1
|
Hodgkiss A, Thomas MSC, Tolmie AK, Farran EK. Associations between spatial skills and physics knowledge in primary school: Spatial skills are more important for conceptual scientific knowledge than for factual scientific knowledge. J Exp Child Psychol 2025; 252:106135. [PMID: 39657299 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106135] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/03/2024] [Revised: 10/28/2024] [Accepted: 11/05/2024] [Indexed: 12/12/2024]
Abstract
Previous research demonstrates an association between spatial ability and science achievement in primary-school-aged children. However, little is known about the mechanisms driving this relationship. We investigated the associations between children's spatial skills and components of physics learning (factual knowledge vs. conceptual knowledge [predictions and explanations]). Participants (N = 103; mean age = 9.6 years) completed a set of spatial tasks based on the "2 × 2" (intrinsic-extrinsic; static-dynamic) model of spatial cognition. They also participated in a whole-class science lesson about sound, followed by an assessment of science knowledge. After controlling for vocabulary and prior knowledge, spatial ability was not associated with factual knowledge scores. However, spatial skills were significantly associated with predictions and explanations; the association was stronger for explanations than for predictions and was driven by intrinsic-dynamic spatial skills. Findings demonstrate that spatial skills are more important for conceptual scientific knowledge than for factual scientific knowledge and further suggest that spatial intervention studies designed to enhance children's science learning should target intrinsic-dynamic spatial skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alex Hodgkiss
- Department of Education, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6PY, UK; Centre for Educational Neuroscience, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, UK
| | - Michael S C Thomas
- Centre for Educational Neuroscience, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, UK; School of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, UK
| | - Andrew K Tolmie
- Centre for Educational Neuroscience, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, UK; Department of Psychology and Human Development, UCL Institute of Education, University College London, London WC1H 0AL, UK
| | - Emily K Farran
- Centre for Educational Neuroscience, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, UK; School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey GU2 7XH, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Morris S, Farran EK, Gilligan-Lee K. Exploring relative strengths in people with Down syndrome: Spatial thinking and its role in mathematics. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 246:105986. [PMID: 38917683 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105986] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/03/2023] [Revised: 04/02/2024] [Accepted: 05/07/2024] [Indexed: 06/27/2024]
Abstract
There is convincing evidence that training spatial abilities leads to improved mathematics performance in typically developing (TD) children. However, a lack of information on mathematical development and spatial-mathematical associations in people with Down syndrome (DS) hinders the translation of these interventions. Here, we established developmental trajectories of mathematics and explored whether spatial ability predicts attainment on different mathematics measures in individuals with DS. Participants with DS (n = 36; ages 9-35 years) and TD children (n = 132; ages 4-11 years) completed three groups of tasks: spatial tasks assessing different subdomains of spatial thinking; mathematics tasks assessing early mathematics skills, mathematical reasoning, arithmetic, and geometry; and IQ tasks. The developmental trajectories of mathematics performance against mental age revealed similar starting points of the trajectories and similar rates of development for DS and TD groups. Furthermore, after controlling for verbal skills, spatial skills explained 5.8% to 18.1% of the variation in mathematical performance across different mathematics tasks, and the pattern of spatial-mathematical relations was similar for DS and mental age-matched TD groups. This shows that mathematical development in DS groups appears to mirror that in TD children, indicative of delay only. Strong spatial-mathematical relations were observed for individuals with DS, like those seen for TD participants. This is the vital preliminary knowledge needed to support the design and use of spatial intervention for improving mathematics in individuals with DS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Su Morris
- School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey GU2 7XH, UK.
| | - Emily K Farran
- School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey GU2 7XH, UK; Centre for Educational Neuroscience, Birkbeck, University of London, London WC1H 0AP, UK
| | - Katie Gilligan-Lee
- School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey GU2 7XH, UK; Centre for Educational Neuroscience, Birkbeck, University of London, London WC1H 0AP, UK; School of Psychology, University College Dublin, Ireland
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Rodriguez KA, Mattox N, Desme C, Hall LV, Wu Y, Pruden SM. Harnessing technology to measure individual differences in spatial thinking in early childhood from a relational developmental systems perspective. ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2024; 67:236-272. [PMID: 39260905 DOI: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2024.07.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/13/2024]
Abstract
According to the Relational Developmental Systems perspective, the development of individual differences in spatial thinking (e.g., mental rotation, spatial reorientation, and spatial language) are attributed to various psychological (e.g., children's cognitive strategies), biological (e.g., structure and function of hippocampus), and cultural systems (e.g., caregiver spatial language input). Yet, measuring the development of individual differences in spatial thinking in young children, as well as the psychological, biological, and cultural systems that influence the development of these abilities, presents unique challenges. The current paper outlines ways to harness available technology including eye-tracking, eye-blink conditioning, MRI, Zoom, and LENA technology, to study the development of individual differences in young children's spatial thinking. The technologies discussed offer ways to examine children's spatial thinking development from different levels of analyses (i.e., psychological, biological, cultural), thereby allowing us to advance the study of developmental theory. We conclude with a discussion of the use of artificial intelligence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karinna A Rodriguez
- Florida International University, Department of Psychology, Miami, FL, United States.
| | - Nick Mattox
- Florida International University, Department of Psychology, Miami, FL, United States
| | - Carlos Desme
- Florida International University, Department of Psychology, Miami, FL, United States
| | - LaTreese V Hall
- Florida International University, Department of Psychology, Miami, FL, United States
| | - Yinbo Wu
- Florida International University, Department of Psychology, Miami, FL, United States
| | - Shannon M Pruden
- Florida International University, Department of Psychology, Miami, FL, United States
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Garcia-Sanchez NL, Dick AS, Hayes T, Pruden SM. Direct and indirect effects of mother's spatial ability on child's spatial ability: What role does the home environment play? Dev Sci 2024; 27:e13480. [PMID: 38321593 PMCID: PMC11610750 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13480] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/22/2020] [Revised: 01/23/2024] [Accepted: 01/24/2024] [Indexed: 02/08/2024]
Abstract
Individual differences in spatial thinking are predictive of children's math and science achievement and later entry into Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM) disciplines. Little is known about whether parent characteristics predict individual differences in children's spatial thinking. This study aims to understand whether, and to what extent, mother's intrinsic (i.e., mental rotation) and extrinsic (i.e., spatial scaling) spatial ability directly and indirectly, via the variation in home spatial environment, predicts children's intrinsic and extrinsic spatial ability. A total of 165 mothers and their 4-6-year-old children were recruited to participate in a remote video session with an experimenter. Mothers were administered a forced-choice Intrinsic Spatial Toy Preference Task gauging their preference for highly spatial versus less spatial toys and asked questions with the Home Intrinsic Spatial Environmental Questionnaire about the frequency with which they engage their child in spatial activities at home. Mothers completed a Mental Rotations Test and a Spatial Scaling Task adapted for adults. Children were administered the Picture Rotation Task, the Spatial Scaling Task, and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Structural equation modeling was used to examine direct and indirect, via home spatial environment and toy choices, influences of mother spatial ability on child spatial ability. Contrary to our predictions, we did not find direct, nor indirect, relations between mother and child spatial ability. These findings suggest that researchers should consider alternative conceptualizations of the early home spatial environment beyond the frequency of spatial play in the home. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: The identification of factors that predict individual differences in children's spatial ability is important in order to maximize STEM learning outcomes. Data collection was conducted remotely rather than in traditional preschool or laboratory settings. Contrary to our pre-registered hypotheses, no significant relations between mother spatial ability, the early home spatial environment, and children's development of spatial skills were found. Future research should consider examining the amount of spatial language used in the home or the quality of parent-child interactions during spatial play as potential explanations for individual differences in children's spatial ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Timothy Hayes
- Florida International University, Miami, Florida, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Szubielska M, Szewczyk M, Augustynowicz P, Kędziora W, Möhring W. Adults' spatial scaling of tactile maps: Insights from studying sighted, early and late blind individuals. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0304008. [PMID: 38814897 PMCID: PMC11139347 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0304008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/25/2023] [Accepted: 05/04/2024] [Indexed: 06/01/2024] Open
Abstract
The current study investigated spatial scaling of tactile maps among blind adults and blindfolded sighted controls. We were specifically interested in identifying spatial scaling strategies as well as effects of different scaling directions (up versus down) on participants' performance. To this aim, we asked late blind participants (with visual memory, Experiment 1) and early blind participants (without visual memory, Experiment 2) as well as sighted blindfolded controls to encode a map including a target and to place a response disc at the same spot on an empty, constant-sized referent space. Maps had five different sizes resulting in five scaling factors (1:3, 1:2, 1:1, 2:1, 3:1), allowing to investigate different scaling directions (up and down) in a single, comprehensive design. Accuracy and speed of learning about the target location as well as responding served as dependent variables. We hypothesized that participants who can use visual mental representations (i.e., late blind and blindfolded sighted participants) may adopt mental transformation scaling strategies. However, our results did not support this hypothesis. At the same time, we predicted the usage of relative distance scaling strategies in early blind participants, which was supported by our findings. Moreover, our results suggested that tactile maps can be scaled as accurately and even faster by blind participants than by sighted participants. Furthermore, irrespective of the visual status, participants of each visual status group gravitated their responses towards the center of the space. Overall, it seems that a lack of visual imagery does not impair early blind adults' spatial scaling ability but causes them to use a different strategy than sighted and late blind individuals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Magdalena Szubielska
- Faculty of Social Sciences, Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Poland
| | - Marta Szewczyk
- Faculty of Social Sciences, Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Poland
| | - Paweł Augustynowicz
- Faculty of Social Sciences, Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Poland
| | | | - Wenke Möhring
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
- Department of Educational and Health Psychology, University of Education Schwäbisch Gmünd, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Orefice C, Cardillo R, Lonciari I, Zoccante L, Mammarella IC. "Picture this from there": spatial perspective-taking in developmental visuospatial disorder and developmental coordination disorder. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1349851. [PMID: 38708023 PMCID: PMC11066165 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1349851] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2023] [Accepted: 04/03/2024] [Indexed: 05/07/2024] Open
Abstract
Introduction Either Developmental Visuospatial Disorder (DVSD) and Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) present with difficulties in visuospatial processing, even though entailing different degrees of impairment. Among the visuospatial domain, spatial perspective taking is essential to interact with the environment and is significantly involved in many daily activities (e.g., environment navigation and spatial orienting). Notwithstanding, no previous studies have investigated this spatial domain in children with DVSD and limited evidence is available regarding DCD. Consistent with a transdiagnostic approach, the first goal of the present study was to compare spatial perspective taking abilities of these groups, also including a control group of not diagnosed peers (ND). Secondly, the role of different fine-motor and visuo-spatial predictors on the spatial perspective taking performance was considered. Method A total of 85 participants (DVSD = 26; DCD = 26; ND = 33), aged between 8 and 16 years old, were included in the study. Tasks assessing spatial perspective taking, fine-motor, visual imagery, and mental rotation skills, as well as visuo-spatial working memory were administered. Results and Discussion Overall, our results confirmed weaknesses in spatial perspective taking in both clinical groups, with the DVSD obtaining the lowest scores. Similarities and differences in the predictors accounting for the performance in the spatial perspective taking task emerged, suggesting the possible employment of different fine-motor or visuospatial strategies by group. Findings are discussed considering the potential impact they may have both in research and clinical practice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Camilla Orefice
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, School of Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
| | - Ramona Cardillo
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, School of Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
- Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, School of Medicine and Surgery, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
| | - Isabella Lonciari
- Division of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, University Pediatric Hospital “IRCCS Burlo Garofolo”, Trieste, Italy
| | - Leonardo Zoccante
- Child and Adolescent Neuropsychiatry Unit, Maternal-Child Integrated Care Department, Integrated University Hospital of Verona, Verona, Italy
| | - Irene C. Mammarella
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, School of Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Möhring W, Szubielska M. Scaling up = scaling down? Children's spatial scaling in different perceptual modalities and scaling directions. Cogn Res Princ Implic 2023; 8:62. [PMID: 37794290 PMCID: PMC10550888 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-023-00517-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2023] [Accepted: 09/23/2023] [Indexed: 10/06/2023] Open
Abstract
The present study examined whether scaling direction and perceptual modality affect children's spatial scaling. Children aged 6-8 years (N = 201) were assigned to a visual, visuo-haptic, and haptic condition in which they were presented with colourful, embossed graphics. In the haptic condition, they were asked to wear a blindfold during the test trials. Across several trials, children were asked to learn about the position of a target in a map and to localize a disc at the same location in a referent space. Scaling factor was manipulated systematically, so that children had to either scale up or scale down spatial information. Their absolute deviations from the correct target location, reversal and signed errors, and response times served as dependent variables. Results revealed higher absolute deviations and response times for the haptic modality as opposed to the visual modality. Children's signed errors, however, showed similar response strategies across the perceptual conditions. Therefore, it seems that a functional equivalence between vision and touch seems to emerge slowly across development for spatial scaling. With respect to scaling directions, findings showed that absolute deviations were affected by scaling factors, with symmetric increases in scaling up and scaling down in the haptic condition. Conversely, children showed an unbalanced pattern in the visual conditions, with higher accuracy in scaling down as opposed to scaling up. Overall, our findings suggest that visibility seems to factor into children's scaling process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wenke Möhring
- Department of Psychology, University of Basel, Missionsstrasse 62, 4055, Basel, Switzerland.
- Department of Educational and Health Psychology, University of Education Schwäbisch Gmünd, Schwäbisch Gmünd, Germany.
| | - Magdalena Szubielska
- Institute of Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Lublin, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Bower CA, Liben LS. Instructors' Gestural Accuracy Affects Geology Learning in Interaction with Students' Spatial Skills. J Intell 2023; 11:192. [PMID: 37888424 PMCID: PMC10607687 DOI: 10.3390/jintelligence11100192] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2023] [Revised: 09/11/2023] [Accepted: 09/27/2023] [Indexed: 10/28/2023] Open
Abstract
Complex and often unobservable STEM constructs and processes are represented using a variety of representations, including iconic gestures in which the body is configured or moved to resemble a referent's spatial properties or actions. Earlier researchers have suggested links between gesturing and expertise, leading some to recommend instructional gestures. Earlier research, however, has been largely correlational; furthermore, some gestures may be made with misleading positions or movements. Using the illustrative topic of strike in structural geology, we investigated the existence and impact of inaccurate instructional gestures. In Study 1, we examined videotapes of participants who had been asked to explain strikes to another person. We observed inaccurate (non-horizontal) strike gestures not only among novices (first introduced to strike during the study itself, n = 68) but also among participants who had greater expertise in geology (n = 21). In Study 2, we randomly assigned novices (N = 167) to watch video lessons in which the instructor accompanied verbal explanations of strikes with accurate, inaccurate, or no iconic gestures and tested students' learning on a strike-mapping task. Students with low spatial-perception skills showed no impact of their gestural condition on performance. Students with high spatial-perception skills showed no advantage from accurate gestures but performed significantly worse in the inaccurate-gesture condition. Findings suggest that recommendations to use gestures during instruction should include professional development programs that reduce the occurrence of inaccurate gestures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Corinne A. Bower
- Department of Psychology, California State University Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90032, USA
| | - Lynn S. Liben
- Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA;
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Dumontheil I, Wilkinson HR, Farran EK, Smid C, Modhvadia R, Mareschal D. How Do Executive Functions Influence Children's Reasoning About Counterintuitive Concepts in Mathematics and Science? JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE ENHANCEMENT 2023; 7:257-275. [PMID: 38186609 PMCID: PMC10770252 DOI: 10.1007/s41465-023-00271-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/13/2023] [Accepted: 08/01/2023] [Indexed: 01/09/2024]
Abstract
Many scientific and mathematical concepts are counterintuitive because they conflict with misleading perceptual cues or incorrect naive theories that we build from our everyday experiences of the world. Executive functions (EFs) influence mathematics and science achievement, and inhibitory control (IC), in particular, might facilitate counterintuitive reasoning. Stop & Think (S&T) is a computerised learning activity that trains IC skills. It has been found effective in improving primary children's mathematics and science academic performance in a large scale RCT trial (Palak et al., 2019; Wilkinson et al., Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 4, 296-314, 2020). The current study aimed to investigate the role of EFs and the moderating effects of S&T training on counterintuitive mathematics and science reasoning. A sample of 372 children in school Years 3 (7- to 8-year-olds) and 5 (9- to 10-year-olds) were allocated to S&T, active control or teaching as usual conditions, and completed tasks assessing verbal and visuospatial working memory (WM), IC, IQ, and counterintuitive reasoning, before and after training. Cross-sectional associations between counterintuitive reasoning and EF were found in Year 5 children, with evidence of a specific role of verbal WM. The intervention benefited counterintuitive reasoning in Year 3 children only and EF measures were not found to predict which children would most benefit from the intervention. Combined with previous research, these results suggest that individual differences in EF play a lesser role in counterintuitive reasoning in younger children, while older children show a greater association between EFs and counterintuitive reasoning and are able to apply the strategies developed during the S&T training to mathematics and science subjects. This work contributes to understanding why specifically the S&T intervention is effective. This work was preregistered with the ISRCTN registry (TRN: 54726482) on 10/10/2017. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s41465-023-00271-0.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iroise Dumontheil
- Centre for Educational Neuroscience, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck College, University of London, Henry Wellcome Building, Malet Street, London, WC1E 7HX UK
| | - Hannah R. Wilkinson
- Centre for Educational Neuroscience, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck College, University of London, Henry Wellcome Building, Malet Street, London, WC1E 7HX UK
| | - Emily K. Farran
- Centre for Educational Neuroscience, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
- School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, UK
| | - Claire Smid
- Centre for Educational Neuroscience, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
- Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Roshni Modhvadia
- Centre for Educational Neuroscience, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
- School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, UK
| | - Denis Mareschal
- Centre for Educational Neuroscience, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck College, University of London, Henry Wellcome Building, Malet Street, London, WC1E 7HX UK
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
İleri Çİ, Erşan M, Kalaça D, Coşkun A, Göksun T, Küntay AC. Malleability of spatial skills: bridging developmental psychology and toy design for joyful STEAM development. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1137003. [PMID: 37771811 PMCID: PMC10523793 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1137003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/03/2023] [Accepted: 08/25/2023] [Indexed: 09/30/2023] Open
Abstract
Previous research has established that advances in spatial cognition predict STEAM success, and construction toys provide ample opportunities to foster spatial cognition. Despite various construction toy designs in the market, mostly brick-shaped building blocks are used in spatial cognition research. This group of toys is known to enhance mental rotation; however, mental rotation is not the only way to comprehend the environment three-dimensionally. More specifically, mental folding and perspective taking training have not received enough attention as they can also be enhanced with the construction toys, which are framed based on the 2×2 classification of spatial skills (intrinsic-static, intrinsic-dynamic, extrinsic-static, extrinsic-dynamic). To address these gaps, we compile evidence from both developmental psychology and toy design fields to show the central role played by mental folding and perspective taking skills as well as the importance of the variety in toy designs. The review was conducted systematically by searching peer reviewed design and psychology journals and conference proceedings. We suggest that, over and above their physical properties, construction toys offer affordances to elicit spatial language, gesture, and narrative among child-caregiver dyads. These interactions are essential for the development of spatial skills in both children and their caregivers. As developmental psychology and toy design fields are two domains that can contribute to the purpose of developing construction toys to boost spatial skills, we put forward six recommendations to bridge the current gaps between these fields. Consequently, new toy designs and empirical evidence regarding malleability of different spatial skills can contribute to the informal STEAM development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Melisa Erşan
- Department of Design and Technology, Parsons School of Design, The New School New York, NY, United States
| | - Duru Kalaça
- Department of Psychology, Koç University, Istanbul, Türkiye
- Department of Media and Visual Arts, Koç University, Istanbul, Türkiye
| | - Aykut Coşkun
- Koç University-Arçelik Research Center for Creative Industries, Koç University, Istanbul, Türkiye
| | - Tilbe Göksun
- Department of Psychology, Koç University, Istanbul, Türkiye
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Szubielska M, Szewczyk M, Augustynowicz P, Kędziora W, Möhring W. Effects of scaling direction on adults' spatial scaling in different perceptual domains. Sci Rep 2023; 13:14690. [PMID: 37673909 PMCID: PMC10482972 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-41533-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2023] [Accepted: 08/28/2023] [Indexed: 09/08/2023] Open
Abstract
The current study investigated adults' strategies of spatial scaling from memory in three perceptual conditions (visual, haptic, and visuo-haptic) when scaling up and down. Following previous research, we predicted the usage of mental transformation strategies. In all conditions, participants (N = 90, aged 19-28 years) were presented with tactile, colored graphics which allowed to visually and haptically explore spatial information. Participants were first asked to encode a map including a target. Then, they were instructed to place a response object at the same place on an empty, constant-sized referent space. Maps had five different sizes resulting in five scaling factors (3:1, 2:1, 1:1, 1:2, 1:3). This manipulation also allowed assessing potentially symmetric effects of scaling direction on adults' responses. Response times and absolute errors served as dependent variables. In line with our hypotheses, the changes in these dependent variables were best explained by a quadratic function which suggests the usage of mental transformation strategies for spatial scaling. There were no differences between perceptual conditions concerning the influence of scaling factor on dependent variables. Results revealed symmetric effects of scaling direction on participants' accuracy whereas there were small differences for response times. Our findings highlight the usage of mental transformation strategies in adults' spatial scaling, irrespective of perceptual modality and scaling direction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Magdalena Szubielska
- Faculty of Social Sciences, Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Lublin, Poland.
| | - Marta Szewczyk
- Faculty of Social Sciences, Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Lublin, Poland
| | - Paweł Augustynowicz
- Faculty of Social Sciences, Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Lublin, Poland
| | | | - Wenke Möhring
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
- Department of Educational and Health Psychology, University of Education Schwäbisch Gmünd, Schwäbisch Gmünd, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Gilligan-Lee KA, Fink E, Jerrom L, Davies MP, Dempsey C, Hughes C, Farran EK. Building Numeracy Skills: Associations between DUPLO ® Block Construction and Numeracy in Early Childhood. J Intell 2023; 11:161. [PMID: 37623544 PMCID: PMC10455495 DOI: 10.3390/jintelligence11080161] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2023] [Revised: 07/24/2023] [Accepted: 08/04/2023] [Indexed: 08/26/2023] Open
Abstract
Research shows that children's block construction skills are positively associated with their concurrent and later mathematics performance. Furthermore, there is evidence that block construction training is particularly beneficial for improving early mathematics skills in children from low-Socio Economic Status (SES) groups who are known to have lower maths performance than their peers. The current study investigates (a) the association between block construction and mathematics in children just before the start of formal schooling (4 years-of-age in the UK) and (b) whether the association between block construction and mathematics differs between children from more compared to less affluent families. Participants in this study included 116 children (M = 3 years 11 months, SD = 3 months) who all completed numeracy, block construction, and receptive vocabulary tasks. Socio-economic status and demographic information (child age, gender, ethnicity) were also obtained from parents. Findings show a strong positive association between block construction and early numeracy skills. Block construction skills explained approximately 5% of the variation in numeracy, even after controlling for age in months, household income, and child receptive vocabulary. When separated by SES group, for children from less affluent families, block construction explained a significant amount of variability (14.5%) in numeracy performance after covariates. For children from more affluent families, block construction did not explain a significant amount of variation in numeracy. These findings suggest that, interventions involving block construction skills may help to reduce SES-based attainment gaps in UK children's mathematics achievement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katie A. Gilligan-Lee
- School of Psychology, University College Dublin, D04 V1W8 Dublin, Ireland
- Centre for Educational Neuroscience, Birkbeck, University of London, London WC1H 0AL, UK
| | - Elian Fink
- School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, East Sussex BN1 9QH, UK
| | - Lewis Jerrom
- School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford GU2 7XH, UK
| | - Megan P. Davies
- School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford GU2 7XH, UK
| | - Caoimhe Dempsey
- Centre for Family Research, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3RQ, UK
| | - Claire Hughes
- Centre for Family Research, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3RQ, UK
| | - Emily K. Farran
- Centre for Educational Neuroscience, Birkbeck, University of London, London WC1H 0AL, UK
- School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford GU2 7XH, UK
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Chen Y, Yang K. Variables for designing cube folding tasks influencing sixth‐graders' performance. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2023. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.4032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/13/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Yun‐Zu Chen
- Taipei Municipal Nanhu High School Taipei Taiwan
| | - Kai‐Lin Yang
- Department of Mathematics National Taiwan Normal University Taipei Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Optimizing the Development of Space-Temporal Orientation in Physical Education and Sports Lessons for Students Aged 8–11 Years. CHILDREN 2022; 9:children9091299. [PMID: 36138608 PMCID: PMC9497162 DOI: 10.3390/children9091299] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2022] [Revised: 08/22/2022] [Accepted: 08/25/2022] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
The purpose of this research was to analyze how we can improve the space–temporal orientation ability with the help of physical exercises in physical education and sports lessons. In total,148 children between the ages of 8 and 11 participated in this study (M = 9.70; SD = 0.79). They were subjected to three tests, which measured general intelligence (Raven Progressive Matrices) and space–temporal orientation skills (Piaget-Head test and Bender–Santucci test). The tests were carried out both in the pre-test and in the post-test period. In the case of participants in the experimental group, a specific program was applied for a period of 12 weeks. The results showed that general intelligence level was identified as a predictor of spatial–temporal orientation (beta = 0.17, t = 2.08, p = 0.03) but only for the Piaget-Head test. Similarly, no differences between children’s age groups were identified in any of the spatial–temporal orientation test scores. However, children in the “+9” age category had higher scores on the intelligence test compared to younger children (77.31 vs. 35.70). In conclusion, the intervention program had a positive effect on spatial orientation skills.
Collapse
|
15
|
Adults’ spatial scaling from memory: Comparing the visual and haptic domain. Mem Cognit 2022; 50:1201-1214. [DOI: 10.3758/s13421-022-01327-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
16
|
Garcia N, Dick AS, Pruden SM. Contributions of Executive Function to Spatial Thinking in Young Children. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2022; 31:e2317. [PMID: 39640003 PMCID: PMC11618850 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2317] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/19/2021] [Accepted: 04/01/2022] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Identifying factors that contribute to spatial thinking is of great interest given links between spatial thinking and success in STEM. Working memory has been found to be predictive of spatial thinking but little research has explored other components of executive function (i.e., inhibition, shifting) in relation to spatial thinking. A total of 131 four- to six-year-olds (M age = 5.06; 53.4% male; 56% Latinx, 18% White, 12% Mixed Race, 5% Asian, and 5% other) were assessed using spatial, executive function, and intelligence tasks. Results show that inhibition, shifting, and working memory are all associated with intrinsic and extrinsic spatial task scores. These results advance developmental theory on spatial thinking and offer a promising route for future interventions in improving spatial ability.
Collapse
|
17
|
Blindfolded adults use mental transformation strategies for spatial scaling of tactile maps. Sci Rep 2022; 12:6275. [PMID: 35428813 PMCID: PMC9012851 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-10401-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2022] [Accepted: 04/07/2022] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
The current study tested strategies of spatial scaling in the haptic domain. Blindfolded adults (N = 31, aged 20–24 years) were presented with an embossed graphic including a target and asked to encode a target location on this map, imagine this map at a given scale, and to localize a target at the same spot on an empty referent space. Maps varied in three different sizes whereas the referent space had a constant size, resulting in three different scaling factors (1:1, 1:2, 1:4). Participants’ response times and localization errors were measured. Analyses indicated that both response times and errors increased with higher scaling factors, suggesting the usage of mental transformation stratergies for spatial scaling. Overall, the present study provides a suitable, novel methodology to assess spatial scaling in the haptic domain.
Collapse
|
18
|
Rocha K, Lussier CM, Atit K. What makes online teaching spatial? Examining the connections between K-12 teachers' spatial skills, affect, and their use of spatial pedagogy during remote instruction. Cogn Res Princ Implic 2022; 7:25. [PMID: 35312888 PMCID: PMC8935615 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-022-00377-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2021] [Accepted: 02/26/2022] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
Spatial skills are critical for student success in K-12 STEM education. Teachers' spatial skills and feelings about completing spatial tasks influence students' spatial and STEM learning at both the primary and secondary levels. However, whether spatial skills and spatial anxiety differ or not between these two teacher levels is unknown. Additionally, the relations between teachers' spatial skills, spatial anxiety, and their use of spatial pedagogical practices in remote learning settings is unknown. Here, we investigated if spatial skills and spatial anxiety differ between teachers working at primary versus secondary levels, and examined the relations between their spatial skills and spatial anxiety while accounting for additional influential factors-general reasoning ability and general anxiety. Lastly, we investigated how teachers' spatial skills in conjunction with their spatial anxiety relate to their use of spatial teaching practices for online instruction. Sixty-two K-12 teachers completed measures of spatial skills, spatial anxiety, general anxiety, general reasoning, and a teaching activities questionnaire. Results indicate that spatial skills and spatial anxiety may not vary between teachers working at primary versus secondary levels, but that higher spatial skills in teachers are associated with lower spatial anxiety for mental manipulation tasks. Additionally, teachers with weaker spatial skills and lower mental manipulation anxiety reported more frequently using spatial teaching practices when teaching remotely due to COVID-19. These findings may have broad implications for teacher professional development with regards to developing students' spatial skills during remote learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kelsey Rocha
- School of Education, University of California, Riverside, 1207 Sproul Hall, Riverside, CA, 92521, USA
| | - Catherine M Lussier
- School of Education, University of California, Riverside, 1207 Sproul Hall, Riverside, CA, 92521, USA
| | - Kinnari Atit
- School of Education, University of California, Riverside, 1207 Sproul Hall, Riverside, CA, 92521, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Brelsford VL, Dimolareva M, Rowan E, Gee NR, Meints K. Can dog-assisted and relaxation interventions boost spatial ability in children with and without special educational needs? A longitudinal, randomized controlled trial. Front Pediatr 2022; 10:886324. [PMID: 35979404 PMCID: PMC9376734 DOI: 10.3389/fped.2022.886324] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2022] [Accepted: 07/08/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Children's spatial cognition abilities are a vital part of their learning and cognitive development, and important for their problem-solving capabilities, the development of mathematical skills and progress in Science, Technology, Engineering and Maths (STEM) topics. As many children have difficulties with STEM topic areas, and as these topics have suffered a decline in uptake in students, it is worthwhile to find out how learning and performance can be enhanced at an early age. The current study is the first to investigate if dog-assisted and relaxation interventions can improve spatial abilities in school children. It makes a novel contribution to empirical research by measuring longitudinally if an Animal-Assisted Intervention (AAI) or relaxation intervention can boost children's development of spatial abilities. Randomized controlled trials were employed over time including dog intervention, relaxation intervention and no treatment control groups. Interventions were carried out over 4 weeks, twice a week for 20 min. Children were tested in mainstream schools (N = 105) and in special educational needs (SEN) schools (N = 64) before and after interventions, after 6 weeks, 6 months and 1 year. To assess intervention type and to provide advice for subsequent best practice recommendations, dog-assisted interventions were run as individual or small group interventions. Overall, children's spatial abilities improved over the year with highest increases in the first 4 months. In Study 1, typically developing children showed higher scores and more continuous learning overall compared to children with special educational needs. Children in the dog intervention group showed higher spatial ability scores immediately after interventions and after a further 6 weeks (short-term). Children in the relaxation group also showed improved scores short-term after relaxation intervention. In contrast, the no treatment control group did not improve significantly. No long-term effects were observed. Interestingly, no gender differences could be observed in mainstream school children's spatial skills. In study 2, children in SEN schools saw immediate improvements in spatial abilities after relaxation intervention sessions. No changes were seen after dog interventions or in the no treatment control group. Participants' pet ownership status did not have an effect in either cohort. These are the first findings showing that AAI and relaxation interventions benefit children's spatial abilities in varied educational settings. This research represents an original contribution to Developmental Psychology and to the field of Human-Animal Interaction (HAI) and is an important step towards further in-depth investigation of how AAI and relaxation interventions can help children achieve their learning potential, both in mainstream schools and in schools for SEN.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Mirena Dimolareva
- School of Science, Department of Psychology, Bath Spa University, Bath, United Kingdom
| | - Elise Rowan
- School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, United Kingdom
| | - Nancy R Gee
- Centre for Human-Animal Interaction, School of Medicine, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, United States
| | - Kerstin Meints
- School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Hodgkiss A, Gilligan-Lee KA, Thomas MSC, Tolmie AK, Farran EK. The developmental trajectories of spatial skills in middle childhood. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2021; 39:566-583. [PMID: 34003512 PMCID: PMC8519147 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12380] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2020] [Revised: 02/07/2021] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The multidimensional structure of spatial ability remains a debated issue. However, the developmental trajectories of spatial skills have yet to be investigated as a source of evidence within this debate. We tested the intrinsic versus extrinsic and static versus dynamic dimensions of the Uttal et al. (2013, Psychol. Bull., 139, 352) typology in relation to spatial development. Participants (N = 184) aged 6–11 completed spatial tasks chosen to measure these spatial dimensions. The results indicated that the developmental trajectories of intrinsic versus extrinsic skills differed significantly. Intrinsic skills improved more between 6 and 8 years, and 7 and 8 years, than extrinsic skills. Extrinsic skills increased more between 8 and 10 years than intrinsic skills. The trajectories of static versus dynamic skills did not differ significantly. The findings support the intrinsic versus extrinsic, but not the static versus dynamic dimension, of the Uttal et al. (2013, Psychol. Bull., 139, 352) typology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Michael S C Thomas
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, UK
| | - Andrew K Tolmie
- Department of Psychology and Human Development, UCL Institute of Education, UCL, London, UK
| | - Emily K Farran
- School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Liu S, Wei W, Chen Y, Hugo P, Zhao J. Visual-Spatial Ability Predicts Academic Achievement Through Arithmetic and Reading Abilities. Front Psychol 2021; 11:591308. [PMID: 33897506 PMCID: PMC8063904 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.591308] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2020] [Accepted: 11/27/2020] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
This study aimed to investigate how visual–spatial ability predicted academic achievement through arithmetic and reading abilities. Four hundred and ninety-nine Chinese children aged from 10.1 to 11.2 years were recruited and measured visual–spatial, arithmetic, and reading abilities. Their mathematical and Chinese language academic achievements were collected for two consecutive school years, respectively, during the same year as cognitive tests and 1 year after the cognitive tests. Correlation analysis indicated that visual–spatial, arithmetic, and reading abilities and academic achievements were significantly correlated with each other. The structural equation modelling analyses showed that there were two paths from visual–spatial ability to academic achievement: a major path mediated by arithmetic ability and a minor serial mediation path from visual–spatial ability to arithmetic ability to reading ability, then to academic achievement. Results shed light on the importance of visual–spatial ability in education.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Saifang Liu
- School of Psychology, Shaanxi Provincial Key Research Center of Child Mental and Behavioral Health, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, China
| | - Wenjun Wei
- School of Psychology, Shaanxi Provincial Key Research Center of Child Mental and Behavioral Health, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, China
| | - Yuan Chen
- School of Psychology, Shaanxi Provincial Key Research Center of Child Mental and Behavioral Health, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, China.,Center for Mental Health Development and Research, Xihua University, Chengdu, China
| | - Peyre Hugo
- Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique (ENS, EHESS, CNRS), Département d'Etudes Cognitives, Ecole Normale Supérieure, PSL Research University, Paris, France.,INSERM UMRS, Paris Diderot University, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France.,Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Robert Debré Hospital, APHP, Paris, France
| | - Jingjing Zhao
- School of Psychology, Shaanxi Provincial Key Research Center of Child Mental and Behavioral Health, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, China
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Bower CA, Liben LS. Can a Domain‐General Spatial Intervention Facilitate Children’s Science Learning? A Lesson From Astronomy. Child Dev 2020; 92:76-100. [DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13439] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
23
|
Harris D, Lowrie T, Logan T, Hegarty M. Spatial reasoning, mathematics, and gender: Do spatial constructs differ in their contribution to performance? BRITISH JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 2020; 91:409-441. [PMID: 32730695 DOI: 10.1111/bjep.12371] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2019] [Revised: 05/19/2020] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The role of gender in both spatial and mathematics performance has been extensively studied separately, with a male advantage often found in spatial tasks and mathematics from adolescence. Spatial reasoning is consistently linked to mathematics proficiency, yet despite this, little research has investigated the role of spatial orientation and gender in the relationship between spatial reasoning and mathematics. AIMS In the present study, three spatial reasoning constructs (mental rotation, spatial visualization, and spatial orientation) were examined for their unique contributions to mathematics performance in two samples (Study 1: grade 5; Study 2: grade 8). In light of the emerging gender gap in mathematics as children develop, these relationships were explored as a function of gender. SAMPLE Eighty-four fifth-grade students participated in Study 1 (43 females, 41 males; mean age = 11.19 years). Nine hundred and three eighth-grade students participated in Study 2 (498 females, 405 males; mean age = 13.83 years). METHODS The three spatial reasoning constructs (mental rotation, spatial visualization, and spatial orientation) were examined for their unique contributions to mathematics performance for females and males in general and across different mathematical content (geometry-measurement and number sense). RESULTS Spatial factors accounted for 51% of the variance in math scores in Study 1 (grade 5) and 32% of the variance in math scores in Study 2 (grade 8). In both studies, spatial factors predicted a larger proportion of variance in geometry-measurement than for number sense. Spatial orientation was found to be a unique contributor in all mathematics models, object-based spatial skills (mental rotation and spatial visualization) varied in their contribution to math performance depending on mathematics content and gender. CONCLUSIONS The present work highlights the unique contribution of spatial orientation in the spatial-mathematics relationship and provides insights into the nature of gender differences in mathematical problem-solving as a function of spatial reasoning and mathematics content.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Danielle Harris
- University of Canberra, Bruce, Australian Capital Territory, Australia
| | - Tom Lowrie
- University of Canberra, Bruce, Australian Capital Territory, Australia
| | - Tracy Logan
- University of Canberra, Bruce, Australian Capital Territory, Australia
| | - Mary Hegarty
- University of California Santa Barbara, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Jung S, Meinhardt A, Braeuning D, Roesch S, Cornu V, Pazouki T, Schiltz C, Lonnemann J, Moeller K. Hierarchical Development of Early Visual-Spatial Abilities - A Taxonomy Based Assessment Using the MaGrid App. Front Psychol 2020; 11:871. [PMID: 32508712 PMCID: PMC7251177 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00871] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/12/2019] [Accepted: 04/07/2020] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Visual-spatial abilities (VSA) are considered a building block of early numerical development. They are intuitively acquired in early childhood and differentiate in further development. However, when children enter school, there already are considerable individual differences in children's visual-spatial and numerical abilities. To better understand this diversity, it is necessary to empirically evaluate the development as well as the latent structure of early VSA as proposed by the 2 by 2 taxonomy of Newcombe and Shipley (2015). In the present study, we report on a tablet-based assessment of VSA using the digital application (app) MaGrid in kindergarten children aged 4-6 years. We investigated whether the visual-spatial tasks implemented in MaGrid are sensitive to replicate previously observed age differences in VSA and thus a hierarchical development of VSA. Additionally, we evaluated whether the selected tasks conform to the taxonomy of VSA by Newcombe and Shipley (2015) applying a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) approach. Our results indicated that the hierarchical development of VSA can be measured using MaGrid. Furthermore, the CFA substantiated the hypothesized factor structure of VSA in line with the dimensions proposed in the taxonomy of Newcombe and Shipley (2015). Taken together, the present results advance our knowledge to the (hierarchical) development as well as the latent structure of early VSA in kindergarten children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stefanie Jung
- Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien, Tübingen, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Eberhard Karls University Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Anna Meinhardt
- Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien, Tübingen, Germany
- DIPF Leibniz Institute for Research and Information in Education, Frankfurt, Germany
- Center for Individual Development and Adaptive Education of Children at Risk (IDeA), Frankfurt, Germany
| | - David Braeuning
- Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien, Tübingen, Germany
- LEAD Graduate School and Research Network, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
- Hector Research Institute of Education Sciences and Psychology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | | | - Véronique Cornu
- Luxembourg Centre for Educational Testing (LUCET), University of Luxembourg, Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg
| | - Tahereh Pazouki
- Luxembourg Centre for Educational Testing (LUCET), University of Luxembourg, Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg
| | - Christine Schiltz
- Department of Behavioral and Cognitive Sciences (DBCS), University of Luxembourg, Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg
| | - Jan Lonnemann
- Empirical Childhood Research, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
| | - Korbinian Moeller
- Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien, Tübingen, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Eberhard Karls University Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
- Center for Individual Development and Adaptive Education of Children at Risk (IDeA), Frankfurt, Germany
- LEAD Graduate School and Research Network, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
- Centre for Mathematical Cognition, School of Science, Loughborough University, Loughborough, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Hund AM, Plumert JM, Recker KM. Visually Scaling Distance from Memory: Do Visible Midline Boundaries Make a Difference? SPATIAL COGNITION AND COMPUTATION 2020; 20:134-159. [PMID: 35082551 DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2020.1734601] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
We examined how 4- to 5-year-old children and adults use perceptual structure (visible midline boundaries) to visually scale distance. Participants completed scaling and no scaling tasks using learning and test mats that were 16 and 64 inches. No boundaries were present in Experiment 1. Children and adults had more difficulty in the scaling than no scaling task when the test mat was 64 inches but not 16 inches. Experiment 2 was identical except visible midline boundaries were present. Again, participants had more difficulty in the scaling than no scaling task when the test mat was 64 inches, suggesting they used the test mat edges (not the midline boundary) as perceptual anchors when scaling from the learning to the test mat.
Collapse
|
26
|
Szubielska M, Möhring W. Adults' spatial scaling: evidence from the haptic domain. Cogn Process 2019; 20:431-440. [PMID: 31054026 PMCID: PMC6841643 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-019-00920-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2018] [Accepted: 04/26/2019] [Indexed: 11/02/2022]
Abstract
The current study investigated adults' spatial-scaling abilities using a haptic localization task. As a first aim, we examined the strategies used to solve this haptic task. Secondly, we explored whether irrelevant visual information influenced adults' spatial-scaling performance. Thirty-two adults were asked to locate targets as presented in maps on a larger or same-sized referent space. Maps varied in size in accordance with different scaling factors (1:4, 1:2, 1:1), whereas the referent space was constant in size throughout the experimental session. The availability of irrelevant, non-informative vision was manipulated by blindfolding half of the participants prior to the experiment (condition without non-informative vision), whereas the other half were able to see their surroundings with the stimuli being hidden behind a curtain (condition with non-informative vision). Analyses with absolute errors (after correcting for reversal errors) as the dependent variable revealed a significant interaction of the scaling factor and non-informative vision condition. Adults in the blindfolded condition showed constant errors and response times irrespective of scaling factor. Such a response pattern indicates the usage of relative strategies. Adults in the curtain condition showed a linear increase in errors with higher scaling factors, whereas their response times remained constant. This pattern of results supports the usage of absolute strategies or mental transformation strategies. Overall, our results indicate different scaling strategies depending on the availability of non-informative vision, highlighting the strong influence of (even irrelevant) vision on adults' haptic processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Magdalena Szubielska
- Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Al. Racławickie 14, 20-950, Lublin, Poland.
| | - Wenke Möhring
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Basel, Missionsstrasse 60/62, 4055, Basel, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Gilligan KA, Thomas MSC, Farran EK. First demonstration of effective spatial training for near transfer to spatial performance and far transfer to a range of mathematics skills at 8 years. Dev Sci 2019; 23:e12909. [PMID: 31599470 PMCID: PMC7379338 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12909] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2018] [Revised: 08/21/2019] [Accepted: 09/23/2019] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
There is evidence that spatial thinking is malleable, and that spatial and mathematical skills are associated (Mix et al. [2016] Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 1206; Mix et al. [2017] Journal of Cognition and Development, 18, 465; Uttal et al. [2013] Psychological Bulletin, 139, 352). However, few studies have investigated transfer of spatial training gains to mathematics outcomes in children, and no known studies have compared different modes of spatial instruction (explicit vs. implicit instruction). Based on a sample of 250 participants, this study compared the effectiveness of explicit and implicit spatial instruction in eliciting near transfer (to the specific spatial skills trained), intermediate transfer (to untrained spatial skills) and far transfer (to mathematics domains) at age 8. Spatial scaling and mental rotation skills were chosen as training targets as previous studies have found, and proposed explanations for, associations between these skills and mathematics in children of this age (Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 2016 and 1206). In this study, spatial training led to near, intermediate and far transfer of gains. Mental visualization and proportional reasoning were proposed to explain far transfer from mental rotation and spatial scaling skills respectively. For most outcomes, except for geometry, there was no difference in the effectiveness of implicit (practice with feedback) compared to explicit instruction (instructional videos). From a theoretical perspective, the study identified a specific causal effect of spatial skills on mathematics skills in children. Practically, the results also highlight the potential of instructional videos as a method of introducing spatial thinking into the classroom.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katie A Gilligan
- School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guilford, UK.,Department of Psychology and Human Development, UCL Institute of Education, University College London, London, UK
| | - Michael S C Thomas
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
| | - Emily K Farran
- School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guilford, UK.,Department of Psychology and Human Development, UCL Institute of Education, University College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Träff U, Olsson L, Skagerlund K, Skagenholt M, Östergren R. Logical Reasoning, Spatial Processing, and Verbal Working Memory: Longitudinal Predictors of Physics Achievement at Age 12-13 Years. Front Psychol 2019; 10:1929. [PMID: 31496982 PMCID: PMC6712504 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01929] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2019] [Accepted: 08/06/2019] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
To date, few studies have tried to pinpoint the mechanisms supporting children’s skills in science. This study investigated to what extent logical reasoning, spatial processing, and working memory, tapped at age 9–10 years, are predictive of physics skills at age 12–13 years. The study used a sample of 81 children (37 girls). Measures of arithmetic calculation and reading comprehension were also included in the study. The multiple regression model accounted for 24% of the variation in physics achievement. The model showed that spatial processing (4.6%) and verbal working memory (4.5%) accounted for a similar amount of unique variance, while logical reasoning accounted for 5.7% variance. The measures of arithmetic calculation and reading comprehension did not account for any unique variance. Nine percent of the accounted variance was shared variance. The results demonstrate that physics is a multivariate discipline that draws upon numerous cognitive resources. Logical reasoning ability is a key component in order for children to learn about abstract physics facts, concepts, theories, and applying complex scientific methods. Spatial processing is important as it may sub-serve the assembly of diverse sources of visual-spatial information into a spatial-schematic image. The working memory system provides a flexible and efficient mental workspace that can supervise, coordinate, and execute processes involved in physics problem-solving.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ulf Träff
- Department of Behavioural Sciences and Learning, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
| | - Linda Olsson
- Department of Behavioural Sciences and Learning, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
| | - Kenny Skagerlund
- Department of Behavioural Sciences and Learning, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
| | - Mikael Skagenholt
- Department of Behavioural Sciences and Learning, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
| | - Rickard Östergren
- Department of Behavioural Sciences and Learning, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
|
30
|
Brookman‐Byrne A, Mareschal D, Tolmie AK, Dumontheil I. The Unique Contributions of Verbal Analogical Reasoning and Nonverbal Matrix Reasoning to Science and Maths Problem-Solving in Adolescence. MIND, BRAIN AND EDUCATION : THE OFFICIAL JOURNAL OF THE INTERNATIONAL MIND, BRAIN, AND EDUCATION SOCIETY 2019; 13:211-223. [PMID: 32362934 PMCID: PMC7189624 DOI: 10.1111/mbe.12212] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2018] [Revised: 06/25/2019] [Accepted: 07/03/2019] [Indexed: 05/14/2023]
Abstract
Relational reasoning, the ability to detect meaningful patterns, matures through adolescence. The unique contributions of verbal analogical and nonverbal matrix relational reasoning to science and maths are not well understood. Functional magnetic resonance imaging data were collected during science and maths problem-solving, and participants (N = 36, 11-15 years) also completed relational reasoning and executive function tasks. Higher verbal analogical reasoning associated with higher accuracy and faster reaction times in science and maths, and higher activation in the left anterior temporal cortex during maths problem-solving. Higher nonverbal matrix reasoning associated with higher science accuracy, higher science activation in regions across the brain, and lower maths activation in the right middle temporal gyrus. Science associations mostly remained significant when individual differences in executive functions and verbal IQ were taken into account, while maths associations typically did not. The findings indicate the potential importance of supporting relational reasoning in adolescent science and maths learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Annie Brookman‐Byrne
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Department of Psychological SciencesBirkbeck, University of London
- Centre for Educational NeuroscienceUniversity of London
| | - Denis Mareschal
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Department of Psychological SciencesBirkbeck, University of London
- Centre for Educational NeuroscienceUniversity of London
| | - Andrew K. Tolmie
- Centre for Educational NeuroscienceUniversity of London
- Department of Psychology and Human Development, UCL Institute of EducationUniversity College London
| | - Iroise Dumontheil
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Department of Psychological SciencesBirkbeck, University of London
- Centre for Educational NeuroscienceUniversity of London
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Szubielska M, Möhring W, Szewczyk M. Spatial scaling in congenitally blind and sighted individuals: similarities and differences. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2019. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2019.1624554] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Magdalena Szubielska
- Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Lublin, Poland
| | - Wenke Möhring
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
| | - Marta Szewczyk
- Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Lublin, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Is scaling up harder than scaling down? How children and adults visually scale distance from memory. Cognition 2019; 185:39-48. [PMID: 30641468 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.12.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2018] [Revised: 12/18/2018] [Accepted: 12/20/2018] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
In three experiments (N = 288), we examined how the direction of the scale translation impacts how 4- to 5-year-old children and adults visually scale distance from memory. Participants first watched an experimenter place an object on a learning mat and then attempted to place a replica object on a test mat that was either identical (no scaling task) or different in scale (scaling task). In Experiment 1, both children and adults had difficulty scaling up from 16 to 128 in. (1:8 scaling ratio) but not scaling down from 128 to 16 in. (8:1 scaling ratio), suggesting that scaling up was harder than scaling down. In Experiment 2, we reduced the scaling ratio from 1:8 to 1:2 and found that children and adults had no difficulty scaling up from 16 to 32 in. or scaling down from 32 to 16 in.. In Experiment 3, we kept the scale ratio the same (1:2) but increased the size of the test mat and found that participants had difficulty with both scaling up from 32 to 64 in. and scaling down from 128 to 64 in.. We conclude that scaling up is not harder than scaling down. Rather, visually scaling distance is more difficult when participants cannot view both edges of the test mat simultaneously while making the scale translation. Across all experiments, 4- to 5-year-olds were less accurate than adults in their placements overall, but they exhibited the same patterns of performance on the scaling and no scaling tasks, suggesting that visual scaling processes are age-independent. The General Discussion focuses on how visual scaling emerges from a complex interplay of cognitive processes and visual constraints.
Collapse
|
33
|
A Heuristic Framework of Spatial Ability: a Review and Synthesis of Spatial Factor Literature to Support its Translation into STEM Education. EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 2018. [DOI: 10.1007/s10648-018-9432-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
|