1
|
Patel J, Boardman M, Files B, Gregory F, Lamb S, Sarkadi S, Tešić M, Yeung N. Give us a hand, mate! A holistic review of research on human-machine teaming. BMJ Mil Health 2024:e002737. [PMID: 39719381 DOI: 10.1136/military-2024-002737] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/01/2024] [Accepted: 10/17/2024] [Indexed: 12/26/2024]
Abstract
Defence has a significant interest in the use of artificial intelligence (AI)-based technologies to address some of the challenges it faces. At the core of future military advantage will be the effective integration of humans and AI into human-machine teams (HMT) that leverages the capabilities of people and technologies to outperform adversaries. Realising the full potential of these technologies will depend on understanding the relative strengths of humans and machines, and how we design effective integration to optimise performance and resilience across all use cases and environments.Since the first robot appeared on the assembly line, machines have effectively augmented human capability and performance; however, they fall short of being a team member-someone you can ask to give you a hand! Working in teams involves collaboration, adaptive and dynamic interactions between team members to achieve a common goal. Currently, human-machine partnership is typically one of humans and machines working alongside each other, with each conducting discrete functions within predicable process and environments. However, with recent advances in neuroscience and AI, we can now envisage the possibility of HMT, not just in physical applications, but also complex cognitive tasks.This paper provides a holistic review of the research conducted in the field of HMT from experts working in this area. It summarises completed and ongoing studies and research in the UK and USA by a broad group of researchers. This work was presented in the HMT thematic session at the Sixth International Congress on Soldiers' Physical Performance (ICSPP23 London).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - B Files
- US Army Research Laboratory, Aberdeen Proving Ground, Maryland, USA
| | - F Gregory
- US Army Research Laboratory, Aberdeen Proving Ground, Maryland, USA
| | | | | | - M Tešić
- Birkbeck University of London, London, UK
- Leverhulme Centre for the Future of Intelligence, Cambridge, UK
| | - N Yeung
- University of Oxford Social Sciences Division, Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Anlló H, Salamander G, Raihani N, Palminteri S, Hertz U. Experience and advice consequences shape information sharing strategies. COMMUNICATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 2024; 2:123. [PMID: 39702539 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00175-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2024] [Accepted: 12/05/2024] [Indexed: 12/21/2024]
Abstract
Individuals often rely on the advice of more experienced peers to minimise uncertainty and increase success likelihood. In most domains where knowledge is acquired through experience, advisers are themselves continuously learning. Here we examine the way advising behaviour changes throughout the learning process, and the way individual traits and costs and benefits of giving advice shape this behaviour. We ran a series of experiments implementing a decision task within a reinforcement learning framework, where participants could decide to share their choices as advice to others. Participants were overall likely to share their choices as advice, even on the first trial before learning. Tendency to share advice and advice quality increased as advisers learned about the value of choices, and moved from exploratory to exploitative behaviour. The introduction of consequences to advising resulted in a shift of the overall tendency to give advice, lowering it when advising implicated monetary loss, and increasing it when advising held reputational value. Individual differences in social anxiety levels were associated with lower tendency to share exploratory decisions. Our results show that advisers tend to share choices that are backed by their own experience, but that this relationship can be altered by advice-consequences and individual traits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hernán Anlló
- Département d'études cognitives, École normale Supérieure-Université Paris Sciences et Lettres, Paris, France.
| | - Gil Salamander
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
| | - Nichola Raihani
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College of London, London, UK
- School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Stefano Palminteri
- Département d'études cognitives, École normale Supérieure-Université Paris Sciences et Lettres, Paris, France
| | - Uri Hertz
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel.
- The Institute of Information Processing and Decision Making (IIPDM), University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Shahabifar S, Yazdanpanah A, Vahabie AH. The effect of an embodied intervention on responsibility: put a load on one's shoulder. Cogn Process 2024; 25:613-620. [PMID: 39101960 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01207-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/26/2024] [Accepted: 07/23/2024] [Indexed: 08/06/2024]
Abstract
Responsibility is an essential part of our social life. Although responsibility is an abstract concept, it can be represented with concrete ideas through conceptual metaphor. Expressions like "carry a lot of responsibility," "shoulder the responsibility" shows that responsibility can be understood as a load on shoulder that one has to carry. Accordingly, this study tests the question that does putting a burden on one's shoulder makes him/her more responsible or not. In order to investigate this, on each trial, we asked participants to decide between risky situations that vary in magnitude, probability of win/lose, and the ambiguity level in two conditions: "self" and 'group." Each subject wears a vest with a load on each shoulder in half of the trials. As expected, Most of participants choose to defer on the group trials more than on the self-trials. This difference between numbers of deferring in group and self conditions is called responsibility aversion. Results indicate that responsibility aversion scores are lower (responsibility-taking was greater) in the state of wearing the vest than in the form of not wearing the vest significantly. We provided evidence that the abstract concept of responsibility is linked to bodily experiences of feeling load on the shoulder consistent with an embodied cognition theory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sara Shahabifar
- Department of Cognitive Psychology, Institute for Cognitive Science Studies, Tehran, Iran
| | - Aryan Yazdanpanah
- Cognitive Systems Laboratory, Control and Intelligent Processing Center of Excellence (CIPCE), School of Electrical and Computer Engineering, College of Engineering, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran
| | - Abdol-Hossein Vahabie
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, Faculty of Psychology and Education, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran.
- School of Cognitive Sciences, Institute for Research in Fundamental Sciences (IPM), Tehran, Iran.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Coucke N, Heinrich MK, Dorigo M, Cleeremans A. Action-based confidence sharing and collective decision making. iScience 2024; 27:111006. [PMID: 39429786 PMCID: PMC11490717 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2024.111006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/18/2024] [Revised: 07/14/2024] [Accepted: 09/17/2024] [Indexed: 10/22/2024] Open
Abstract
Joint action research explores how multiple humans can coordinate their movements to achieve common goals. When there is uncertainty about the joint goal, individuals need to integrate their perceptual information of the environment to collaboratively determine their new goal. To ensure that a group reaches a consensus about the goal, collective decision making among the individuals is required. Collective decision making can be facilitated by nonverbal expressions of opinions and associated confidence levels. Here, we show that confidence sharing in groups of 2, 3, and 4 individuals can be studied using their trajectories when jointly moving toward one of several options. We found that both opinions and confidence levels can be distinguished in individual movement trajectories, and found that movement features can predict an individual's influence. Our results suggest that movement trajectories are a valid way to study confidence sharing in human collective decision making.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicolas Coucke
- Center for Research in Cognition and Neurosciences, Université libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
- IRIDIA, Université libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
- Moral and Social Brain Lab, Department of Experimental Psychology, Universiteit Gent, Ghent, Belgium
| | | | - Marco Dorigo
- IRIDIA, Université libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Axel Cleeremans
- Center for Research in Cognition and Neurosciences, Université libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Burton JW, Lopez-Lopez E, Hechtlinger S, Rahwan Z, Aeschbach S, Bakker MA, Becker JA, Berditchevskaia A, Berger J, Brinkmann L, Flek L, Herzog SM, Huang S, Kapoor S, Narayanan A, Nussberger AM, Yasseri T, Nickl P, Almaatouq A, Hahn U, Kurvers RHJM, Leavy S, Rahwan I, Siddarth D, Siu A, Woolley AW, Wulff DU, Hertwig R. How large language models can reshape collective intelligence. Nat Hum Behav 2024; 8:1643-1655. [PMID: 39304760 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-024-01959-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/06/2023] [Accepted: 07/17/2024] [Indexed: 09/22/2024]
Abstract
Collective intelligence underpins the success of groups, organizations, markets and societies. Through distributed cognition and coordination, collectives can achieve outcomes that exceed the capabilities of individuals-even experts-resulting in improved accuracy and novel capabilities. Often, collective intelligence is supported by information technology, such as online prediction markets that elicit the 'wisdom of crowds', online forums that structure collective deliberation or digital platforms that crowdsource knowledge from the public. Large language models, however, are transforming how information is aggregated, accessed and transmitted online. Here we focus on the unique opportunities and challenges this transformation poses for collective intelligence. We bring together interdisciplinary perspectives from industry and academia to identify potential benefits, risks, policy-relevant considerations and open research questions, culminating in a call for a closer examination of how large language models affect humans' ability to collectively tackle complex problems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jason W Burton
- Department of Digitalization, Copenhagen Business School, Frederiksberg, Denmark.
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany.
| | - Ezequiel Lopez-Lopez
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | - Shahar Hechtlinger
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Zoe Rahwan
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | - Samuel Aeschbach
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Center for Cognitive and Decision Sciences, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
| | | | - Joshua A Becker
- UCL School of Management, University College London, London, UK
| | | | - Julian Berger
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Levin Brinkmann
- Center for Humans and Machines, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | - Lucie Flek
- Bonn-Aachen International Center for Information Technology, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany
- Lamarr Institute for Machine Learning and Artificial Intelligence, Bonn, Germany
| | - Stefan M Herzog
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | - Saffron Huang
- Collective Intelligence Project, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Sayash Kapoor
- Center for Information Technology Policy, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA
- Department of Computer Science, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA
| | - Arvind Narayanan
- Center for Information Technology Policy, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA
- Department of Computer Science, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA
| | - Anne-Marie Nussberger
- Center for Humans and Machines, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | - Taha Yasseri
- School of Sociology, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
- Geary Institute for Public Policy, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
| | - Pietro Nickl
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Abdullah Almaatouq
- Sloan School of Management, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Ulrike Hahn
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
| | - Ralf H J M Kurvers
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Science of Intelligence Excellence Cluster, Technical University Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Susan Leavy
- School of Information and Communication, Insight SFI Research Centre for Data Analytics, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
| | - Iyad Rahwan
- Center for Humans and Machines, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | - Divya Siddarth
- Collective Intelligence Project, San Francisco, CA, USA
- Oxford Internet Institute, Oxford University, Oxford, UK
| | - Alice Siu
- Deliberative Democracy Lab, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Anita W Woolley
- Tepper School of Business, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - Dirk U Wulff
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Center for Cognitive and Decision Sciences, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
| | - Ralph Hertwig
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Deroy O, Longin L, Bahrami B. Co-perceiving: Bringing the social into perception. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2024; 15:e1681. [PMID: 38706396 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1681] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/18/2022] [Revised: 04/15/2024] [Accepted: 04/16/2024] [Indexed: 05/07/2024]
Abstract
Humans and other animals possess the remarkable ability to effectively navigate a shared perceptual environment by discerning which objects and spaces are perceived by others and which remain private to themselves. Traditionally, this capacity has been encapsulated under the umbrella of joint attention or joint action. In this comprehensive review, we advocate for a broader and more mechanistic understanding of this phenomenon, termed co-perception. Co-perception encompasses the sensitivity to the perceptual engagement of others and the capability to differentiate between objects perceived privately and those perceived commonly with others. It represents a distinct concept from mere simultaneous individual perception. Moreover, discerning between private and common objects doesn't necessitate intricate mind-reading abilities or mutual coordination. The act of perceiving objects as either private or common provides a comprehensive account for social scenarios where individuals simply share the same context or may even engage in competition. This conceptual framework encourages a re-examination of classical paradigms that demonstrate social influences on perception. Furthermore, it suggests that the impacts of shared experiences extend beyond affective responses, also influencing perceptual processes. This article is categorized under: Psychology > Attention Philosophy > Foundations of Cognitive Science Philosophy > Psychological Capacities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ophelia Deroy
- Faculty of Philosophy, Philosophy of Science and the Study of Religion, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany
- Munich Centre for Neurosciences-Brain & Mind, Munich, Germany
- Institute of Philosophy, School of Advanced Study, University of London, London, UK
| | - Louis Longin
- Faculty of Philosophy, Philosophy of Science and the Study of Religion, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany
| | - Bahador Bahrami
- Crowd Cognition Group, Faculty of General Psychology and Education, Ludwig Maxilian University, Munich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Meimandi M, Taghizadeh G, Kheirollahi G, Haj Ghani J, HojabriFard F, von Rosen P, Azad A. A Delphi Panel of People With Parkinson's Disease Regarding Responsibility: Toward a Preliminary Taxonomy. Am J Occup Ther 2024; 78:7803205130. [PMID: 38634671 DOI: 10.5014/ajot.2024.050463] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/19/2024] Open
Abstract
IMPORTANCE A sense of agency is associated with complex occupation-related responsibilities. A taxonomy can guide clinicians in enhancing responsibility in patients with Parkinson's disease (PwPD). OBJECTIVE To (1) discover levels of responsibility in occupations for PwPD and (2) propose a taxonomy for occupations. DESIGN A two-round Delphi study with PwPD and a one-round Delphi study with international experts. SETTING Electronic survey. PARTICIPANTS PwPD (N = 75) and international experts (N = 8). OUTCOMES AND MEASURES PwPD expressed their levels of an inherent sense of responsibility for each occupation (1 = very low responsibility, 5 = very high responsibility). International experts rated their level of agreement (5 = strongly agree, 1 = strongly disagree) with each dimension of the taxonomy. A consensus was determined to have been reached if the interquartile range was ≤1 and 70% agreement in two adjacent categories was achieved. RESULTS Thirty-three occupation categories were deemed as having very high to moderate responsibility for PwPD. Consequences of actions and the presence of others made up the two-dimensional responsibility taxonomy. Occupations have more challenging responsibility characteristics when they are performed with free choice, a level of high physical effort, alone, and with moral consequences. CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE This study yielded the first consensus among PwPD regarding responsibility in occupations as well as a classification system for charting the complexity of responsibility in occupations. The occupation list we have created can be beneficial to health care professionals when providing interventions or conducting outcome assessments. Plain-Language Summary: When planning interventions for patients with Parkinson's disease, it can be helpful for clinicians to be aware of patients' perspectives regarding their sense of responsibility to perform occupations. The use of a systematic sequence of challenging occupations with responsibility attributes ranging from less complex to more complex can help enhance patient occupational participation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mahsa Meimandi
- Mahsa Meimandi, PhD, is Clinician, Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Ghorban Taghizadeh
- Ghorban Taghizadeh, PhD, is Associate Professor, Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Golnoush Kheirollahi
- Golnoush Kheirollahi, BSc, is Clinician, Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Jafar Haj Ghani
- Jafar Haj Ghani, is MSc student, Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Fatemeh HojabriFard
- Fatemeh HojabriFard, BSc, is Clinician, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Philip von Rosen
- Philip von Rosen, PhD, is Associate Professor, Division of Physiotherapy, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences, and Society, Karolinska Institutet, Huddinge, Sweden
| | - Akram Azad
- Akram Azad, PhD, is Associate Professor, Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran;
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Stinson C, Kagan I, Pooresmaeili A. The contribution of sensory information asymmetry and bias of attribution to egocentric tendencies in effort comparison tasks. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1304372. [PMID: 38638515 PMCID: PMC11025643 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1304372] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/09/2023] [Accepted: 03/25/2024] [Indexed: 04/20/2024] Open
Abstract
When comparing themselves with others, people often evaluate their own behaviors more favorably. This egocentric tendency is often categorized as a bias of attribution, with favorable self-evaluation resulting from differing explanations of one's own behavior and that of others. However, studies on information availability in social contexts offer an alternative explanation, ascribing egocentric biases to the inherent informational asymmetries between performing an action and merely observing it. Since biases of attribution and availability often co-exist and interact with each other, it is not known whether they are both necessary for the egocentric biases to emerge. In this study, we used a design that allowed us to directly compare the contribution of these two distinct sources of bias to judgements about the difficulty of an effortful task. Participants exhibited no attribution bias as judgements made for themselves did not differ from those made for others. Importantly, however, participants perceived the tasks they actively performed to be harder than the tasks they observed, and this bias was magnified as the overall task difficulty increased. These findings suggest that information asymmetries inherent to the difference between actively performing a task and observing it can drive egocentric biases in effort evaluations on their own and without a contribution from biases of attribution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caedyn Stinson
- Perception and Cognition Lab, European Neuroscience Institute Göttingen—A Joint Initiative of the University Medical Center Göttingen and the Max Planck Society, Göttingen, Germany
- Biological Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience Berlin, Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | - Igor Kagan
- Decision and Awareness Group, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Göttingen, Germany
- Leibniz Science Campus Primate Cognition, Göttingen, Germany
| | - Arezoo Pooresmaeili
- Perception and Cognition Lab, European Neuroscience Institute Göttingen—A Joint Initiative of the University Medical Center Göttingen and the Max Planck Society, Göttingen, Germany
- Leibniz Science Campus Primate Cognition, Göttingen, Germany
- School of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Shirado H, Kasahara S, Christakis NA. Emergence and collapse of reciprocity in semiautomatic driving coordination experiments with humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2023; 120:e2307804120. [PMID: 38079552 PMCID: PMC10743379 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2307804120] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2023] [Accepted: 10/10/2023] [Indexed: 12/18/2023] Open
Abstract
Forms of both simple and complex machine intelligence are increasingly acting within human groups in order to affect collective outcomes. Considering the nature of collective action problems, however, such involvement could paradoxically and unintentionally suppress existing beneficial social norms in humans, such as those involving cooperation. Here, we test theoretical predictions about such an effect using a unique cyber-physical lab experiment where online participants (N = 300 in 150 dyads) drive robotic vehicles remotely in a coordination game. We show that autobraking assistance increases human altruism, such as giving way to others, and that communication helps people to make mutual concessions. On the other hand, autosteering assistance completely inhibits the emergence of reciprocity between people in favor of self-interest maximization. The negative social repercussions persist even after the assistance system is deactivated. Furthermore, adding communication capabilities does not relieve this inhibition of reciprocity because people rarely communicate in the presence of autosteering assistance. Our findings suggest that active safety assistance (a form of simple AI support) can alter the dynamics of social coordination between people, including by affecting the trade-off between individual safety and social reciprocity. The difference between autobraking and autosteering assistance appears to relate to whether the assistive technology supports or replaces human agency in social coordination dilemmas. Humans have developed norms of reciprocity to address collective challenges, but such tacit understandings could break down in situations where machine intelligence is involved in human decision-making without having any normative commitments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hirokazu Shirado
- Human-Computer Interaction Institute, School of Computer Science, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15206
| | - Shunichi Kasahara
- Sony Computer Science Laboratoires, Inc., Tokyo 141-0022, Japan
- Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology Graduate University, Onna son, Okinawa 904-0412, Japan
| | - Nicholas A Christakis
- Yale Institute for Network Science, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520
- Department of Sociology, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520
- Department of Statistics and Data Science, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Dritsch N, Baras A, Vergnes JN, Bedos C. Towards planetary oral health. SANTE PUBLIQUE (VANDOEUVRE-LES-NANCY, FRANCE) 2023; 35:163-171. [PMID: 38040640 DOI: 10.3917/spub.hs1.2023.0163] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/03/2023]
Abstract
It is time to consider the protection of our environment as a major public health issue in oral medicine. Evidence shows that activities related to dental practice, such as patient transportation, use of rare materials and chemicals, or energy consumption, affect our ecosystems and contribute to the global degradation we are increasingly observing. The degradation of our environment is considered the greatest threat to our health. Exposure of oral tissues to multiple environmental factors can lead to pathological conditions. In addition to these direct effects, there are more complex phenomena, leading to co-deficits in the health of populations. The example of the sugar industry illustrates the systemic failures resulting in the double degradation of the environment and the health of individuals. Face with these dynamically interacting phenomena, human communities must consider systemic responses such as those described in this article. The dental community will need to do its part and consider global oral health as a central issue. This conceptual work will help define the innovations and action needed to ensure equitable practice that respects planetary limits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicolas Dritsch
- Chirurgien-dentiste, pratique privée, CH d’Ardèche méridionale, Saint-Sernin/Aubenas, France
| | - Alice Baras
- Chirurgienne-dentiste, ECOPS Conseil, Lille, France
| | - Jean-Noel Vergnes
- PU-PH, UFR Santé de Toulouse, CHU de Toulouse, UMR 1295, CERPOP Centre d’épidémiologie et de recherche en santé des populations, Toulouse, France
| | - Christophe Bedos
- Professeur agrégé, Faculty of dental medicine and oral health sciences, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Meimandi M, Azad A, Ghani JH, HojabriFard F, von Rosen P, Alizadeh NH, Taghizadeh G. A comparison of the effects of occupation-based interventions with and without responsibility feedback and conventional interventions on participation in people with idiopathic Parkinson's disease: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 2023; 24:610. [PMID: 37749629 PMCID: PMC10521555 DOI: 10.1186/s13063-023-07526-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/08/2023] [Accepted: 07/18/2023] [Indexed: 09/27/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Parkinson's disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disorder with debilitating motor and non-motor symptoms which affect participation in meaningful occupations. Occupation-based interventions can improve participation in people with PD. Evidence for incorporating structured and intensive occupational therapy by considering the concept of responsibility is lacking for this population. This trial will compare the effects of occupation-based interventions with and without responsibility feedback and conventional interventions on participation in people with idiopathic PD. METHODS A total of 45 people with PD, between 35 and 85 years old and Hoehn and Yahr stages between I to III, will be recruited from movement disorder centers for this three-armed study. Participants will be randomized into three groups (occupation-based interventions with responsibility feedback, occupation-based interventions without responsibility feedback, and conventional interventions). All participants will receive intervention for 24 sessions during a period of 12 weeks (2 sessions per week). The primary outcome measure will be participation satisfaction. Participation frequency and restriction, self-perceived performance, performance satisfaction, motivation, volition, sense of agency, responsibility, physical activity, community integration, activities of daily living (ADL), instrumental ADL, upper extremity function, balance, fatigue, and quality of life will be measured as secondary outcome measures. All outcomes will be measured at baseline, session 9, session 17, post-intervention (week 13), and follow-up (week 25). DISCUSSION This home-based high-intensity, structured, client-centered, and occupation-based intervention will be conducted by utilizing the concept of responsibility. This proposed trial may result in enhanced participation that would benefit other motor and non-motor symptoms in people living with PD. Findings from this proposed study are expected to expand the knowledge of clinicians and help them in evidence-based decision-making processes. TRIAL REGISTRATION Iranian Registry of Clinical Trials IRCT20140304016830N13. Registered on August 19, 2022.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mahsa Meimandi
- Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Shahnazari Street, Mirdamad Boulevard, Tehran, Iran
| | - Akram Azad
- Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Shahnazari Street, Mirdamad Boulevard, Tehran, Iran
| | - Jafar Haj Ghani
- Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Shahnazari Street, Mirdamad Boulevard, Tehran, Iran
| | - Fatemeh HojabriFard
- Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Philip von Rosen
- Division of Physiotherapy, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences, and Society (NVS), Karolinska Institutet, Huddinge, Sweden
| | - Naeeme Haji Alizadeh
- Department of Neuroscience, School of Advanced Technology in Medicine, Iran University of Medical Sciences (IUMS), Tehran, Iran
| | - Ghorban Taghizadeh
- Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Shahnazari Street, Mirdamad Boulevard, Tehran, Iran.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
von Mohr M, Finotti G, Esposito G, Bahrami B, Tsakiris M. Social interoception: Perceiving events during cardiac afferent activity makes people more suggestible to other people's influence. Cognition 2023; 238:105502. [PMID: 37336022 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105502] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2022] [Revised: 05/04/2023] [Accepted: 05/24/2023] [Indexed: 06/21/2023]
Abstract
Our judgements are often influenced by other people's views and opinions. Interoception also influences decision making, but little is known about its role in social influence and particularly, the extent to which other people may influence our decisions. Across two experiments, using different forms of social influence, participants judged the trustworthiness of faces presented either during the systolic phase of the cardiac cycle, when baroreceptors convey information from the heart to the brain, or during diastolic phase, when baroreceptors are quiescent. We quantified the extent to which participants changed their minds (as an index of social influence) following the social feedback, in order to compare two competing hypotheses. According to the Arousal-Confidence Hypothesis, cardiac signals create a context of heightened bodily arousal that increases confidence in perceptual judgements. People should, therefore, be less subject to social influence during systole. By contrast, according to the Uncertainty-Conformity Hypothesis, cardiac signals increase neural noise and sensory attenuation, such that people should display greater effects of social influence during systole, as they then underweight private interoceptive signals in favour of the external social information. Across two studies that used different kind of social interactions, we found that participants changed their minds more when faces were presented at systole. Our results, therefore, support the Uncertainly-Conformity hypothesis and highlight how cardiac afferent signals contribute to shape our social decision-making in different types of social interactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mariana von Mohr
- Lab of Action and Body, Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, UK; Centre for the Politics of Feelings, School of Advanced Study, University of London, UK.
| | - Gianluca Finotti
- Center for Studies and Research in Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Italy
| | | | - Bahador Bahrami
- Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany; Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, UK
| | - Manos Tsakiris
- Lab of Action and Body, Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, UK; Centre for the Politics of Feelings, School of Advanced Study, University of London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Gerstenberg T, Lagnado DA, Zultan R. Making a positive difference: Criticality in groups. Cognition 2023; 238:105499. [PMID: 37327565 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105499] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2023] [Revised: 05/09/2023] [Accepted: 05/20/2023] [Indexed: 06/18/2023]
Abstract
How critical are individual members perceived to be for their group's performance? In this paper, we show that judgments of criticality are intimately linked to considering responsibility. Prospective responsibility attributions in groups are relevant across many domains and situations, and have the potential to influence motivation, performance, and allocation of resources. We develop various models that differ in how the relationship between criticality and responsibility is conceptualized. To test our models, we experimentally vary the task structure (disjunctive, conjunctive, and mixed) and the abilities of the group members (which affects their probability of success). We show that both factors influence criticality judgments, and that a model which construes criticality as anticipated credit best explains participants' judgments. Unlike prior work that has defined criticality as anticipated responsibility for both success and failures, our results suggest that people only consider the possible outcomes in which an individual contributed to a group success, but disregard group failure.
Collapse
|
14
|
Longin L, Bahrami B, Deroy O. Intelligence brings responsibility - Even smart AI assistants are held responsible. iScience 2023; 26:107494. [PMID: 37609629 PMCID: PMC10440553 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2023.107494] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/06/2022] [Revised: 06/07/2023] [Accepted: 07/22/2023] [Indexed: 08/24/2023] Open
Abstract
People will not hold cars responsible for traffic accidents, yet they do when artificial intelligence (AI) is involved. AI systems are held responsible when they act or merely advise a human agent. Does this mean that as soon as AI is involved responsibility follows? To find out, we examined whether purely instrumental AI systems stay clear of responsibility. We compared AI-powered with non-AI-powered car warning systems and measured their responsibility rating alongside their human users. Our findings show that responsibility is shared when the warning system is powered by AI but not by a purely mechanical system, even though people consider both systems as mere tools. Surprisingly, whether the warning prevents the accident introduces an outcome bias: the AI takes higher credit than blame depending on what the human manages or fails to do.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Louis Longin
- Faculty of Philosophy, Philosophy of Science and the Study of Religion, LMU Munich, Geschwister-Scholl-Platz 1, 80539 Munich, Germany
| | - Bahador Bahrami
- Crowd Cognition Group, Department of General Psychology and Education, LMU-Munich, Gabelsbergerstraße 62, 80333 Munich, Germany
| | - Ophelia Deroy
- Faculty of Philosophy, Philosophy of Science and the Study of Religion, LMU Munich, Geschwister-Scholl-Platz 1, 80539 Munich, Germany
- Munich Centre for Neurosciences-Brain & Mind, Großhaderner Str. 2, 82152 Munich, Germany
- Institute of Philosophy, School of Advanced Study, University of London, Senate House, Malet Street, London WC1E 7HU, UK
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Meimandi M, Azad A, von Rosen P, Taghizadeh G. Consensus on Feedback Statements That Create Responsibility Among People With Parkinson's Disease: A Delphi Study. Am J Occup Ther 2023; 77:7704205160. [PMID: 37585597 DOI: 10.5014/ajot.2023.050231] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 08/18/2023] Open
Abstract
IMPORTANCE Sense of agency is associated with a sense of responsibility, which is essential to performing goal-directed occupations. OBJECTIVE To reach consensus on a set of extrinsic feedback statements that have the potential to create a sense of responsibility among patients with neurological disorders in the course of performing daily or social occupations. DESIGN Anonymous Delphi study with two rounds with international experts and one round with Irani patients with Parkinson's disease (PD). SETTING Electronic survey. PARTICIPANTS One hundred experts and 73 patients with idiopathic PD. OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Experts and patients anonymously rated (5 = strongly agree/very effective, 4 = agree/effective, 3 = neither agree nor disagree/uncertain, 2 = disagree/ineffective, 1 = strongly disagree/very ineffective) their level of agreement with each survey statement and the effectiveness of each statement in creating a sense of responsibility in the course of performing daily or social occupations. Consensus was set as an interquartile range of ≤1 and ≥70% agreement in two adjacent categories of a Likert scale. RESULTS In the experts' first round, consensus was reached on the level of agreement and effectiveness of 18 statements. In the second round, final consensus was achieved on all statements. In the one patient round, patients reached consensus on all statements. Finally, 34 statements were rated as 4 or 5 in terms of agreement and effectiveness, based on the opinions of experts and patients. CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE This study has produced a collection of feedback statements that might be useful in occupation-based interventions. What This Article Adds: Extrinsic responsibility feedback delivered while administering occupation-based interventions may increase volition, motivation, and engagement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mahsa Meimandi
- Mahsa Meimandi, MSc, is PhD Candidate, Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Akram Azad
- Akram Azad, PhD, is Associate Professor, Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Philip von Rosen
- Philip von Rosen, PhD, is Associate Professor, Division of Physiotherapy, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences, and Society, Karolinska Institutet, Huddinge, Sweden
| | - Ghorban Taghizadeh
- Ghorban Taghizadeh, PhD, is Associate Professor, Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran; or
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Meimandi M, Taghizadeh G, von Rosen P, Azad A. Occupations to Enhance Responsibility Among Patients With Parkinson's Disease: An International Delphi Study. Am J Occup Ther 2023; 77:7704205080. [PMID: 37585598 DOI: 10.5014/ajot.2023.050128] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 08/18/2023] Open
Abstract
IMPORTANCE The ability to perform voluntary actions is disrupted in Parkinson's disease (PD). Voluntary activities play a critical role in generating sense of agency, which underpins the concept of responsibility for people's daily occupations and their outcomes. According to this concept, the dearth of research regarding the concept of responsibility in rehabilitation hampers practitioners in delivering evidence-based care. OBJECTIVE To generate a list of occupations that enhance an inherent sense of responsibility among people with PD that is based on consensus among experts. DESIGN An anonymous and iterative Delphi study with two rounds. SETTING Electronic survey. PARTICIPANTS One hundred sixteen experts participated in the first round of the study, and 95 participated in the second round. OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Panelists rated the level of inherent responsibility in each occupation and the importance of types of patient-related information on a 5-point Likert scale. Consensus was defined as reaching an interquartile range of >1. RESULTS In the first round, consensus was reached on 19 occupations and all 38 types of patient-related information. Also, an additional 15 occupations and 16 types of patient-related information were added to the lists. Consensus was reached for all occupations and patient-related information presented in the second round. CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE Our results indicate that 61 occupations were deemed to enhance a moderate to a very high inherent sense of responsibility among people with PD. In addition, a wide range of patient-related information is considered very important or important while these occupation-focused interventions are delivered. What This Article Adds: Subjective knowledge of one's actions and their consequences lies behind people's daily occupations. Considering this knowledge when administering occupation-focused interventions can be beneficial for individuals with PD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mahsa Meimandi
- Mahsa Meimandi, MSc, is PhD Candidate, Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Ghorban Taghizadeh
- Ghorban Taghizadeh, PhD, is Associate Professor, Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Philip von Rosen
- Philip von Rosen, PhD, is Associate Professor, Division of Physiotherapy, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences, and Society, Karolinska Institutet, Huddinge, Sweden
| | - Akram Azad
- Akram Azad, PhD, is Associate Professor, Rehabilitation Research Center, Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran;
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Pellikka HK. Shared responsibility for decision-making in NICU: A scoping review. Nurs Ethics 2023; 30:462-476. [PMID: 36688269 PMCID: PMC10185855 DOI: 10.1177/09697330221134948] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/24/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Shared responsibility is an essential part of family-centred care and it characterizes the relationship between parents and healthcare professionals. Despite this, little is known about their shared responsibility for decision-making in neonatal intensive care units. AIM The aim of this scoping review was to identify previous studies on the subject and to summarize the knowledge that has been published so far. METHOD The review was conducted using electronic searches in the CINAHL, PubMed, Scopus and PsycINFO databases and manual searches of the reference lists of the selected papers. The searches were limited to peer-reviewed papers that had been published in English from 2010 to September 2021. The data were selected based on inclusion and exclusion criteria and the findings were inductively summarized. We identified eight papers that met the inclusion criteria. ETHICAL CONSIDERATIONS The scoping review was conducted according to good scientific practice by respecting authorship and reporting the study processes accurately, honestly and transparently. RESULTS The results showed that shared responsibility for decision-making was based on the parents' intentions, but the degree to which they were willing to take responsibility varied. The facilitating and inhibiting factors for shared responsibility for decision-making were related to the communication between parents and professionals. The impact was related to the parents' emotions. CONCLUSION It is essential that parents and professionals negotiate how both parties will contribute to their shared responsibility for decision-making. This will enable them to reach a mutual understanding of what is in the infants' best interests and to mitigate the emotional burden of decisions in neonatal intensive care units. More research is needed to clarify the concept of shared responsibility for decision-making in this intensive care context.
Collapse
|
18
|
Ferreiro DN, Deroy O, Bahrami B. Compromising improves forecasting. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2023; 10:221216. [PMID: 37206966 PMCID: PMC10189590 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.221216] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2022] [Accepted: 04/28/2023] [Indexed: 05/21/2023]
Abstract
Predicting the future can bring enormous advantages. Across the ages, reliance on supernatural foreseeing was substituted by the opinion of expert forecasters, and now by collective intelligence approaches which draw on many non-expert forecasters. Yet all of these approaches continue to see individual forecasts as the key unit on which accuracy is determined. Here, we hypothesize that compromise forecasts, defined as the average prediction in a group, represent a better way to harness collective predictive intelligence. We test this by analysing 5 years of data from the Good Judgement Project and comparing the accuracy of individual versus compromise forecasts. Furthermore, given that an accurate forecast is only useful if timely, we analyze how the accuracy changes through time as the events approach. We found that compromise forecasts are more accurate, and that this advantage persists through time, though accuracy varies. Contrary to what was expected (i.e. a monotonous increase in forecasting accuracy as time passes), forecasting error for individuals and for team compromise starts its decline around two months prior to the event. Overall, we offer a method of aggregating forecasts to improve accuracy, which can be straightforwardly applied in noisy real-world settings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dardo N. Ferreiro
- Faculty of General Psychology and Education, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany
- Division of Neurobiology, Faculty of Biology, Ludwig Maximilian University, Planegg-Martinsried, Germany
| | - Ophelia Deroy
- Munich Center for Neuroscience, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany
- Faculty of Philosophy and Philosophy and Science, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany
- Institute of Philosophy, School of Advanced Study, University of London, London, UK
| | - Bahador Bahrami
- Faculty of General Psychology and Education, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany
- Centre for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Shirasuna M, Honda H. Can individual subjective confidence in training questions predict group performance in test questions? PLoS One 2023; 18:e0280984. [PMID: 36881594 PMCID: PMC9990919 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0280984] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2022] [Accepted: 01/11/2023] [Indexed: 03/08/2023] Open
Abstract
When people have to solve many tasks, they can aggregate diverse individuals' judgments using the majority rule, which often improves the accuracy of judgments (wisdom of crowds). When aggregating judgments, individuals' subjective confidence is a useful cue for deciding which judgments to accept. However, can confidence in one task set predict performance not only in the same task set, but also in another? We examined this issue through computer simulations using behavioral data obtained from binary-choice experimental tasks. In our simulations, we developed a "training-test" approach: We split the questions used in the behavioral experiments into "training questions" (as questions to identify individuals' confidence levels) and "test questions" (as questions to be solved), similar to the cross-validation method in machine learning. We found that (i) through analyses of behavioral data, confidence in a certain question could predict accuracy in the same question, but not always well in another question. (ii) Through a computer simulation for the accordance of two individuals' judgments, individuals with high confidence in one training question tended to make less diverse judgments in other test questions. (iii) Through a computer simulation of group judgments, the groups constructed from individuals with high confidence in the training question(s) generally performed well; however, their performance sometimes largely decreased in the test questions especially when only one training question was available. These results suggest that when situations are highly uncertain, an effective strategy is to aggregate various individuals regardless of confidence levels in the training questions to avoid decreasing the group accuracy in test questions. We believe that our simulations, which follow a "training-test" approach, provide practical implications in terms of retaining groups' ability to solve many tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Masaru Shirasuna
- Faculty of Psychology, Otemon Gakuin University, Ibaraki-shi, Osaka, Japan
- * E-mail:
| | - Hidehito Honda
- Faculty of Psychology, Otemon Gakuin University, Ibaraki-shi, Osaka, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Carlebach N, Yeung N. Flexible use of confidence to guide advice requests. Cognition 2023; 230:105264. [PMID: 36087357 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105264] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/10/2021] [Revised: 08/22/2022] [Accepted: 08/23/2022] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
Abstract
Subjective confidence plays an important role in guiding behavior, for example, people typically commit to decisions immediately if high in confidence and seek additional information if not. The present study examines whether people are flexible in their use of confidence, such that the mapping between confidence and behavior is not fixed but can instead vary depending on the specific context. To investigate this proposal, we tested the hypothesis that the seemingly natural relationship between low confidence and requesting advice varies according to whether people know, or do not know, the quality of the advice. Participants made an initial perceptual judgement and then chose between re-sampling evidence or receiving advice from a virtual advisor, before committing to a final decision. The results indicated that, when objective information about advisor reliability was not available, participants selected advice more often when their confidence was high rather than when it was low. This pattern reflects the use of confidence as a feedback proxy to learn about advisor quality: Participants were able to learn about the reliability of advice even in the absence of feedback and subsequently requested more advice from better advisors. In contrast, when participants had prior knowledge about the reliability of advisors, they requested advice more often when their confidence was low, reflecting the use of confidence as a self-monitoring tool signaling that help should be solicited. These findings indicate that people use confidence in a way that is context-dependent and directed towards achieving their current goals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nomi Carlebach
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK.
| | - Nick Yeung
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Park D, Son D, Hamada T, Imaoka S, Lee Y, Kamimoto M, Inoue K, Matsumoto H, Shimosaka T, Sasaki S, Koda M, Taniguchi SI. The Effectiveness of the Multiple-Attending-Physicians System Compared With the Single Attending-Physician System in Inpatient Setting: A Mixed-Method Study. J Prim Care Community Health 2023; 14:21501319231175054. [PMID: 37191304 DOI: 10.1177/21501319231175054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/17/2023] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Medical facilities have been required to effectively utilize insufficient human resources in many countries. Therefore, we qualitatively and quantitively compared physicians' working burden, and assessed advantages and disadvantages of the single- and the multiple-attending physicians systems in inpatient care. METHODS In this cross-sectional study, we extracted electronic health record of patients from a hospital in Japan from April 2017 to October 2018 to compare anonymous statistical data between the single-attending and multiple-attending-physicians system. Then, we conducted a questionnaire survey for all physicians of single and multiple-attending systems, asking about their physical and psychiatric workload, and their reasons and comments on their working styles. RESULTS The average length of hospital stay was significantly shorter in the multiple-attending system than in the single-attending system, while patients' age, gender, and diagnoses were similar. From the questionnaire survey, no significant difference was found in all categories although physical burden in multiple-attending system tended to be lower than that in single-attending system. Advantages of multiple-attending system extracted from qualitative analysis are (1) improvement of physicians' quality of life (QOL), (2) lifelong-learning effect, and (3) improving the quality of medical care, while disadvantages were (1) risk of miscommunications, (2) conflicting treatment policies among physicians, and (3) patients' concern. CONCLUSIONS The multiple-attending physician system in the inpatient setting can reduce the average length of stay for patients and also reduce the physical burden on physicians without compromising their clinical performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daeho Park
- Family Clinic Kakogawa, Kakogawa, Hyogo, Japan
| | - Daisuke Son
- Tottori University, Yonago, Tottori, Japan
- Hino Hospital, Hino, Tottori, Japan
| | | | - Shintaro Imaoka
- Tottori University, Yonago, Tottori, Japan
- Hino Hospital, Hino, Tottori, Japan
| | - Young Lee
- Tottori University, Yonago, Tottori, Japan
- Hino Hospital, Hino, Tottori, Japan
| | | | - Kazuoki Inoue
- National Health Insurance Daisen Clinic, Saihaku-gun, Tottori, Japan
| | - Hiromi Matsumoto
- Kawasaki University of Medical Welfare, Kurashiki, Okayama, Japan
| | | | | | | | - Shin-Ichi Taniguchi
- Tottori University, Yonago, Tottori, Japan
- Hino Hospital, Hino, Tottori, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Shreedhar G, Thomas‐Walters L. Experimental evidence of the impact of framing of actors and victims in conservation narratives. CONSERVATION BIOLOGY : THE JOURNAL OF THE SOCIETY FOR CONSERVATION BIOLOGY 2022; 36:e14015. [PMID: 36301017 PMCID: PMC10092605 DOI: 10.1111/cobi.14015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2022] [Revised: 06/04/2022] [Accepted: 06/30/2022] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
Media narratives play a crucial role in framing marine conservation dilemmas by depicting human actors, such as fish consumers or the fishing industry, as responsible for negative effects of their actions on species and ecosystems. However, there is little evidence documenting how such narratives affect preferences for reducing bycatch. Behavioral science research shows that people can act less prosocially when more actors are responsible for a collective outcome (responsibility diffusion effect) and when more victims need to be helped (compassion fade effect); thus, the media's framing of actors and victims may have a significant effect on preferences. We conducted the first test of responsibility diffusion and compassion fade in a marine context in an online experiment (1548 participants in the United Kingdom). In 9 media narratives, we varied the type of actors responsible for fisheries bycatch (e.g., consumers and industry) and victims (e.g., a single species, multiple species, and ecosystems) in media narratives and determined the effects of the narratives on participants' support for bycatch policies and intentions to alter fish consumption. When responsibility for negative effects was attributed to consumers and industry, the probability of participants reporting support for fisheries policies (e.g., bycatch enforcement or consumer taxes) was ∼30% higher (odds ratio = 1.32) than when only consumers were attributed responsibility. These effects were primarily driven by female participants. Narratives had no effect on personal intentions to consume fish. Varying the type of victim had no effect on policy support and intentions. Our results suggest that neither responsibility diffusion nor compassion fade automatically follows from increasing the types of actors and victims in media narratives and that effects can depend on the type of outcome and population subgroup.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ganga Shreedhar
- Department of Psychological and Behavioural ScienceLondon School of Economics and Political ScienceHoughton StreetLondonWC2A 2AEUK
| | | |
Collapse
|
23
|
Abstract
Sharing responsibility in social decision-making helps individuals use the flexibility of the collective context to benefit themselves by claiming credit for good outcomes or avoiding the blame for bad outcomes. Using magnetoencephalography, we examined the neuronal basis of the impact that social context has on this flexible sense of responsibility. Participants performed a gambling task in various social contexts and reported feeling less responsibility when playing as a member of a team. A reduced magnetoencephalography outcome processing effect was observed as a function of decreasing responsibility at 200 msec post outcome onset and was centered over parietal, central, and frontal brain regions. Before outcome revelation in socially made decisions, an attenuated motor preparation signature at 500 msec after stimulus onset was found. A boost in reported responsibility for positive outcomes in social contexts was associated with increased activity in regions related to social and reward processing. Together, these results show that sharing responsibility with others reduces agency, influencing pre-outcome motor preparation and post-outcome processing, and provides opportunities to flexibly claim credit for positive outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marwa El Zein
- Max Planck Center for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- University College London, United Kingdom
| | | | - Bahador Bahrami
- University College London, United Kingdom
- Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Keshmirian A, Hemmatian B, Bahrami B, Deroy O, Cushman F. Diffusion of punishment in collective norm violations. Sci Rep 2022; 12:15318. [PMID: 36097011 PMCID: PMC9467972 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-19156-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2022] [Accepted: 08/24/2022] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
People assign less punishment to individuals who inflict harm collectively, compared to those who do so alone. We show that this arises from judgments of diminished individual causal responsibility in the collective cases. In Experiment 1, participants (N = 1002) assigned less punishment to individuals involved in collective actions leading to intentional and accidental deaths, but not failed attempts, emphasizing that harmful outcomes, but not malicious intentions, were necessary and sufficient for the diffusion of punishment. Experiments 2.a compared the diffusion of punishment for harmful actions with 'victimless' purity violations (e.g., eating a dead human's flesh as a group; N = 752). In victimless cases, where the question of causal responsibility for harm does not arise, diffusion of collective responsibility was greatly reduced-an outcome replicated in Experiment 2.b (N = 479). Together, the results are consistent with discounting in causal attribution as the underlying mechanism of reduction in proposed punishment for collective harmful actions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anita Keshmirian
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, USA. .,Graduate School for Neuroscience, Ludwig-Maximilians-University of Munich, Munich, Germany. .,Faculty of Philosophy, Ludwig-Maximilians University, Munich, Germany. .,Munich Center for Mathematical Philosophy, Munich, Germany.
| | - Babak Hemmatian
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, USA
| | - Bahador Bahrami
- Faculty of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-University, Munich, Germany.,Department for Psychology, Royal Holloway University of London, Egham, UK.,Centre for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | - Ophelia Deroy
- Faculty of Philosophy, Ludwig-Maximilians University, Munich, Germany.,Munich Center for Neuroscience, Munich, Germany.,Institute of Philosophy, School of Advanced Study, University of London, London, UK
| | - Fiery Cushman
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, USA
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Chan TKH, Cheung CMK, Benbasat I, Xiao B, Lee ZWY. Bystanders Join in Cyberbullying on Social Networking Sites: The Deindividuation and Moral Disengagement Perspectives. INFORMATION SYSTEMS RESEARCH 2022. [DOI: 10.1287/isre.2022.1161] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Bystanders Join in Cyberbullying on Social Networking Sites: The Deindividuation and Moral Disengagement Perspectives Cyberbullying on social networking sites escalates when bystanders join in the bullying. Bystanders’ joining-in behaviors reinforce the abuse, expose victims to a larger audience, and encourage further abuse by signaling their approval of the aggressive behavior. This study developed an integrative model that explains bystanders’ joining-in cyberbullying behaviors on SNSs to offer actionable insights into reducing such harmful behaviors. We tested the model using 1,179 responses using a scenario survey study. Our findings suggest that IT artifacts (including digital profile, search and privacy, relational ties, and network transparency) activated two key mechanisms that lead to cyberbullying joining-in behaviors: (i) the deindividuation experiences that attenuate self-identity and put salience on group/social identity, and (ii) the moral disengagement practices that permit the exercise of cognitive maneuvers to justify group-interested choices that do not align with social standard. The findings explain why people who do not know each other gang up to bully a target on social media. Platform owners who wish to discourage bystanders from joining in undesirable activities may consider regulating how users could share and access digital resources in a social network and should acknowledge the influence of social identity in igniting, driving, and prolonging harmful online group behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tommy K. H. Chan
- Alliance Manchester Business School, The University of Manchester, Manchester M15 6PB, United Kingdom
| | | | - Izak Benbasat
- Sauder School of Business, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver BC V6T 1Z2, Canada
| | - Bo Xiao
- Shidler College of Business, The University of Hawaiʻi at Mānoa, Honolulu, Hawaii 96822
| | - Zach W. Y. Lee
- Durham University Business School, Durham University, Durham DH1 3LB, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Curioni A. What makes us act together? On the cognitive models supporting humans’ decisions for joint action. Front Integr Neurosci 2022; 16:900527. [PMID: 35990592 PMCID: PMC9381741 DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2022.900527] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2022] [Accepted: 07/13/2022] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
We face tasks every day that we can solve alone but decide to solve together with others. When do we choose to act together vs. alone? How long do we persist in working together when doing so is difficult? Do we prefer to act together when times are uncertain? An open question in joint action research is under what conditions humans prefer to act together or alone to achieve a certain goal, and whether their preference is based on a utility calculus that takes into account the costs and benefits associated with individual and joint action alternatives. Research on cooperation reveals that frequent engagement in joint activities provides high survival benefits, as it allows individuals to achieve goals together that are otherwise unavailable. Yet, survival advantage does not wholly explain the reasons for human cooperative behavior. In fact, humans are motivated to cooperate even when it is not necessary to achieve an outcome. Research in cognitive science suggests that navigating the potential costs of joint actions is a challenge for humans, and that joint actions might provide individuals with rewards that go beyond the achievement of instrumental goals. We here address the influence of key factors on the decision to engage in joint action, such as the coordination costs arising when acting together compared to alone and the social and instrumental rewards expected when acting together compared to alone. Addressing these questions will provide critical insight for the design of cognitive models of human decisions for cooperation.
Collapse
|
27
|
Tunçgenç B, Newson M, Sulik J, Zhao Y, Dezecache G, Deroy O, Zein ME. Social alignment matters: Following pandemic guidelines is associated with better wellbeing. BMC Public Health 2022; 22:821. [PMID: 35501759 PMCID: PMC9060841 DOI: 10.1186/s12889-022-13130-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2021] [Accepted: 03/30/2022] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND In response to the Covid-19 pandemic, most countries implemented physical distancing measures. Many mental health experts warned that through increasing social isolation and anxiety, these measures could negatively affect psychosocial wellbeing. However, socially aligning with others by adhering to these measures may also be beneficial for wellbeing. METHODS We examined these two contrasting hypotheses using cross-national survey data (N = 6675) collected fortnightly from participants in 115 countries over 3 months at the beginning of the pandemic. Participants reported their wellbeing, perceptions of how vulnerable they were to Covid-19 (i.e., high risk of infection) and how much they, and others in their social circle and country, were adhering to the distancing measures. RESULTS Linear mixed-effects models showed that being a woman, having lower educational attainment, living alone and perceived high vulnerability to Covid-19 were risk factors for poorer wellbeing. Being young (18-25) was associated with lower wellbeing, but longitudinal analyses showed that young people's wellbeing improved over 3 months. In contrast to widespread views that physical distancing measures negatively affect wellbeing, results showed that following the guidelines was positively associated with wellbeing even for people in high-risk groups. CONCLUSIONS These findings provide an important counterpart to the idea that pandemic containment measures such as physical distancing negatively impacted wellbeing unequivocally. Despite the overall burden of the pandemic on psychosocial wellbeing, social alignment with others can still contribute to positive wellbeing. The pandemic has manifested our propensity to adapt to challenges, particularly highlighting how social alignment can forge resilience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bahar Tunçgenç
- Department of Psychology, Nottingham Trent University, London, UK.
- Institute of Cognitive & Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK.
| | - Martha Newson
- Institute of Cognitive & Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
- School of Anthropology and Conservation, University of Kent, Kent, UK
| | - Justin Sulik
- Cognition, Values and Behaviour, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany
| | - Yi Zhao
- School of Medicine, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana, USA
| | | | - Ophelia Deroy
- Cognition, Values and Behaviour, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany
- Munich Center for Neuroscience, Ludwig-Maximilian University, Munich, Germany
- Institute of Philosophy, School of Advanced Study, University of London, London, UK
| | - Marwa El Zein
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK
- Adaptive Rationality Center, Max-Planck for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Jaquiery M, El Zein M. Stage 2 Registered Report: How responsibility attributions to self and others relate to outcome ownership in group decisions. Wellcome Open Res 2022; 6:362. [PMID: 35368906 PMCID: PMC8961199 DOI: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.17504.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/07/2022] [Indexed: 01/13/2023] Open
Abstract
Background: Responsibility judgements have important consequences in human society. Previous research focused on how someone's responsibility determines the outcome they deserve, for example, whether they are rewarded or punished. Here, in a pre-registered study (Stage 1 Registered Report: https://doi.org/10.12688/wellcomeopenres.16480.2), we investigate the opposite link: How outcome ownership influences responsibility attributions in a social context. Methods: In an online study, participants in a group of three perform a majority vote decision-making task between gambles that can lead to a reward or no reward. Only one group member receives the outcome and participants evaluate their and the other players' responsibility for the obtained outcome. Results: We found that outcome ownership increases responsibility attributions even when the control over an outcome is similar. Moreover, ownership had an effect on the valence bias: participants' higher responsibility attributions for positive vs negative outcomes was stronger for players who received the outcome. Finally, this effect was more pronounced when people rated their own responsibility as compared to when they were rating another's player responsibility. Conclusions: The findings of this study reveal how credit attributions can be biased toward particular individuals who receive outcomes as a result of collective work, both when people judge their own and someone else's responsibility.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matt Jaquiery
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Marwa El Zein
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK
- Adaptive Rationality Center, Max-Planck for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Valera L, López Barreda R. Bioethics and COVID-19: Considering the Social Determinants of Health. Front Med (Lausanne) 2022; 9:824791. [PMID: 35391891 PMCID: PMC8980461 DOI: 10.3389/fmed.2022.824791] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2021] [Accepted: 02/28/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In this paper, we focus on a novel bioethical approach concerning the ethical implications of the Social Determinants of Health (SDs) in the time of COVID-19, offering a fresh interpretation of our agency and responsibility in the current pandemic era. Our interpretation is grounded on the idea that our health basically depends on factors that go beyond our organism. In this sense, we stress the radical importance of circumstances to ethically assess an action, in the current pandemic context. Moreover, due the centrality of the SDs in our bioethical assessments-that implies that our health does not exclusively depend on our choices, behaviors, and lifestyle-we can affirm that we are not entirely responsible for our wellness or diseases. As health depends on economic, social, cultural, and environmental factors, we argue that the analysis of personal responsibility facing personal health status should receive further consideration. In this sense, following the "social connection model," we stress the importance of the concept of "shared responsibility" in collective decisions: if we make many decisions collectively, we are also collectively responsible of these decisions. Furthermore, to responsibly tackle the social inequalities that are the underlying cause of disparities in health outcomes, we propose two main strategies based on the Capability Approach: 1. empowering the individuals, especially the most vulnerable ones; and 2. designing preventive policies and interventions that provides an opportunity to address the disparities moving forward. This will help us going beyond the "individualistic medical ethics paradigm" and integrating our concept of health with social factors (e.g., the SDs), based on a more relational and interdependent anthropological thought.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luca Valera
- Bioethics Centre, School of Medicine, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago de Chile, Chile
- Department of Philosophy, Universidad de Valladolid, Valladolid, Spain
| | - Rodrigo López Barreda
- Bioethics Centre, School of Medicine, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago de Chile, Chile
- Department of Anesthesiology, School of Medicine, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago de Chile, Chile
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Ivany E, Lotto R, Lip GYH, Lane D. Managing uncertainty: Physicians’ decision-making for stroke prevention for patients with atrial fibrillation and intracerebral haemorrhage. Thromb Haemost 2022; 122:1603-1611. [DOI: 10.1055/a-1789-4824] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Background Stroke prevention in patients with atrial fibrillation (AF) post-intracerebral haemorrhage (ICH) is an area of clinical equipoise. Little is known about the tools and processes that physicians use to make decisions regarding anticoagulation in this high-risk patient population.
Objective To explore physicians’ decision-making process regarding stroke prevention in patients with AF and a recent history of ICH.
Method Qualitative study, utilising semi-structured interviews and analysed using Framework analysis.
Results Twenty physicians from five European countries (Austria, France, Germany, Spain, United Kingdom) participated. The over-arching theme ‘Managing uncertainty’, addressed the process of making high-risk clinical decisions in the context of little available robust clinical evidence for best practice. Three sub-themes were identified under the umbrella theme: (1) ‘Computing the Risks’, captured the challenge of balancing the risks of ischaemic stroke with the risk of recurrent ICH in a complex patient population; (2) ‘Patient Factors’ highlighted the influence that patients’ beliefs and previous experience of stroke had on physicians’ decisions; and (3) ‘Making a Decision’ explored the process of reaching a final decision regarding initiation of OAC therapy or not.
Conclusion Physicians described the process of deciding on stroke prevention in patients with AF post-ICH as ‘challenging’ due to considerable ‘clinical equipoise’. Key factors that affected decision-making was patient comorbidities, functional status, and patient willingness to engage with oral anticoagulation therapy. Shared decision-making was believed to be beneficial, but physicians believed that the ultimate responsibility to decide on stroke prevention lay with the clinician.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Ivany
- Liverpool Centre for Cardiovascular Science, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | - Robyn Lotto
- Liverpool John Moores University - City Campus, Liverpool, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | - Gregory YH Lip
- Liverpool Centre for Cardiovascular Science, University of Liverpool Institute of Ageing and Chronic Disease, Liverpool, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | - Deirdre Lane
- Liverpool Centre of Cardiovascular Science, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Mahmoodi A, Nili H, Bang D, Mehring C, Bahrami B. Distinct neurocomputational mechanisms support informational and socially normative conformity. PLoS Biol 2022; 20:e3001565. [PMID: 35239647 PMCID: PMC8893340 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001565] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2021] [Accepted: 02/02/2022] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
A change of mind in response to social influence could be driven by informational conformity to increase accuracy, or by normative conformity to comply with social norms such as reciprocity. Disentangling the behavioural, cognitive, and neurobiological underpinnings of informational and normative conformity have proven elusive. Here, participants underwent fMRI while performing a perceptual task that involved both advice-taking and advice-giving to human and computer partners. The concurrent inclusion of 2 different social roles and 2 different social partners revealed distinct behavioural and neural markers for informational and normative conformity. Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) BOLD response tracked informational conformity towards both human and computer but tracked normative conformity only when interacting with humans. A network of brain areas (dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) and temporoparietal junction (TPJ)) that tracked normative conformity increased their functional coupling with the dACC when interacting with humans. These findings enable differentiating the neural mechanisms by which different types of conformity shape social changes of mind.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ali Mahmoodi
- Bernstein Centre Freiburg, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
- Faculty of Biology, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
- Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
- * E-mail: (AM); (BB)
| | - Hamed Nili
- Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, Centre for Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
- Department of Excellence for Neural Information Processing, Center for Molecular Neurobiology (ZMNH), University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf (UKE), Hamburg, Germany
| | - Dan Bang
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London, London, United Kingdom
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Carsten Mehring
- Bernstein Centre Freiburg, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
- Faculty of Biology, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Bahador Bahrami
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, United Kingdom
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- * E-mail: (AM); (BB)
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Zapparoli L, Paulesu E, Mariano M, Ravani A, Sacheli LM. The sense of agency in joint actions: a theory-driven meta-analysis. Cortex 2022; 148:99-120. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.01.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2021] [Revised: 11/15/2021] [Accepted: 01/05/2022] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
|
33
|
Jaquiery M, El Zein M. Stage 2 Registered Report: How responsibility attributions to self and others relate to outcome ownership in group decisions. Wellcome Open Res 2021; 6:362. [PMID: 35368906 PMCID: PMC8961199 DOI: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.17504.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/20/2021] [Indexed: 01/13/2023] Open
Abstract
Background: Responsibility judgements have important consequences in human society. Previous research focused on how someone's responsibility determines the outcome they deserve, for example, whether they are rewarded or punished. Here, in a pre-registered study (Stage 1 Registered Report: https://doi.org/10.12688/wellcomeopenres.16480.2), we investigate the opposite link: How outcome ownership influences responsibility attributions in a social context. Methods: In an online study, participants in a group of three perform a majority vote decision-making task between gambles that can lead to a reward or no reward. Only one group member receives the outcome and participants evaluate their and the other players' responsibility for the obtained outcome. Results: We found that outcome ownership increases responsibility attributions even when the control over an outcome is similar. Moreover, ownership had an effect on the valence bias: participants' higher responsibility attributions for positive vs negative outcomes was stronger for players who received the outcome. Finally, this effect was more pronounced when people rated their own responsibility as compared to when they were rating another's player responsibility. Conclusions: The findings of this study reveal how credit attributions can be biased toward particular individuals who receive outcomes as a result of collective work, both when people judge their own and someone else's responsibility.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matt Jaquiery
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Marwa El Zein
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK
- Adaptive Rationality Center, Max-Planck for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Many heads are more utilitarian than one. Cognition 2021; 220:104965. [PMID: 34872034 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104965] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2020] [Revised: 11/15/2021] [Accepted: 11/18/2021] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Moral judgments have a very prominent social nature, and in everyday life, they are continually shaped by discussions with others. Psychological investigations of these judgments, however, have rarely addressed the impact of social interactions. To examine the role of social interaction on moral judgments within small groups, we had groups of 4 to 5 participants judge moral dilemmas first individually and privately, then collectively and interactively, and finally individually a second time. We employed both real-life and sacrificial moral dilemmas in which the character's action or inaction violated a moral principle to benefit the greatest number of people. Participants decided if these utilitarian decisions were morally acceptable or not. In Experiment 1, we found that collective judgments in face-to-face interactions were more utilitarian than the statistical aggregate of their members compared to both first and second individual judgments. This observation supported the hypothesis that deliberation and consensus within a group transiently reduce the emotional burden of norm violation. In Experiment 2, we tested this hypothesis more directly: measuring participants' state anxiety in addition to their moral judgments before, during, and after online interactions, we found again that collectives were more utilitarian than those of individuals and that state anxiety level was reduced during and after social interaction. The utilitarian boost in collective moral judgments is probably due to the reduction of stress in the social setting.
Collapse
|
35
|
Baumgartner T, Hausfeld J, Dos Santos M, Knoch D. Who initiates punishment, who joins punishment? Disentangling types of third-party punishers by neural traits. Hum Brain Mapp 2021; 42:5703-5717. [PMID: 34523772 PMCID: PMC8559474 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.25648] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2020] [Revised: 08/19/2021] [Accepted: 08/21/2021] [Indexed: 12/23/2022] Open
Abstract
The act of punishing unfair behavior by unaffected observers (i.e., third‐party punishment) is a crucial factor in the functioning of human societies. In everyday life, we see different types of individuals who punish. While some individuals initiate costly punishment against an unfair person independently of what other observers do (independent punishers), others condition their punishment engagement on the presence of another person who punishes (conditional punishers). Still others do not want to partake in any sort of punishment (nonpunishers). Although these distinct behavioral types have a divergent impact on human society, the sources of heterogeneity are poorly understood. We present novel laboratory evidence on the existence of these three types. We use anatomical brain characteristics in combination with stated motives to characterize these types. Findings revealed that independent punishers have larger gray matter volume in the right temporo‐parietal junction compared to conditional punishers and nonpunishers, an area involved in social cognition. Conditional punishers are characterized by larger gray matter volume in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, a brain area known to be involved in behavioral control and strategic reasoning, compared to independent punishers and nonpunishers. Finally, both independent punishers and nonpunishers are characterized by larger gray matter volume in an area involved in the processing of social and monetary rewards, that is, the bilateral caudate. By using a neural trait approach, we were able to differentiate these three types clearly based on their neural signatures, allowing us to shed light on the underlying psychological mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Baumgartner
- Department of Social Neuroscience and Social Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| | - Jan Hausfeld
- Department of Social Neuroscience and Social Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland.,CREED, Amsterdam School of Economics, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Miguel Dos Santos
- Department of Social Neuroscience and Social Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| | - Daria Knoch
- Department of Social Neuroscience and Social Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Task complexity moderates group synergy. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2021; 118:2101062118. [PMID: 34479999 PMCID: PMC8433503 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2101062118] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/28/2021] [Accepted: 07/02/2021] [Indexed: 01/20/2023] Open
Abstract
Scientists and managers alike have been preoccupied with the question of whether and, if so, under what conditions groups of interacting problem solvers outperform autonomous individuals. Here we describe an experiment in which individuals and groups were evaluated on a series of tasks of varying complexity. We find that groups are as fast as the fastest individual and more efficient than the most efficient individual when the task is complex but not when the task is simple. We then precisely quantify synergistic gains and process losses associated with interacting groups, finding that the balance between the two depends on complexity. Our study has the potential to reconcile conflicting findings about group synergy in previous work. Complexity—defined in terms of the number of components and the nature of the interdependencies between them—is clearly a relevant feature of all tasks that groups perform. Yet the role that task complexity plays in determining group performance remains poorly understood, in part because no clear language exists to express complexity in a way that allows for straightforward comparisons across tasks. Here we avoid this analytical difficulty by identifying a class of tasks for which complexity can be varied systematically while keeping all other elements of the task unchanged. We then test the effects of task complexity in a preregistered two-phase experiment in which 1,200 individuals were evaluated on a series of tasks of varying complexity (phase 1) and then randomly assigned to solve similar tasks either in interacting groups or as independent individuals (phase 2). We find that interacting groups are as fast as the fastest individual and more efficient than the most efficient individual for complex tasks but not for simpler ones. Leveraging our highly granular digital data, we define and precisely measure group process losses and synergistic gains and show that the balance between the two switches signs at intermediate values of task complexity. Finally, we find that interacting groups generate more solutions more rapidly and explore the solution space more broadly than independent problem solvers, finding higher-quality solutions than all but the highest-scoring individuals.
Collapse
|
37
|
Affiliation(s)
| | - Chris D Frith
- Institute of Philosophy, School of Advanced Study, University of London, London, UK; Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London, London, UK
| | - Ophelia Deroy
- Institute of Philosophy, School of Advanced Study, University of London, London, UK; Munich Center for Neuroscience, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany; Faculty of Philosophy, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany; NOMIS Foundation, Zurich, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Kurvers RH, Herzog SM, Hertwig R, Krause J, Wolf M. Pooling decisions decreases variation in response bias and accuracy. iScience 2021; 24:102740. [PMID: 34278254 PMCID: PMC8267549 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2021.102740] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2020] [Revised: 05/03/2021] [Accepted: 06/15/2021] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Decision makers in contexts as diverse as medical, judicial, and political decision making are known to differ substantially in response bias and accuracy, and these differences are a major factor undermining the reliability and fairness of the respective decision systems. Using theoretical modeling and empirical testing across five domains, we show that collective systems based on pooling decisions robustly overcome this important but as of now unresolved problem of experts' heterogeneity. In breast and skin cancer diagnostics and fingerprint analysis, we find that pooling the decisions of five experts reduces the variation in sensitivity among decision makers by 52%, 54%, and 41%, respectively. Similar reductions are achieved for specificity and response bias, and in other domains. Thus, although outcomes in individual decision systems are highly variable and at the mercy of individual decision makers, collective systems based on pooling decrease this variation, thereby promoting reliability, fairness, and possibly even trust.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ralf H.J.M. Kurvers
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany
- Leibniz Institute of Freshwater Ecology and Inland Fisheries, Müggelseedamm 310, 12587 Berlin, Germany
| | - Stefan M. Herzog
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany
| | - Ralph Hertwig
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany
| | - Jens Krause
- Leibniz Institute of Freshwater Ecology and Inland Fisheries, Müggelseedamm 310, 12587 Berlin, Germany
- Faculty of Life Sciences, Albrecht Daniel Thaer-Institute of Agricultural and Horticultural Sciences, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Invalidenstrasse 42, 10115 Berlin, Germany
| | - Max Wolf
- Leibniz Institute of Freshwater Ecology and Inland Fisheries, Müggelseedamm 310, 12587 Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Pescetelli N, Hauperich AK, Yeung N. Confidence, advice seeking and changes of mind in decision making. Cognition 2021; 215:104810. [PMID: 34147712 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104810] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2020] [Revised: 06/07/2021] [Accepted: 06/10/2021] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Humans and other animals rely on social learning strategies to guide their behaviour, especially when the task is difficult and individual learning might be costly or ineffective. Recent models of individual and group decision-making suggest that subjective confidence judgments are a prime candidate in guiding the way people seek and integrate information from social sources. The present study investigates the way people choose and use advice as a function of the confidence in their decisions, using a perceptual decision task to carefully control the quality of participants' decisions and the advice provided. The results show that reported confidence guides the search for new information in accordance with probabilistic normative models. Moreover, large inter-individual differences were found, which strongly correlated with more traditional measures of metacognition. However, the extent to which participants used the advice they received deviated from what would be expected under a Bayesian update of confidence, and instead was characterised by heuristic-like strategies of categorically ignoring vs. accepting advice provided, again with substantial individual differences apparent in the relative dominance of these strategies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Niccolò Pescetelli
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK; Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany.
| | | | - Nick Yeung
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Jaquiery M, El Zein M. Stage 1 Registered Report: How responsibility attributions to self and others relate to outcome ownership in group decisions. Wellcome Open Res 2021; 6:24. [PMID: 34250261 PMCID: PMC8258704 DOI: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.16480.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/26/2021] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Responsibility judgements have important consequences in human society. Previous research focused on how someone's responsibility determines the outcome they deserve, for example, whether they are rewarded or punished. Here, we investigate the opposite link: How outcome ownership influences responsibility attributions in a social context. Participants in a group of three perform a majority vote decision-making task between gambles that can lead to a reward or no reward. Only one group member receives the outcome and participants evaluate their and the other players' responsibility for the obtained outcome. Two hypotheses are tested: 1) Whether outcome ownership increases responsibility attributions even when the control over an outcome is similar. 2) Whether people's tendency to attribute higher responsibility for positive vs negative outcomes will be stronger for players who received the outcome. The findings of this study may help reveal how credit attributions can be biased toward particular individuals who receive outcomes as a result of collective work.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matt Jaquiery
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Marwa El Zein
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, WC1N 3AZ, UK.,Adaptive Rationality Center, Max-Planck for Human Development, Berlin, 14195, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Jaquiery M, El Zein M. Stage 1 Registered Report: How responsibility attributions to self and others relate to outcome ownership in group decisions. Wellcome Open Res 2021; 6:24. [DOI: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.16480.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 01/25/2021] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Responsibility judgements have important consequences in human society. Previous research focused on how someone's responsibility determines the outcome they deserve, for example, whether they are rewarded or punished. Here, we investigate the opposite link: How outcome ownership influences responsibility attributions in a social context. Participants in a group of three perform a majority vote decision-making task between gambles that can lead to a reward or no reward. Only one group member receives the outcome and participants evaluate their and the other players' responsibility for the obtained outcome. Two hypotheses are tested: 1) Whether outcome ownership increases responsibility attributions even when the control over an outcome is similar. 2) Whether people's tendency to attribute higher responsibility for positive vs negative outcomes will be stronger for players who received the outcome. The findings of this study may help reveal how credit attributions can be biased toward particular individuals who receive outcomes as a result of collective work.
Collapse
|
42
|
Hayashida K, Miyawaki Y, Nishi Y, Morioka S. Changes of Causal Attribution by a Co-actor in Situations of Obvious Causality. Front Psychol 2021; 11:588089. [PMID: 33488455 PMCID: PMC7815702 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.588089] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2020] [Accepted: 12/03/2020] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In social contexts, people are responsible for their actions and outcomes. Diffusion of responsibility is a well-known social phenomenon: people feel less responsible when performing an action with co-actors than when acting alone. In previous studies, co-actors reduced the participant’s responsibility attribution by making the cause of the outcomes ambiguous. Meanwhile, it is unclear whether the presence of co-actors creates diffusion of responsibility even in situations where it is “obvious” that both oneself and the co-actor are the causes of an outcome. To investigate this potential diffusion of responsibility, we used a temporal binding (TB) task as a measure of causal attribution. Low TB effects indicate the enhancement of external attribution (i.e., diffusion of responsibility) in perceptual processing for the action and outcomes. To investigate the influence of presence of a co-actor on causal attribution, participants were required to act under two experimental conditions: an ALONE condition (participant only) or a TOGETHER condition (with a co-actor). The only difference between the two conditions was whether the actions were shared. In addition, to make participants feel responsible, they were induced to feel guilt. In the High-harm condition, participants gave a financial reduction to a third party. When guilt was induced, participants showed lower TB effects in the TOGETHER condition compared to the ALONE condition. Our study suggests that actions with a co-actor change causal attributions even though the causes of the outcome are obvious. This may have implications for understanding diffusion of responsibility in inhumane situations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kazuki Hayashida
- Department of Neurorehabilitation, Graduate School of Health Sciences, Kio University, Koryo, Japan.,Department of Rehabilitation, Fujiikai Rehabilitation Hospital, Osaka, Japan
| | - Yu Miyawaki
- Department of Neurorehabilitation, Graduate School of Health Sciences, Kio University, Koryo, Japan.,Research Fellow of Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo, Japan.,Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Keio University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Yuki Nishi
- Department of Neurorehabilitation, Graduate School of Health Sciences, Kio University, Koryo, Japan
| | - Shu Morioka
- Department of Neurorehabilitation, Graduate School of Health Sciences, Kio University, Koryo, Japan.,Neurorehabilitation Research Center, Kio University, Koryo, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Etgar R, Tamir E. Are millennial students better equipped to overcome choice bias? INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ADOLESCENCE AND YOUTH 2020. [DOI: 10.1080/02673843.2019.1657026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Ran Etgar
- Faculty of Engineering, Ruppin academic centre , Emek-Hefer, Israel
| | - Emanuel Tamir
- Faculty of Engineering, Ohalo Academic Institute for Education , Kazrin, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Cartaud A, Quesque F, Coello Y. Wearing a face mask against Covid-19 results in a reduction of social distancing. PLoS One 2020; 15:e0243023. [PMID: 33284812 PMCID: PMC7721169 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 79] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2020] [Accepted: 11/15/2020] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
In the context of the Covid-19 pandemic, barrier gestures such as regular hand-washing, social distancing, and wearing a face mask are highly recommended. Critically, interpersonal distance (IPD) depends on the affective dimension of social interaction, which might be affected by the current Covid-19 context. In the present internet-based experimental study, we analyzed the preferred IPD of 457 French participants when facing human-like characters that were either wearing a face mask or displaying a neutral, happy or angry facial expression. Results showed that IPD was significantly reduced when characters were wearing a face mask, as they were perceived as more trustworthy compared to the other conditions. Importantly, IPD was even more reduced in participants infected with Covid-19 or living in low-risk areas, while it was not affected by the predicted health of the characters. These findings shed further light on the psychological factors that motivate IPD adjustments, in particular when facing a collective threat. They are also of crucial importance for policy makers as they reveal that despite the indisputable value of wearing a face mask in the current pandemic context, their use should be accompanied by an emphasis on social distancing to prevent detrimental health consequences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alice Cartaud
- CNRS, UMR 9193 - SCALab - Sciences Cognitives et Sciences Affectives, Univ. Lille, Lille, France
| | - François Quesque
- Inserm, CHU Lille, U1172 - LilNCog - Lille Neuroscience & Cognition, Univ. Lille, Lille, France
| | - Yann Coello
- CNRS, UMR 9193 - SCALab - Sciences Cognitives et Sciences Affectives, Univ. Lille, Lille, France
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Schulze C, Gaissmaier W, Newell BR. Maximizing as satisficing: On pattern matching and probability maximizing in groups and individuals. Cognition 2020; 205:104382. [PMID: 32854942 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104382] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2019] [Revised: 06/16/2020] [Accepted: 06/17/2020] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Distinguishing meaningful structure from unpredictable randomness is a key challenge in many domains of life. We examined whether collaborating three-person groups (n = 81) outperform individuals (n = 81) in facing this challenge with a two-part repeated choice task, where outcomes were either serially independent (probabilistic part) or fixed in a particular sequence (pattern part). Groups performed as well as the best individuals in the probabilistic part but groups' accuracy did not credibly exceed that of the average individual in the pattern part. Qualitative coding of group discussion data revealed that failures to identify existing patterns were related to groups accepting probability maximizing as a "good enough" strategy rather than expending effort to search for patterns. These results suggest that probability maximizing can arise via two routes: recognizing that probabilistic processes cannot be outdone (maximizing as optimizing) or settling for an imperfect but easily implementable strategy (maximizing as satisficing).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christin Schulze
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany.
| | - Wolfgang Gaissmaier
- Department of Psychology and Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany
| | - Ben R Newell
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Pescetelli N, Yeung N. The effects of recursive communication dynamics on belief updating. Proc Biol Sci 2020; 287:20200025. [PMID: 32693730 PMCID: PMC7423656 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2020.0025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2020] [Accepted: 07/01/2020] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Many social interactions are characterized by dynamic interplay, such that individuals exert reciprocal influence over each other's behaviours and beliefs. The present study investigated how the dynamics of reciprocal influence affect individual beliefs in a social context, over and above the information communicated in an interaction. To this end, we developed a simple social decision-making paradigm in which two people are asked to make perceptual judgments while receiving information about each other's decisions. In a Static condition, information about the partner only conveyed their initial, independent judgment. However, in a Dynamic condition, each individual saw the evolving belief of their partner as they learnt about and responded to the individual's own judgment. The results indicated that in both conditions, the majority of confidence adjustments were characterized by an abrupt change followed by smaller adjustments around an equilibrium, and that participants' confidence was used to arbitrate conflict (although deviating from Bayesian norm). Crucially, recursive interaction had systematic effects on belief change relative to the static baseline, magnifying confidence change when partners agreed and reducing confidence change when they disagreed. These findings indicate that during dynamic interactions-often a characteristic of real-life and online social contexts-information is collectively transformed rather than acted upon by individuals in isolation. Consequently, the output of social events is not only influenced by what the dyad knows but also by predictable recursive and self-reinforcing dynamics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Niccolò Pescetelli
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development, 94 Lentzeallee, Berlin 14195, Germany
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Anna Watts Building, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, Woodstock Road, Oxford OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Nick Yeung
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Anna Watts Building, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, Woodstock Road, Oxford OX2 6GG, UK
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Tangen JM, Kent KM, Searston RA. Collective intelligence in fingerprint analysis. Cogn Res Princ Implic 2020; 5:23. [PMID: 32430615 PMCID: PMC7237548 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-020-00223-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2019] [Accepted: 03/30/2020] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
When a fingerprint is located at a crime scene, a human examiner is counted upon to manually compare this print to those stored in a database. Several experiments have now shown that these professional analysts are highly accurate, but not infallible, much like other fields that involve high-stakes decision-making. One method to offset mistakes in these safety-critical domains is to distribute these important decisions to groups of raters who independently assess the same information. This redundancy in the system allows it to continue operating effectively even in the face of rare and random errors. Here, we extend this "wisdom of crowds" approach to fingerprint analysis by comparing the performance of individuals to crowds of professional analysts. We replicate the previous findings that individual experts greatly outperform individual novices, particularly in their false-positive rate, but they do make mistakes. When we pool the decisions of small groups of experts by selecting the decision of the majority, however, their false-positive rate decreases by up to 8% and their false-negative rate decreases by up to 12%. Pooling the decisions of novices results in a similar drop in false negatives, but increases their false-positive rate by up to 11%. Aggregating people's judgements by selecting the majority decision performs better than selecting the decision of the most confident or the most experienced rater. Our results show that combining independent judgements from small groups of fingerprint analysts can improve their performance and prevent these mistakes from entering courts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jason M Tangen
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, 4072, Queensland, Australia.
| | - Kirsty M Kent
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, 4072, Queensland, Australia
| | - Rachel A Searston
- School of Psychology, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide, 5005, South Australia, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
El Zein M, Bahrami B. Joining a group diverts regret and responsibility away from the individual. Proc Biol Sci 2020; 287:20192251. [PMID: 32156213 PMCID: PMC7126069 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2019.2251] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
It has recently been proposed that a key motivation for joining groups is the protection from the negative consequences of undesirable outcomes. To test this claim, we investigated how experienced outcomes triggering loss and regret impacted people's tendency to decide alone or join a group, and how decisions differed when voluntarily made alone versus in group. Replicated across two experiments, participants (n = 125 and n = 496) selected whether to play alone or contribute their vote to a group decision. Next, they chose between two lotteries with different probabilities of winning and losing. The higher the negative outcome, the more participants switched from deciding alone to with others. When joining a group to choose the lottery, choices were less driven by outcome and regret anticipation. Moreover, negative outcomes experienced alone, not part of a group vote, led to worse subsequent choices than positive outcomes. These results suggest that the protective shield of the collective reduces the influence of negative emotions that may help individuals re-evaluate past choices.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marwa El Zein
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, Alexandra House, 17-19 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AZ, UK.,Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany
| | - Bahador Bahrami
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany.,Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Ludwig Maximilian University, Leopoldstrasse 13, 80802 Munich, Germany.,Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, Surrey TW20 0EX, UK
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
El Zein M, Seikus C, De-Wit L, Bahrami B. Punishing the individual or the group for norm violation. Wellcome Open Res 2020; 4:139. [PMID: 32190748 PMCID: PMC7062205 DOI: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15474.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/10/2020] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Background: It has recently been proposed that a key motivation for joining groups is the protection from consequences of negative behaviours, such as norm violations. Here we empirically test this claim by investigating whether cooperative decisions and the punishment of associated fairness-based norm violations are different in individuals vs. collectives in economic games. Methods: In the ultimatum game, participants made or received offers that they could reject at a cost to their outcome, a form of social punishment. In the dictator game with third-party punishment, participants made offers to a receiver while being observed by a punisher, or could themselves punish unfair offers. Results: Participants made lower offers when making their decision as part of a group as compared to alone. This difference correlated with participants' overall mean offers: those who were generally less generous were even less so in a group, suggesting that the collective structure was compatible with their intention. Participants were slower when punishing vs not punishing an unfair offer. Importantly here, they were slower when deciding whether to punish or not to punish groups as compared to individuals, only when the offer concerned them directly in second party punishment. Participants thus take more time to punish others, and to make their mind on whether to punish or not when facing a group of proposers. Conclusions: Together, these results show that people behave differently in a group, both in their willingness to share with others and in their punishment of norm violations. This could be explained by the fact that being in a collective structure allows to share responsibility with others, thereby protecting from negative consequences of norm violations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marwa El Zein
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, WC1N 3AZ, UK
- Centre for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Centre for Human Development, Berlin, 14195, Germany
| | - Chloe Seikus
- Division of Psychology & Language Sciences, University College London, London, WC1H 0AP, UK
| | - Lee De-Wit
- Division of Psychology & Language Sciences, University College London, London, WC1H 0AP, UK
- Department of Psychology, Cambridge University, Cambridge, CB2 3EB, UK
| | - Bahador Bahrami
- Centre for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Centre for Human Development, Berlin, 14195, Germany
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, 80802, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, Surrey, TW20 0EX, UK
| |
Collapse
|
50
|
El Zein M, Seikus C, De-Wit L, Bahrami B. Punishing the individual or the group for norm violation. Wellcome Open Res 2019; 4:139. [PMID: 32190748 PMCID: PMC7062205 DOI: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15474.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/18/2019] [Indexed: 03/29/2024] Open
Abstract
Background: It has recently been proposed that a key motivation for joining groups is the protection from consequences of negative behaviours, such as norm violations. Here we empirically test this claim by investigating whether cooperative decisions and the punishment of associated fairness-based norm violations are different in individuals vs. collectives in economic games. Methods: In the ultimatum game, participants made or received offers that they could reject at a cost to their outcome, a form of social punishment. In the dictator game with third-party punishment, participants made offers to a receiver while being observed by a punisher, or could themselves punish unfair offers. Results: Participants made lower offers when making a collective rather than an individual decision. This difference correlated with participants' overall mean offers: those who were generally less generous were even less so in a group, suggesting that the collective structure was compatible with their intention. Participants were slower when punishing vs not punishing an unfair offer. Importantly here, they were slower when deciding whether to punish groups as compared to individuals, only when the offer concerned them directly in second party punishment. Participants thus seem reluctant to punish others, and even more so when facing a group of proposers. Conclusions: Together, these results show that people behave differently in a group, both in their willingness to share with others and in their punishment of norm violations. This could be explained by the fact that being in a collective structure allows to share responsibility with others, thereby protecting from negative consequences of norm violations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marwa El Zein
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, WC1N 3AZ, UK
- Centre for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Centre for Human Development, Berlin, 14195, Germany
| | - Chloe Seikus
- Division of Psychology & Language Sciences, University College London, London, WC1H 0AP, UK
| | - Lee De-Wit
- Division of Psychology & Language Sciences, University College London, London, WC1H 0AP, UK
- Department of Psychology, Cambridge University, Cambridge, CB2 3EB, UK
| | - Bahador Bahrami
- Centre for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Centre for Human Development, Berlin, 14195, Germany
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, 80802, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, Surrey, TW20 0EX, UK
| |
Collapse
|