual cortex, with possible implications for disorders hallmarked by hallucinations like schizophrenia and Parkinson's disease.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joost Haarsma
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom
| | - Narin Deveci
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom
| | - Nadege Corbin
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom
- Centre de Résonance Magnétique des Systèmes Biologiques, Unité Mixte de Recherche 5536, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Université de Bordeaux, 33076 Bordeaux, France
| | - Martina F Callaghan
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom
| | - Peter Kok
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Cheng FL, Horikawa T, Majima K, Tanaka M, Abdelhack M, Aoki SC, Hirano J, Kamitani Y. Reconstructing visual illusory experiences from human brain activity. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2023; 9:eadj3906. [PMID: 37967184 PMCID: PMC10651116 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.adj3906] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2023] [Accepted: 10/13/2023] [Indexed: 11/17/2023]
Abstract
Visual illusions provide valuable insights into the brain's interpretation of the world given sensory inputs. However, the precise manner in which brain activity translates into illusory experiences remains largely unknown. Here, we leverage a brain decoding technique combined with deep neural network (DNN) representations to reconstruct illusory percepts as images from brain activity. The reconstruction model was trained on natural images to establish a link between brain activity and perceptual features and then tested on two types of illusions: illusory lines and neon color spreading. Reconstructions revealed lines and colors consistent with illusory experiences, which varied across the source visual cortical areas. This framework offers a way to materialize subjective experiences, shedding light on the brain's internal representations of the world.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fan L. Cheng
- Graduate School of Informatics, Kyoto University, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
- ATR Computational Neuroscience Laboratories, Soraku, Kyoto 619-0288, Japan
| | - Tomoyasu Horikawa
- ATR Computational Neuroscience Laboratories, Soraku, Kyoto 619-0288, Japan
| | - Kei Majima
- Graduate School of Informatics, Kyoto University, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
| | - Misato Tanaka
- Graduate School of Informatics, Kyoto University, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
| | - Mohamed Abdelhack
- Graduate School of Informatics, Kyoto University, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
| | - Shuntaro C. Aoki
- Graduate School of Informatics, Kyoto University, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
| | - Jin Hirano
- Graduate School of Informatics, Kyoto University, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
| | - Yukiyasu Kamitani
- Graduate School of Informatics, Kyoto University, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
- ATR Computational Neuroscience Laboratories, Soraku, Kyoto 619-0288, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Heij J, Raimondo L, Siero JCW, Dumoulin SO, van der Zwaag W, Knapen T. A selection and targeting framework of cortical locations for line-scanning fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 2023; 44:5471-5484. [PMID: 37608563 PMCID: PMC10543358 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.26459] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/19/2023] [Revised: 07/15/2023] [Accepted: 08/02/2023] [Indexed: 08/24/2023] Open
Abstract
Depth-resolved functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) is an emerging field growing in popularity given the potential of separating signals from different computational processes in cerebral cortex. Conventional acquisition schemes suffer from low spatial and temporal resolutions. Line-scanning methods allow depth-resolved fMRI by sacrificing spatial coverage to sample blood oxygenated level-dependent (BOLD) responses at ultra-high temporal and spatial resolution. For neuroscience applications, it is critical to be able to place the line accurately to (1) sample the right neural population and (2) target that neural population with tailored stimuli or tasks. To this end, we devised a multi-session framework where a target cortical location is selected based on anatomical and functional properties. The line is then positioned according to this information in a separate second session, and we tailor the experiment to focus on the target location. Anatomically, the precision of the line placement was confirmed by projecting a nominal representation of the acquired line back onto the surface. Functional estimates of neural selectivities in the line, as quantified by a visual population-receptive field model, resembled the target selectivities well for most subjects. This functional precision was quantified in detail by estimating the distance between the visual field location of the targeted vertex and the location in visual cortex (V1) that most closely resembled the line-scanning estimates; this distance was on average ~5.5 mm. Given the dimensions of the line, differences in acquisition, session, and stimulus design, this validates that line-scanning can be used to probe local neural sensitivities across sessions. In summary, we present an accurate framework for line-scanning MRI; we believe such a framework is required to harness the full potential of line-scanning and maximize its utility. Furthermore, this approach bridges canonical fMRI experiments with electrophysiological experiments, which in turn allows novel avenues for studying human physiology non-invasively.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jurjen Heij
- Spinoza Centre for NeuroimagingAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of Computational Cognitive Neuroscience and NeuroimagingNetherlands Institute for NeuroscienceAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of Experimental and Applied PsychologyVU UniversityAmsterdamNetherlands
| | - Luisa Raimondo
- Spinoza Centre for NeuroimagingAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of Computational Cognitive Neuroscience and NeuroimagingNetherlands Institute for NeuroscienceAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of Experimental and Applied PsychologyVU UniversityAmsterdamNetherlands
| | - Jeroen C. W. Siero
- Spinoza Centre for NeuroimagingAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of Computational Cognitive Neuroscience and NeuroimagingNetherlands Institute for NeuroscienceAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of RadiologyUniversity Medical Center UtrechtUtrechtthe Netherlands
| | - Serge O. Dumoulin
- Spinoza Centre for NeuroimagingAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of Computational Cognitive Neuroscience and NeuroimagingNetherlands Institute for NeuroscienceAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of Experimental and Applied PsychologyVU UniversityAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of Experimental PsychologyUtrecht UniversityUtrechtNetherlands
| | - Wietske van der Zwaag
- Spinoza Centre for NeuroimagingAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of Computational Cognitive Neuroscience and NeuroimagingNetherlands Institute for NeuroscienceAmsterdamNetherlands
| | - Tomas Knapen
- Spinoza Centre for NeuroimagingAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of Computational Cognitive Neuroscience and NeuroimagingNetherlands Institute for NeuroscienceAmsterdamNetherlands
- Department of Experimental and Applied PsychologyVU UniversityAmsterdamNetherlands
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Malekian V, Graedel NN, Hickling A, Aghaeifar A, Dymerska B, Corbin N, Josephs O, Maguire EA, Callaghan MF. Mitigating susceptibility-induced distortions in high-resolution 3DEPI fMRI at 7T. Neuroimage 2023; 279:120294. [PMID: 37517572 PMCID: PMC10951962 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120294] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/12/2023] [Revised: 07/08/2023] [Accepted: 07/22/2023] [Indexed: 08/01/2023] Open
Abstract
Geometric distortion is a major limiting factor for spatial specificity in high-resolution fMRI using EPI readouts and is exacerbated at higher field strengths due to increased B0 field inhomogeneity. Prominent correction schemes are based on B0 field-mapping or acquiring reverse phase-encoded (reversed-PE) data. However, to date, comparisons of these techniques in the context of fMRI have only been performed on 2DEPI data, either at lower field or lower resolution. In this study, we investigate distortion compensation in the context of sub-millimetre 3DEPI data at 7T. B0 field-mapping and reversed-PE distortion correction techniques were applied to both partial coverage BOLD-weighted and whole brain MT-weighted 3DEPI data with matched distortion. Qualitative assessment showed overall improvement in cortical alignment for both correction techniques in both 3DEPI fMRI and whole-brain MT-3DEPI datasets. The distortion-corrected MT-3DEPI images were quantitatively evaluated by comparing cortical alignment with an anatomical reference using dice coefficient (DC) and correlation ratio (CR) measures. These showed that B0 field-mapping and reversed-PE methods both improved correspondence between the MT-3DEPI and anatomical data, with more substantial improvements consistently obtained using the reversed-PE approach. Regional analyses demonstrated that the largest benefit of distortion correction, and in particular of the reversed-PE approach, occurred in frontal and temporal regions where susceptibility-induced distortions are known to be greatest, but had not led to complete signal dropout. In conclusion, distortion correction based on reversed-PE data has shown the greater capacity for achieving faithful alignment with anatomical data in the context of high-resolution fMRI at 7T using 3DEPI.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vahid Malekian
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, UK.
| | - Nadine N Graedel
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, UK
| | - Alice Hickling
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, UK
| | - Ali Aghaeifar
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, UK; MR Research Collaborations, Siemens Healthcare Limited, Frimley, UK
| | - Barbara Dymerska
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, UK
| | - Nadège Corbin
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, UK; Centre de Résonance Magnétique des Systèmes Biologiques, CNRS-University Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
| | - Oliver Josephs
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, UK
| | - Eleanor A Maguire
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, UK
| | - Martina F Callaghan
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, University College London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Martin A, Lane TJ, Hsu TY. DLPFC-PPC-cTBS effects on metacognitive awareness. Cortex 2023; 167:41-50. [PMID: 37523964 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2023.05.022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2022] [Revised: 02/12/2023] [Accepted: 05/16/2023] [Indexed: 08/02/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Neuroimaging and lesion studies suggested that the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices mediate visual metacognitive awareness. The causal evidence provided by non-invasive brain stimulation, however, is inconsistent. OBJECTIVE/HYPOTHESIS Here we revisit a major figure discrimination experiment adding a new Kanizsa figure task trying to resolve whether bilateral continuous theta-burst transcranial magnetic stimulation (cTBS) over these regions affects perceptual metacognition. Specifically, we tested whether subjective visibility ratings and/or metacognitive efficiency are lower when cTBS is applied to these two regions in comparison to an active control region. METHODS A within-subjects design including three sessions spaced by one-week intervals was implemented. In each session, every participant was administered bilateral cTBS to either prefrontal, control or parietal cortices. Two concurrent tasks were performed, a real and an illusory figure task, stabilising objective performance with use of an adaptive staircase procedure. RESULTS When performing the replicated task, cTBS was found insufficient to disrupt neither visibility ratings nor metacognitive efficiency. However, with use of Kanizsa style illusory figures, cTBS over the dorsolateral prefrontal, but not over the posterior parietal cortex, was observed to significantly diminish metacognitive efficiency. CONCLUSION(S) Real and illusory figure tasks demonstrated different cTBS effects. A possible explanation is the involvement of the prefrontal cortex in the creation of expectations, which is necessary for efficient metacognition. Failure to replicate previous findings for the real figure task, however, cannot be said to support, conclusively, the notion that these brain regions have a causal role in metacognitive awareness. This inconsistent finding may result from certain limitations of our study, thereby suggesting the need for yet further investigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Antonio Martin
- Graduate Institute of Mind, Brain and Consciousness (GIMBC), Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Timothy J Lane
- Graduate Institute of Mind, Brain and Consciousness (GIMBC), Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan; Brain and Consciousness Research Center (BCRC), TMU-Shuang Ho Hospital, New Taipei City, Taiwan
| | - Tzu-Yu Hsu
- Graduate Institute of Mind, Brain and Consciousness (GIMBC), Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan; Brain and Consciousness Research Center (BCRC), TMU-Shuang Ho Hospital, New Taipei City, Taiwan.
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Stange L, Ossandón JP, Röder B. Crossmodal visual predictions elicit spatially specific early visual cortex activity but later than real visual stimuli. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2023; 378:20220339. [PMID: 37545314 PMCID: PMC10404923 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2022.0339] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2022] [Accepted: 06/30/2023] [Indexed: 08/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Previous studies have indicated that crossmodal visual predictions are instrumental in controlling early visual cortex activity. The exact time course and spatial precision of such crossmodal top-down influences on the visual cortex have been unknown. In the present study, participants were exposed to audiovisual combinations comprising one of two sounds and a Gabor patch either in the top left or in the bottom right visual field. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded to these frequent crossmodal combinations (standards) as well as to trials in which the visual stimulus was omitted (omissions) or the visual and auditory stimuli were recombined (deviants). Standards and deviants elicited an ERP between 50 and 100 ms of opposite polarity known as the C1 effect commonly associated with retinotopic processing in early visual cortex. By contrast, a C1 effect was not observed in omission trials. Spatially specific omission and mismatch effects (deviants minus standards) started only later with a latency of 230 ms and 170 ms, respectively. These results suggest that crossmodal visual predictions control visual cortex activity in a spatially specific manner. However, visual predictions do not modulate visual cortex activity with the same timing as visual stimulation activates these areas but rather seem to involve distinct neural mechanisms. This article is part of the theme issue 'Decision and control processes in multisensory perception'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Liesa Stange
- Biological Psychology and Neuropsychology, Hamburg University, Von-Melle-Park 11, Hamburg 20148, Germany
| | - José P. Ossandón
- Biological Psychology and Neuropsychology, Hamburg University, Von-Melle-Park 11, Hamburg 20148, Germany
| | - Brigitte Röder
- Biological Psychology and Neuropsychology, Hamburg University, Von-Melle-Park 11, Hamburg 20148, Germany
- LV Prasad Eye Institute, Hyderabad 500 034, India
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Jia K, Goebel R, Kourtzi Z. Ultra-High Field Imaging of Human Visual Cognition. Annu Rev Vis Sci 2023; 9:479-500. [PMID: 37137282 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-vision-111022-123830] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/05/2023]
Abstract
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), the key methodology for mapping the functions of the human brain in a noninvasive manner, is limited by low temporal and spatial resolution. Recent advances in ultra-high field (UHF) fMRI provide a mesoscopic (i.e., submillimeter resolution) tool that allows us to probe laminar and columnar circuits, distinguish bottom-up versus top-down pathways, and map small subcortical areas. We review recent work demonstrating that UHF fMRI provides a robust methodology for imaging the brain across cortical depths and columns that provides insights into the brain's organization and functions at unprecedented spatial resolution, advancing our understanding of the fine-scale computations and interareal communication that support visual cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ke Jia
- Department of Neurobiology, Affiliated Mental Health Center & Hangzhou Seventh People's Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine, Hangzhou, China
- Liangzhu Laboratory, MOE Frontier Science Center for Brain Science and Brain-machine Integration, State Key Laboratory of Brain-machine Intelligence, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
- NHC and CAMS Key Laboratory of Medical Neurobiology, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom;
| | - Rainer Goebel
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Zoe Kourtzi
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom;
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Riley SN, Davies J. Vividness as the similarity between generated imagery and an internal model. Brain Cogn 2023; 169:105988. [PMID: 37150045 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2023.105988] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/13/2023] [Revised: 04/17/2023] [Accepted: 04/18/2023] [Indexed: 05/09/2023]
Abstract
Vividness in visual mental imagery has been relatively under-explored compared to imagery's representational format and neural mechanisms. In this paper, we take a deeper look at vividness and suggest that in re-framing it, we can potentially reconcile disparate findings regarding visual cortex activation during imagery. Unlike traditional views of vividness that define the concept in terms of perception, we frame vividness in terms of imagery's relation to an internal model; the closer the generated imagery is to this model, the more vivid it is. This view is considered alongside existing neuroscientific, psychological, and philosophical research, as well as directions for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sean N Riley
- Department of Cognitive Science, Carleton University, Canada
| | - Jim Davies
- Department of Cognitive Science, Carleton University, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Chu Q, Ma O, Hang Y, Tian X. Dual-stream cortical pathways mediate sensory prediction. Cereb Cortex 2023:7169133. [PMID: 37197767 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhad168] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/20/2022] [Revised: 04/24/2023] [Accepted: 04/26/2023] [Indexed: 05/19/2023] Open
Abstract
Predictions are constantly generated from diverse sources to optimize cognitive functions in the ever-changing environment. However, the neural origin and generation process of top-down induced prediction remain elusive. We hypothesized that motor-based and memory-based predictions are mediated by distinct descending networks from motor and memory systems to the sensory cortices. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and a dual imagery paradigm, we found that motor and memory upstream systems activated the auditory cortex in a content-specific manner. Moreover, the inferior and posterior parts of the parietal lobe differentially relayed predictive signals in motor-to-sensory and memory-to-sensory networks. Dynamic causal modeling of directed connectivity revealed selective enabling and modulation of connections that mediate top-down sensory prediction and ground the distinctive neurocognitive basis of predictive processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Qian Chu
- Shanghai Frontiers Science Center of Artificial Intelligence and Deep Learning, Division of Arts and Sciences, New York University Shanghai, Shanghai 200126, China
- NYU-ECNU Institute of Brain and Cognitive Science at NYU Shanghai, Shanghai 200062, China
- Max Planck-University of Toronto Centre for Neural Science and Technology, Toronto, ON M5S 2E4, Canada
| | - Ou Ma
- NYU-ECNU Institute of Brain and Cognitive Science at NYU Shanghai, Shanghai 200062, China
- Shanghai Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics (Ministry of Education), School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai 200062, China
| | - Yuqi Hang
- NYU-ECNU Institute of Brain and Cognitive Science at NYU Shanghai, Shanghai 200062, China
- Department of Administration, Leadership, and Technology, Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human Development, New York University, New York, NY 10003, United States
| | - Xing Tian
- Shanghai Frontiers Science Center of Artificial Intelligence and Deep Learning, Division of Arts and Sciences, New York University Shanghai, Shanghai 200126, China
- NYU-ECNU Institute of Brain and Cognitive Science at NYU Shanghai, Shanghai 200062, China
- Shanghai Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics (Ministry of Education), School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai 200062, China
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Olman CA. What multiplexing means for the interpretation of functional MRI data. Front Hum Neurosci 2023; 17:1134811. [PMID: 37091812 PMCID: PMC10117671 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1134811] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/30/2022] [Accepted: 03/20/2023] [Indexed: 04/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Despite technology advances that have enabled routine acquisition of functional MRI data with sub-millimeter resolution, the inferences that cognitive neuroscientists must make to link fMRI data to behavior are complicated. Thus, a single dataset subjected to different analyses can be interpreted in different ways. This article presents two optical analogies that can be useful for framing fMRI analyses in a way that allows for multiple interpretations of fMRI data to be valid simultaneously without undermining each other. The first is reflection: when an object is reflected in a mirrored surface, it appears as if the reflected object is sharing space with the mirrored object, but of course it is not. This analogy can be a good guide for interpreting the fMRI signal, since even at sub-millimeter resolutions the signal is determined by a mixture of local and long-range neural computations. The second is refraction. If we view an object through a multi-faceted prism or gemstone, our view will change-sometimes dramatically-depending on our viewing angle. In the same way, interpretation of fMRI data (inference of underlying neuronal activity) can and should be different depending on the analysis approach. Rather than representing a weakness of the methodology, or the superiority of one approach over the other (for example, simple regression analysis versus multi-voxel pattern analysis), this is an expected consequence of how information is multiplexed in the neural networks of the brain: multiple streams of information are simultaneously present in each location. The fact that any one analysis typically shows only one view of the data also puts some parentheses around fMRI practitioners' constant search for ground truth against which to compare their data. By holding our interpretations lightly and understanding that many interpretations of the data can all be true at the same time, we do a better job of preparing ourselves to appreciate, and eventually understand, the complexity of the brain and the behavior it produces.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cheryl A. Olman
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Knudsen L, Bailey CJ, Blicher JU, Yang Y, Zhang P, Lund TE. Improved sensitivity and microvascular weighting of 3T laminar fMRI with GE-BOLD using NORDIC and phase regression. Neuroimage 2023; 271:120011. [PMID: 36914107 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/09/2022] [Revised: 03/03/2023] [Accepted: 03/09/2023] [Indexed: 03/15/2023] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Functional MRI with spatial resolution in the submillimeter domain enables measurements of activation across cortical layers in humans. This is valuable as different types of cortical computations, e.g., feedforward versus feedback related activity, take place in different cortical layers. Laminar fMRI studies have almost exclusively employed 7T scanners to overcome the reduced signal stability associated with small voxels. However, such systems are relatively rare and only a subset of those are clinically approved. In the present study, we examined if the feasibility of laminar fMRI at 3T could be improved by use of NORDIC denoising and phase regression. METHODS 5 healthy subjects were scanned on a Siemens MAGNETOM Prisma 3T scanner. To assess across-session reliability, each subject was scanned in 3-8 sessions on 3-4 consecutive days. A 3D gradient echo EPI (GE-EPI) sequence was used for BOLD acquisitions (voxel size 0.82 mm isotopic, TR = 2.2 s) using a block design finger tapping paradigm. NORDIC denoising was applied to the magnitude and phase time series to overcome limitations in temporal signal-to-noise ratio (tSNR) and the denoised phase time series were subsequently used to correct for large vein contamination through phase regression. RESULTS AND CONCLUSION NORDIC denoising resulted in tSNR values comparable to or higher than commonly observed at 7T. Layer-dependent activation profiles could thus be extracted robustly, within and across sessions, from regions of interest located in the hand knob of the primary motor cortex (M1). Phase regression led to substantially reduced superficial bias in obtained layer profiles, although residual macrovascular contribution remained. We believe the present results support an improved feasibility of laminar fMRI at 3T.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lasse Knudsen
- Department of Clinical Medicine, Center of Functionally Integrative Neuroscience (CFIN), Aarhus University, Universitetsbyen 3, Aarhus C 8000, Denmark; Sino-Danish Center for Education and Research (SDC), University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, 380 Huaibeizhuang, Huairou District, Beijing 101400, PR China.
| | - Christopher J Bailey
- Department of Clinical Medicine, Center of Functionally Integrative Neuroscience (CFIN), Aarhus University, Universitetsbyen 3, Aarhus C 8000, Denmark; Sino-Danish Center for Education and Research (SDC), University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, 380 Huaibeizhuang, Huairou District, Beijing 101400, PR China
| | - Jakob U Blicher
- Department of Clinical Medicine, Center of Functionally Integrative Neuroscience (CFIN), Aarhus University, Universitetsbyen 3, Aarhus C 8000, Denmark; Department of Neurology, Aalborg University Hospital, Aalborg, Denmark
| | - Yan Yang
- Sino-Danish Center for Education and Research (SDC), University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, 380 Huaibeizhuang, Huairou District, Beijing 101400, PR China; Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, PR China
| | - Peng Zhang
- Sino-Danish Center for Education and Research (SDC), University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, 380 Huaibeizhuang, Huairou District, Beijing 101400, PR China; Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, PR China
| | - Torben E Lund
- Department of Clinical Medicine, Center of Functionally Integrative Neuroscience (CFIN), Aarhus University, Universitetsbyen 3, Aarhus C 8000, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Scheeringa R, Bonnefond M, van Mourik T, Jensen O, Norris DG, Koopmans PJ. Relating neural oscillations to laminar fMRI connectivity in visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 2023; 33:1537-1549. [PMID: 35512361 PMCID: PMC9977363 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhac154] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/17/2021] [Revised: 03/30/2022] [Accepted: 03/31/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Laminar functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) holds the potential to study connectivity at the laminar level in humans. Here we analyze simultaneously recorded electroencephalography (EEG) and high-resolution fMRI data to investigate how EEG power modulations, induced by a task with an attentional component, relate to changes in fMRI laminar connectivity between and within brain regions in visual cortex. Our results indicate that our task-induced decrease in beta power relates to an increase in deep-to-deep layer coupling between regions and to an increase in deep/middle-to-superficial layer connectivity within brain regions. The attention-related alpha power decrease predominantly relates to reduced connectivity between deep and superficial layers within brain regions, since, unlike beta power, alpha power was found to be positively correlated to connectivity. We observed no strong relation between laminar connectivity and gamma band oscillations. These results indicate that especially beta band, and to a lesser extent, alpha band oscillations relate to laminar-specific fMRI connectivity. The differential effects for alpha and beta bands indicate that they relate to different feedback-related neural processes that are differentially expressed in intra-region laminar fMRI-based connectivity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- René Scheeringa
- Erwin L. Hahn Institute for Magnetic Resonance Imaging, UNESCO-Weltkulturerbe Zollverein, University of Duisburg-Essen, Kokereiallee 7, 45141 Essen, Germany.,High-Field and Hybrid MR Imaging, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Hufelandstr. 55, 45147 Essen, Germany.,Lyon Neuroscience Research Center; CRNL, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, University of Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Bâtiment 462 - Neurocampus, 95 Bd Pinel, 69500 Bron, France.,Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Trigon 204, P.O. Box 9101, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Mathilde Bonnefond
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center; CRNL, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, University of Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Bâtiment 462 - Neurocampus, 95 Bd Pinel, 69500 Bron, France
| | - Tim van Mourik
- Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Trigon 204, P.O. Box 9101, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Ole Jensen
- School of Psychology, Centre for Human Brain Health, University of Birmingham, Hills Building, Birmingham B15 2TT, United Kingdom
| | - David G Norris
- Erwin L. Hahn Institute for Magnetic Resonance Imaging, UNESCO-Weltkulturerbe Zollverein, University of Duisburg-Essen, Kokereiallee 7, 45141 Essen, Germany.,Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Trigon 204, P.O. Box 9101, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Peter J Koopmans
- Erwin L. Hahn Institute for Magnetic Resonance Imaging, UNESCO-Weltkulturerbe Zollverein, University of Duisburg-Essen, Kokereiallee 7, 45141 Essen, Germany.,High-Field and Hybrid MR Imaging, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Hufelandstr. 55, 45147 Essen, Germany.,Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Trigon 204, P.O. Box 9101, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands.,Department of Radiation Oncology, Radboud University Medical Center, Geert Grooteplein Zuid 10, 6525 GA, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Faes LK, De Martino F, Huber L(R. Cerebral blood volume sensitive layer-fMRI in the human auditory cortex at 7T: Challenges and capabilities. PLoS One 2023; 18:e0280855. [PMID: 36758009 PMCID: PMC9910709 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0280855] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2022] [Accepted: 01/09/2023] [Indexed: 02/10/2023] Open
Abstract
The development of ultra high field fMRI signal readout strategies and contrasts has led to the possibility of imaging the human brain in vivo and non-invasively at increasingly higher spatial resolutions of cortical layers and columns. One emergent layer-fMRI acquisition method with increasing popularity is the cerebral blood volume sensitive sequence named vascular space occupancy (VASO). This approach has been shown to be mostly sensitive to locally-specific changes of laminar microvasculature, without unwanted biases of trans-laminar draining veins. Until now, however, VASO has not been applied in the technically challenging cortical area of the auditory cortex. Here, we describe the main challenges we encountered when developing a VASO protocol for auditory neuroscientific applications and the solutions we have adopted. With the resulting protocol, we present preliminary results of laminar responses to sounds and as a proof of concept for future investigations, we map the topographic representation of frequency preference (tonotopy) in the auditory cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lonike K. Faes
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
- * E-mail:
| | - Federico De Martino
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
- Department of Radiology, Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota, United States of America
| | - Laurentius (Renzo) Huber
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
Ye Z, Ding J, Tu Y, Zhang Q, Chen S, Yu H, Sun Q, Hua T. Suppression of top-down influence decreases both behavioral and V1 neuronal response sensitivity to stimulus orientations in cats. Front Behav Neurosci 2023; 17:1061980. [PMID: 36844652 PMCID: PMC9944033 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1061980] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2022] [Accepted: 01/24/2023] [Indexed: 02/11/2023] Open
Abstract
How top-down influence affects behavioral detection of visual signals and neuronal response sensitivity in the primary visual cortex (V1) remains poorly understood. This study examined both behavioral performance in stimulus orientation identification and neuronal response sensitivity to stimulus orientations in the V1 of cat before and after top-down influence of area 7 (A7) was modulated by non-invasive transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Our results showed that cathode (c) but not sham (s) tDCS in A7 significantly increased the behavioral threshold in identifying stimulus orientation difference, which effect recovered after the tDCS effect vanished. Consistently, c-tDCS but not s-tDCS in A7 significantly decreased the response selectivity bias of V1 neurons for stimulus orientations, which effect could recover after withdrawal of the tDCS effect. Further analysis showed that c-tDCS induced reduction of V1 neurons in response selectivity was not resulted from alterations of neuronal preferred orientation, nor of spontaneous activity. Instead, c-tDCS in A7 significantly lowered the visually-evoked response, especially the maximum response of V1 neurons, which caused a decrease in response selectivity and signal-to-noise ratio. By contrast, s-tDCS exerted no significant effect on the responses of V1 neurons. These results indicate that top-down influence of A7 may enhance behavioral identification of stimulus orientations by increasing neuronal visually-evoked response and response selectivity in the V1.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zheng Ye
- College of Life sciences, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, Anhui, China
| | - Jian Ding
- College of Life sciences, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, Anhui, China,School of Basic Medical, Wannan Medical College, Wuhu, Anhui, China
| | - Yanni Tu
- College of Life sciences, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, Anhui, China
| | - Qiuyu Zhang
- College of Life sciences, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, Anhui, China
| | - Shunshun Chen
- College of Life sciences, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, Anhui, China
| | - Hao Yu
- College of Life sciences, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, Anhui, China
| | - Qingyan Sun
- College of Life sciences, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, Anhui, China
| | - Tianmiao Hua
- College of Life sciences, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, Anhui, China,*Correspondence: Tianmiao Hua,
| |
Collapse
|
50
|
Lankinen K, Ahlfors SP, Mamashli F, Blazejewska AI, Raij T, Turpin T, Polimeni JR, Ahveninen J. Cortical depth profiles of auditory and visual 7 T functional MRI responses in human superior temporal areas. Hum Brain Mapp 2023; 44:362-372. [PMID: 35980015 PMCID: PMC9842898 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.26046] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2022] [Revised: 07/06/2022] [Accepted: 07/16/2022] [Indexed: 02/02/2023] Open
Abstract
Invasive neurophysiological studies in nonhuman primates have shown different laminar activation profiles to auditory vs. visual stimuli in auditory cortices and adjacent polymodal areas. Means to examine the underlying feedforward vs. feedback type influences noninvasively have been limited in humans. Here, using 1-mm isotropic resolution 3D echo-planar imaging at 7 T, we studied the intracortical depth profiles of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) signals to brief auditory (noise bursts) and visual (checkerboard) stimuli. BOLD percent-signal-changes were estimated at 11 equally spaced intracortical depths, within regions-of-interest encompassing auditory (Heschl's gyrus, Heschl's sulcus, planum temporale, and posterior superior temporal gyrus) and polymodal (middle and posterior superior temporal sulcus) areas. Effects of differing BOLD signal strengths for auditory and visual stimuli were controlled via normalization and statistical modeling. The BOLD depth profile shapes, modeled with quadratic regression, were significantly different for auditory vs. visual stimuli in auditory cortices, but not in polymodal areas. The different depth profiles could reflect sensory-specific feedforward versus cross-sensory feedback influences, previously shown in laminar recordings in nonhuman primates. The results suggest that intracortical BOLD profiles can help distinguish between feedforward and feedback type influences in the human brain. Further experimental studies are still needed to clarify how underlying signal strength influences BOLD depth profiles under different stimulus conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kaisu Lankinen
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of RadiologyMassachusetts General HospitalCharlestownMassachusettsUSA
- Department of RadiologyHarvard Medical SchoolBostonMassachusettsUSA
| | - Seppo P. Ahlfors
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of RadiologyMassachusetts General HospitalCharlestownMassachusettsUSA
- Department of RadiologyHarvard Medical SchoolBostonMassachusettsUSA
| | - Fahimeh Mamashli
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of RadiologyMassachusetts General HospitalCharlestownMassachusettsUSA
- Department of RadiologyHarvard Medical SchoolBostonMassachusettsUSA
| | - Anna I. Blazejewska
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of RadiologyMassachusetts General HospitalCharlestownMassachusettsUSA
- Department of RadiologyHarvard Medical SchoolBostonMassachusettsUSA
| | - Tommi Raij
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of RadiologyMassachusetts General HospitalCharlestownMassachusettsUSA
- Department of RadiologyHarvard Medical SchoolBostonMassachusettsUSA
| | - Tori Turpin
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of RadiologyMassachusetts General HospitalCharlestownMassachusettsUSA
| | - Jonathan R. Polimeni
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of RadiologyMassachusetts General HospitalCharlestownMassachusettsUSA
- Department of RadiologyHarvard Medical SchoolBostonMassachusettsUSA
- Division of Health Sciences and TechnologyMassachusetts Institute of TechnologyCambridgeMassachusettsUSA
| | - Jyrki Ahveninen
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of RadiologyMassachusetts General HospitalCharlestownMassachusettsUSA
- Department of RadiologyHarvard Medical SchoolBostonMassachusettsUSA
| |
Collapse
|