1
|
Boublil BL, Yu C, Shewmaker G, Sterbing S, Moss CF. Ventral wing hairs provide tactile feedback for aerial prey capture in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2024; 210:761-770. [PMID: 38097720 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01682-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/13/2023] [Revised: 11/07/2023] [Accepted: 11/11/2023] [Indexed: 09/10/2024]
Abstract
Bats rely on their hand-wings to execute agile flight maneuvers, to grasp objects, and cradle young. Embedded in the dorsal and ventral membranes of bat wings are microscopic hairs. Past research findings implicate dorsal wing hairs in airflow sensing for flight control, but the function of ventral wing hairs has not been previously investigated. Here, we test the hypothesis that ventral wing hairs carry mechanosensory signals for flight control, prey capture, and handling. To test this hypothesis, we used synchronized high-speed stereo video and audio recordings to quantify flight and echolocation behaviors of big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) engaged in an aerial insect capture task. We analyzed prey-capture strategy and performance, along with flight kinematics, before and after depilation of microscopic hairs from the bat's ventral wing and tail membranes. We found that ventral wing hair depilation significantly impaired the bat's prey-capture performance. Interestingly, ventral wing hair depilation also produced increases in the bat's flight speed, an effect previously attributed exclusively to airflow sensing along the dorsal wing surface. These findings demonstrate that microscopic hairs embedded in the ventral wing and tail membranes of insectivorous bats provide mechanosensory feedback for prey handling and flight control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brittney L Boublil
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, 3400 N Charles St., Ames 200B, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA
- Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, 92612, USA
| | - Chao Yu
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, 3400 N Charles St., Ames 200B, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA
- Nanjing Research Institute of Electronic Technology, Nanjing, Jiangsu, China
| | - Grant Shewmaker
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, 3400 N Charles St., Ames 200B, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA
| | - Susanne Sterbing
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, 3400 N Charles St., Ames 200B, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA
| | - Cynthia F Moss
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, 3400 N Charles St., Ames 200B, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA.
- Department of Neuroscience, Kavli Neuroscience Discovery Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA.
- Department of Mechanical Engineering, Kavli Neuroscience Discovery Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Moss CF, Ortiz ST, Wahlberg M. Adaptive echolocation behavior of bats and toothed whales in dynamic soundscapes. J Exp Biol 2023; 226:jeb245450. [PMID: 37161774 PMCID: PMC10184770 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.245450] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/11/2023]
Abstract
Journal of Experimental Biology has a long history of reporting research discoveries on animal echolocation, the subject of this Centenary Review. Echolocating animals emit intense sound pulses and process echoes to localize objects in dynamic soundscapes. More than 1100 species of bats and 70 species of toothed whales rely on echolocation to operate in aerial and aquatic environments, respectively. The need to mitigate acoustic clutter and ambient noise is common to both aerial and aquatic echolocating animals, resulting in convergence of many echolocation features, such as directional sound emission and hearing, and decreased pulse intervals and sound intensity during target approach. The physics of sound transmission in air and underwater constrains the production, detection and localization of sonar signals, resulting in differences in response times to initiate prey interception by aerial and aquatic echolocating animals. Anti-predator behavioral responses of prey pursued by echolocating animals affect behavioral foraging strategies in air and underwater. For example, many insect prey can detect and react to bat echolocation sounds, whereas most fish and squid are unresponsive to toothed whale signals, but can instead sense water movements generated by an approaching predator. These differences have implications for how bats and toothed whales hunt using echolocation. Here, we consider the behaviors used by echolocating mammals to (1) track and intercept moving prey equipped with predator detectors, (2) interrogate dynamic sonar scenes and (3) exploit visual and passive acoustic stimuli. Similarities and differences in animal sonar behaviors underwater and in air point to open research questions that are ripe for exploration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cynthia F. Moss
- Johns Hopkins University, Departments of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Neuroscience and Mechanical Engineering, 3400 N. Charles St., Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
- Kavli Neuroscience Discovery Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
| | - Sara Torres Ortiz
- Marine Biological Research Center, University of Southern Denmark, Hindsholmvej 11, 5300 Kerteminde, Denmark
| | - Magnus Wahlberg
- Marine Biological Research Center, University of Southern Denmark, Hindsholmvej 11, 5300 Kerteminde, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Wohlgemuth M, Salles A, Moss C. Spatial attention in natural tasks [version 1; peer review: 2 approved with reservations]. MOLECULAR PSYCHOLOGY 2022; 1:4. [PMID: 37325441 PMCID: PMC10269881 DOI: 10.12688/molpsychol.17488.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/17/2023]
Abstract
Little is known about fine scale neural dynamics that accompany rapid shifts in spatial attention in freely behaving animals, primarily because reliable indicators of attention are lacking in standard model organisms engaged in natural tasks. The echolocating bat can serve to bridge this gap, as it exhibits robust dynamic behavioral indicators of overt spatial attention as it explores its environment. In particular, the bat actively shifts the aim of its sonar beam to inspect objects in different directions, akin to eye movements and foveation in humans and other visually dominant animals. Further, the bat adjusts the temporal features of sonar calls to attend to objects at different distances, yielding a metric of acoustic gaze along the range axis. Thus, an echolocating bat's call features not only convey the information it uses to probe its surroundings, but also provide fine scale metrics of auditory spatial attention in 3D natural tasks. These explicit metrics of overt spatial attention can be leveraged to uncover general principles of neural coding in the mammalian brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Angeles Salles
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL, 60607, USA
| | - Cynthia Moss
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA
- Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA
- Department of Mechanical Engineering, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Sarel A, Palgi S, Blum D, Aljadeff J, Las L, Ulanovsky N. Natural switches in behaviour rapidly modulate hippocampal coding. Nature 2022; 609:119-127. [PMID: 36002570 PMCID: PMC9433324 DOI: 10.1038/s41586-022-05112-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/13/2021] [Accepted: 07/14/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Throughout their daily lives, animals and humans often switch between different behaviours. However, neuroscience research typically studies the brain while the animal is performing one behavioural task at a time, and little is known about how brain circuits represent switches between different behaviours. Here we tested this question using an ethological setting: two bats flew together in a long 135 m tunnel, and switched between navigation when flying alone (solo) and collision avoidance as they flew past each other (cross-over). Bats increased their echolocation click rate before each cross-over, indicating attention to the other bat1–9. Hippocampal CA1 neurons represented the bat’s own position when flying alone (place coding10–14). Notably, during cross-overs, neurons switched rapidly to jointly represent the interbat distance by self-position. This neuronal switch was very fast—as fast as 100 ms—which could be revealed owing to the very rapid natural behavioural switch. The neuronal switch correlated with the attention signal, as indexed by echolocation. Interestingly, the different place fields of the same neuron often exhibited very different tuning to interbat distance, creating a complex non-separable coding of position by distance. Theoretical analysis showed that this complex representation yields more efficient coding. Overall, our results suggest that during dynamic natural behaviour, hippocampal neurons can rapidly switch their core computation to represent the relevant behavioural variables, supporting behavioural flexibility. During rapid behavioural switches in flying bats, hippocampal neurons can rapidly switch their core computation to represent the relevant behavioural variables, supporting behavioural flexibility.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ayelet Sarel
- Department of Brain Sciences, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel
| | - Shaked Palgi
- Department of Brain Sciences, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel
| | - Dan Blum
- Department of Brain Sciences, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel
| | - Johnatan Aljadeff
- Department of Brain Sciences, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel.,Department of Neurobiology, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Liora Las
- Department of Brain Sciences, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel.
| | - Nachum Ulanovsky
- Department of Brain Sciences, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Wen C, Horiuchi TK. The Curved Openspace Algorithm and a Spike-Latency Model for Sonar-Based Obstacle Avoidance. Front Neurorobot 2022; 16:850013. [PMID: 35721278 PMCID: PMC9198458 DOI: 10.3389/fnbot.2022.850013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2022] [Accepted: 05/04/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The rapid control of a sonar-guided vehicle to pursue a goal while avoiding obstacles has been a persistent research topic for decades. Taking into account the limited field-of-view of practical sonar systems and vehicle kinematics, we propose a neural model for obstacle avoidance that maps the 2-D sensory space into a 1-D motor space and evaluates motor actions while combining obstacles and goal information. A two-stage winner-take-all (WTA) mechanism is used to select the final steering action. To avoid excessive scanning of the environment, an attentional system is proposed to control the directions of sonar pings for efficient, task-driven, sensory data collection. A mobile robot was used to test the proposed model navigating through a cluttered environment using a narrow field-of-view sonar system. We further propose a spiking neural model using spike-timing representations, a spike-latency memory, and a “race-to-first-spike” WTA circuit.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chenxi Wen
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, United States
| | - Timothy K. Horiuchi
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, United States
- Neuroscience and Cognitive Sciences Program, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, United States
- *Correspondence: Timothy K. Horiuchi
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Beetz MJ, Hechavarría JC. Neural Processing of Naturalistic Echolocation Signals in Bats. Front Neural Circuits 2022; 16:899370. [PMID: 35664459 PMCID: PMC9157489 DOI: 10.3389/fncir.2022.899370] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/18/2022] [Accepted: 04/21/2022] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Echolocation behavior, a navigation strategy based on acoustic signals, allows scientists to explore neural processing of behaviorally relevant stimuli. For the purpose of orientation, bats broadcast echolocation calls and extract spatial information from the echoes. Because bats control call emission and thus the availability of spatial information, the behavioral relevance of these signals is undiscussable. While most neurophysiological studies, conducted in the past, used synthesized acoustic stimuli that mimic portions of the echolocation signals, recent progress has been made to understand how naturalistic echolocation signals are encoded in the bat brain. Here, we review how does stimulus history affect neural processing, how spatial information from multiple objects and how echolocation signals embedded in a naturalistic, noisy environment are processed in the bat brain. We end our review by discussing the huge potential that state-of-the-art recording techniques provide to gain a more complete picture on the neuroethology of echolocation behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M. Jerome Beetz
- Zoology II, Biocenter, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Julio C. Hechavarría
- Institute of Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Tuninetti A, Ming C, Hom KN, Simmons JA, Simmons AM. Spatiotemporal patterning of acoustic gaze in echolocating bats navigating gaps in clutter. iScience 2021; 24:102353. [PMID: 33870143 PMCID: PMC8047172 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2021.102353] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/24/2021] [Revised: 03/07/2021] [Accepted: 03/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
We challenged four big brown bats to maneuver through abrupt turns in narrow corridors surrounded by dense acoustic clutter. We quantified bats' performance, sonar beam focus, and sensory acquisition rate. Performance was excellent in straight corridors, with sonar beam aim deviating less than 5° from the corridor midline. Bats anticipated an upcoming abrupt turn to the right or left by slowing flight speed and shifting beam aim to "look" proactively into one side of the corridor to identify the new flightpath. All bats mastered the right turn, but two bats consistently failed the left turn. Bats increased their sensory acquisition rate when confronting abrupt turns in both successful and failed flights. Limitations on biosonar performance reflected failures to switch beam aim and to modify a learned spatial map, rather than failures to update acquisition rate.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amaro Tuninetti
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, & Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
| | - Chen Ming
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
- Carney Institute for Brain Science, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
| | - Kelsey N. Hom
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, & Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
| | - James A. Simmons
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
- Carney Institute for Brain Science, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
| | - Andrea Megela Simmons
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, & Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
- Carney Institute for Brain Science, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Abstract
Insectivorous bats capture their prey in flight with impressive success. They rely on the echoes of their own ultrasonic vocalization that yield acoustic snapshots, which enable target tracking on a rapid time scale. This task requires the use of intermittent information to navigate a dynamically changing environment. Bats may solve this challenging task by building internal models that estimate target velocity to anticipate the future location of a prey item. This has been recently tested empirically in perched bats tracking a target moving across their acoustic field. In this report, we build on past work to propose a new model that describes bat flight trajectories employing predictive strategies. Furthermore, we compare this model with a previous model of bat target interception that has also been employed by some visually guided animals: parallel navigation. Abbreviations: HTTP, Hybrid Target Trajectory Prediction; CATD, Constant Absolute Target Direction; CB, Constant Bearing; PN, Parallel Navigation
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Angeles Salles
- Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Clarice A Diebold
- Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Cynthia F Moss
- Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Zhang Y, Liu Y, Jin R, Tao J, Chen L, Wu X. GLLPA: A Graph Layout based Label Propagation Algorithm for community detection. Knowl Based Syst 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.knosys.2020.106363] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
10
|
Abstract
The wide frequency spectrum of FM bat biosonar sounds enables accurate perception of echo delay (target distance) by contributing numerous delay estimates across frequencies. However, bats require the lowest frequencies in the broadcast to be present in echoes for all higher frequencies to contribute, too. By incorporating this feature into an existing auditory model of FM biosonar, the model can reject echoes that lack the lowest frequencies in the most recent broadcast, thus suppressing echoes of an earlier broadcast that has slightly higher low-end frequencies. This biologically inspired method adopts the bat’s frequency-hopping technique to suppress pulse-echo ambiguity in wideband systems, a serious problem for man-made wideband radar and sonar systems. Big brown bats transmit wideband FM biosonar sounds that sweep from 55 to 25 kHz (first harmonic, FM1) and from 110 to 50 kHz (second harmonic, FM2). FM1 is required to perceive echo delay for target ranging; FM2 contributes only if corresponding FM1 frequencies are present. We show that echoes need only the lowest FM1 broadcast frequencies of 25 to 30 kHz for delay perception. If these frequencies are removed, no delay is perceived. Bats begin echo processing at the lowest frequencies and accumulate perceptual acuity over successively higher frequencies, but they cannot proceed without the low-frequency starting point in their broadcasts. This reveals a solution to pulse-echo ambiguity, a serious problem for radar or sonar. In dense, extended biosonar scenes, bats have to emit sounds rapidly to avoid collisions with near objects. But if a new broadcast is emitted when echoes of the previous broadcast still are arriving, echoes from both broadcasts intermingle, creating ambiguity about which echo corresponds to which broadcast. Frequency hopping by several kilohertz from one broadcast to the next can segregate overlapping narrowband echo streams, but wideband FM echoes ordinarily do not segregate because their spectra still overlap. By starting echo processing at the lowest frequencies in frequency-hopped broadcasts, echoes of the higher hopped broadcast are prevented from being accepted by lower hopped broadcasts, and ambiguity is avoided. The bat-inspired spectrogram correlation and transformation (SCAT) model also begins at the lowest frequencies; echoes that lack them are eliminated from processing of delay and no longer cause ambiguity.
Collapse
|
11
|
Diebold CA, Salles A, Moss CF. Adaptive Echolocation and Flight Behaviors in Bats Can Inspire Technology Innovations for Sonar Tracking and Interception. SENSORS 2020; 20:s20102958. [PMID: 32456142 PMCID: PMC7285367 DOI: 10.3390/s20102958] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2020] [Revised: 05/12/2020] [Accepted: 05/18/2020] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Target tracking and interception in a dynamic world proves to be a fundamental challenge faced by both animals and artificial systems. To track moving objects under natural conditions, agents must employ strategies to mitigate interference and conditions of uncertainty. Animal studies of prey tracking and capture reveal biological solutions, which can inspire new technologies, particularly for operations in complex and noisy environments. By reviewing research on target tracking and interception by echolocating bats, we aim to highlight biological solutions that could inform new approaches to artificial sonar tracking and navigation systems. Most bat species use wideband echolocation signals to navigate dense forests and hunt for evasive insects in the dark. Importantly, bats exhibit rapid adaptations in flight trajectory, sonar beam aim, and echolocation signal design, which appear to be key to the success of these animals in a variety of tasks. The rich suite of adaptive behaviors of echolocating bats could be leveraged in new sonar tracking technologies by implementing dynamic sensorimotor feedback control of wideband sonar signal design, head, and ear movements.
Collapse
|
12
|
Simmons JA, Brown PE, Vargas-Irwin CE, Simmons AM. Big brown bats are challenged by acoustically-guided flights through a circular tunnel of hoops. Sci Rep 2020; 10:832. [PMID: 31964933 PMCID: PMC6972939 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-57632-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2019] [Accepted: 01/02/2020] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Mines and caves provide essential roosting places for bats, but often they are obstructed to prevent entry by humans. To allow bats to access their roosts, metal corrugated culvert pipes are sometimes installed. Wildlife surveys indicate, however, that bats may abandon caves having corrugated culvert entrances. Culverts may be confusing to bats due to the complex patterns of echoes returned by the regular, ring-like corrugations. We tested the hypothesis that a circular tunnel composed of successive hoops is difficult for big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) to navigate. Experiments challenged bats with flights through a tunnel of round plastic hoops or a corridor flanked left and right by rows of plastic hanging chains. The bats swerved sideways and left the pathway on more flights in the hoop tunnel compared to only rarely in the chain corridor. Even during successful flights through the hoops, bats changed the temporal patterning of their echolocation pulses to compress them into more sonar sound groups. From prior research, this active reaction is an indicator of a perceptually more difficult task. To allow bats access to mines through culverts without affecting their echolocation behavior, smoothing or masking the regular corrugations inside with concrete may be effective.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James A Simmons
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island, 02912-9067, United States
| | | | - Carlos E Vargas-Irwin
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island, 02912-9067, United States
| | - Andrea M Simmons
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island, 02912-9067, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Bou Mansour C, Koreman E, Steckel J, Peremans H, Vanderelst D. Avoidance of non-localizable obstacles in echolocating bats: A robotic model. PLoS Comput Biol 2019; 15:e1007550. [PMID: 31856162 PMCID: PMC6941896 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007550] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2019] [Revised: 01/03/2020] [Accepted: 11/17/2019] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
Most objects and vegetation making up the habitats of echolocating bats return a multitude of overlapping echoes. Recent evidence suggests that the limited temporal and spatial resolution of bio-sonar prevents bats from separately perceiving the objects giving rise to these overlapping echoes. Therefore, bats often operate under conditions where their ability to localize obstacles is severely limited. Nevertheless, bats excel at avoiding complex obstacles. In this paper, we present a robotic model of bat obstacle avoidance using interaural level differences and distance to the nearest obstacle as the minimal set of cues. In contrast to previous robotic models of bats, the current robot does not attempt to localize obstacles. We evaluate two obstacle avoidance strategies. First, the Fixed Head Strategy keeps the acoustic gaze direction aligned with the direction of flight. Second, the Delayed Linear Adaptive Law (DLAL) Strategy uses acoustic gaze scanning, as observed in hunting bats. Acoustic gaze scanning has been suggested to aid the bat in hunting for prey. Here, we evaluate its adaptive value for obstacle avoidance when obstacles can not be localized. The robot's obstacle avoidance performance is assessed in two environments mimicking (highly cluttered) experimental setups commonly used in behavioral experiments: a rectangular arena containing multiple complex cylindrical reflecting surfaces and a corridor lined with complex reflecting surfaces. The results indicate that distance to the nearest object and interaural level differences allows steering the robot clear of obstacles in environments that return non-localizable echoes. Furthermore, we found that using acoustic gaze scanning reduced performance, suggesting that gaze scanning might not be beneficial under conditions where the animal has limited access to angular information, which is in line with behavioral evidence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carl Bou Mansour
- Department of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America
| | - Elijah Koreman
- Department of Computer Science, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, United States of America
| | - Jan Steckel
- Constrained Systems Lab, University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Herbert Peremans
- Department of Engineering Management, University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Dieter Vanderelst
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Beetz MJ, Kössl M, Hechavarría JC. Adaptations in the call emission pattern of frugivorous bats when orienting under challenging conditions. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2019; 205:457-467. [PMID: 30997534 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-019-01337-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/11/2019] [Revised: 04/01/2019] [Accepted: 04/10/2019] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
Echolocating bats emit biosonar calls and use echoes arising from call reflections, for orientation. They often pattern their calls into groups which increases the rate of sensory feedback. Insectivorous bats emit call groups at a higher rate when orienting in cluttered compared to uncluttered environments. Frugivorous bats increase the rate of call group emission when they echolocate in noisy environments. In frugivorous bats, it remains unclear if call group emission represents an exclusive adaptation to avoid acoustic interference by signals of conspecifics or if it represents an adaptation that allows to orient under demanding environmental conditions. Here, we compared the emission pattern of the frugivorous bat Carolliaperspicillata when the bats were flying in narrow versus wide or cluttered versus non-cluttered corridors. The bats emitted larger call groups and they increased the call rate within call groups when navigating in narrow or cluttered environments. These adaptations resemble the ones shown when the bats navigate in noisy environments. Thus, call group emission represents an adaptive behavior when the bats orient in complex environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Jerome Beetz
- Institute for Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Goethe-University, Frankfurt, Germany. .,Zoology II Emmy-Noether Animal Navigation Group, Biocenter, University of Wuerzburg, Am Hubland, 97074, Wuerzburg, Germany.
| | - Manfred Kössl
- Institute for Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Goethe-University, Frankfurt, Germany
| | - Julio C Hechavarría
- Institute for Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Goethe-University, Frankfurt, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Simmons JA, Hiryu S, Shriram U. Biosonar interpulse intervals and pulse-echo ambiguity in four species of echolocating bats. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2019; 222:jeb.195446. [PMID: 30877230 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.195446] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2018] [Accepted: 03/07/2019] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
In complex biosonar scenes, the delay of echoes represents the spatial distribution of objects in depth. To avoid overlap of echo streams from successive broadcasts, individual echolocation sounds should only be emitted after all echoes of previous sounds have returned. However, close proximity of obstacles demands rapid pulse updates for steering to avoid collisions, which often means emitting a new sound before all of the previous echoes have returned. When two echo streams overlap, there is ambiguity about assigning echoes to the corresponding broadcasts. In laboratory tests of flight in dense, cluttered scenes, four species of echolocating bats exhibited different patterns of pulse emissions to accommodate potential pulse-echo ambiguity. Miniopterus fuliginosus emitted individual FM pulses only after all echoes of previous pulses had returned, with no alternating between long and short intervals. Pipistrellus abramus and Eptesicus fuscus alternated between emitting long FM pulse intervals to receive all echoes before the next pulse, and short intervals to update the rapidly changing scene while accepting partial overlap of successive echo streams. Rhinolophus ferrumequinum nippon transmitted CF/FM pulses in alternating short and long intervals, usually two to four closely spaced sounds that produced overlapping echo streams, followed by a longer interval that separated echo streams. Rhinolophus f. nippon is a statistical outlier from the three FM species, which are more similar to each other. The repeated overlap of CF/FM echo streams suggests that CF components have a distinct role in rejection of clutter and mitigation of ambiguity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James A Simmons
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, RI 03912, USA
| | - Shizuko Hiryu
- Faculty of Life and Medical Sciences, Doshisha University, Kyotanabe, Japan
| | - Uday Shriram
- Program in Biotechnology, Brown University, Providence, RI 03912, USA
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) successfully navigate through clutter after exposure to intense band-limited sound. Sci Rep 2018; 8:13555. [PMID: 30201987 PMCID: PMC6131230 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-31872-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/13/2018] [Accepted: 08/28/2018] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Echolocating big brown bats fly, orient, forage, and roost in cluttered acoustic environments in which aggregate sound pressure levels can be as intense as 100 to 140 dB SPL, levels that would impair auditory perception in other terrestrial mammals. We showed previously that bats exposed to intense wide-band sound (116 dB SPL) can navigate successfully through dense acoustic clutter. Here, we extend these results by quantifying performance of bats navigating through a cluttered scene after exposure to intense band-limited sounds (bandwidths 5-25 kHz, 123 dB SPL). Behavioral performance was not significantly affected by prior sound exposure, with the exception of one bat after exposure to one sound. Even in this outlying case, performance recovered rapidly, by 10 min post-exposure. Temporal patterning of biosonar emissions during successful flights showed that bats maintained their individual strategies for navigating through the cluttered scene before and after exposures. In unsuccessful flights, interpulse intervals were skewed towards shorter values, suggesting a shift in strategy for solving the task rather than a hearing impairment. Results confirm previous findings that big brown bats are not as susceptible to noise-induced perceptual impairments as are other terrestrial mammals exposed to sounds of similar intensity and bandwidth.
Collapse
|
17
|
Warnecke M, Falk B, Moss CF. Echolocation and flight behavior of the bat Hipposideros armiger terasensis in a structured corridor. THE JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA 2018; 144:806. [PMID: 30180698 DOI: 10.1121/1.5050525] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/19/2018] [Accepted: 07/27/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
In this study, the echolocation and flight behaviors of the Taiwanese leaf-nosed bat (Hipposideros armiger terasensis), which uses constant-frequency (CF) biosonar signals combined with a frequency-modulated (FM) sweep, are compared with those of the big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus), which uses FM signals alone. The CF-FM bat flew through a corridor bounded by vertical poles on either side, and the inter-pole spacing of the walls was manipulated to create different echo flow conditions. The bat's flight trajectories and echolocation behaviors across corridor conditions were analyzed. Like the big brown bat, the Taiwanese leaf-nosed bat centered its flight trajectory within the corridor when the pole spacing was the same on the two walls. However, the two species showed different flight behaviors when the pole spacing differed on the two walls. While the big brown bat deviated from the corridor center towards the wall with sparse pole spacing, the Taiwanese leaf-nosed bat did not. Further, in comparison to E. fuscus, H. a. terasensis utilized different echolocation patterns showing a prevalence of grouping sounds into clusters of three. These findings indicate that the two species' distinct sonar signal designs contribute to their differences in flight trajectories in a structured corridor.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michaela Warnecke
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, 3400 North Charles Street, Baltimore, Maryland 21218, USA
| | - Benjamin Falk
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, 3400 North Charles Street, Baltimore, Maryland 21218, USA
| | - Cynthia F Moss
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, 3400 North Charles Street, Baltimore, Maryland 21218, USA
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Macías S, Luo J, Moss CF. Natural echolocation sequences evoke echo-delay selectivity in the auditory midbrain of the FM bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J Neurophysiol 2018; 120:1323-1339. [PMID: 29924708 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00160.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Echolocating bats must process temporal streams of sonar sounds to represent objects along the range axis. Neuronal echo-delay tuning, the putative mechanism of sonar ranging, has been characterized in the inferior colliculus (IC) of the mustached bat, an insectivorous species that produces echolocation calls consisting of constant frequency and frequency modulated (FM) components, but not in species that use FM signals alone. This raises questions about the mechanisms that give rise to echo-delay tuning in insectivorous bats that use different signal designs. To investigate whether stimulus context may account for species differences in echo-delay selectivity, we characterized single-unit responses in the IC of awake passively listening FM bats, Eptesicus fuscus, to broadcasts of natural sonar call-echo sequences, which contained dynamic changes in signal duration, interval, spectrotemporal structure, and echo-delay. In E. fuscus, neural selectivity to call-echo delay emerges in a population of IC neurons when stimulated with call-echo pairs presented at intervals mimicking those in a natural sonar sequence. To determine whether echo-delay selectivity also depends on the spectrotemporal features of individual sounds within natural sonar sequences, we studied responses to computer-generated echolocation signals that controlled for call interval, duration, bandwidth, sweep rate, and echo-delay. A subpopulation of IC neurons responded selectively to the combination of the spectrotemporal structure of natural call-echo pairs and their temporal patterning within a dynamic sonar sequence. These new findings suggest that the FM bat's fine control over biosonar signal parameters may modulate IC neuronal selectivity to the dimension of echo-delay. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Echolocating bats perform precise auditory temporal computations to estimate their distance to objects. Here, we report that response selectivity of neurons in the inferior colliculus of a frequency modulated bat to call-echo delay, or target range tuning, depends on the temporal patterning and spectrotemporal features of sound elements in a natural echolocation sequence. We suggest that echo responses to objects at different distances are gated by the bat's active control over the spectrotemporal patterning of its sonar emissions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Silvio Macías
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University , Baltimore, Maryland
| | - Jinhong Luo
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University , Baltimore, Maryland
| | - Cynthia F Moss
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University , Baltimore, Maryland
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Beetz MJ, García-Rosales F, Kössl M, Hechavarría JC. Robustness of cortical and subcortical processing in the presence of natural masking sounds. Sci Rep 2018; 8:6863. [PMID: 29717258 PMCID: PMC5931562 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-25241-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/22/2018] [Accepted: 04/17/2018] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Processing of ethologically relevant stimuli could be interfered by non-relevant stimuli. Animals have behavioral adaptations to reduce signal interference. It is largely unexplored whether the behavioral adaptations facilitate neuronal processing of relevant stimuli. Here, we characterize behavioral adaptations in the presence of biotic noise in the echolocating bat Carollia perspicillata and we show that the behavioral adaptations could facilitate neuronal processing of biosonar information. According to the echolocation behavior, bats need to extract their own signals in the presence of vocalizations from conspecifics. With playback experiments, we demonstrate that C. perspicillata increases the sensory acquisition rate by emitting groups of echolocation calls when flying in noisy environments. Our neurophysiological results from the auditory midbrain and cortex show that the high sensory acquisition rate does not vastly increase neuronal suppression and that the response to an echolocation sequence is partially preserved in the presence of biosonar signals from conspecifics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Jerome Beetz
- Institute for Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Goethe-University, 60438, Frankfurt/M., Germany. .,Department of Behavioral Physiology and Sociobiology, Biozentrum, University of Würzburg, Am Hubland, Würzburg, 97074, Germany.
| | | | - Manfred Kössl
- Institute for Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Goethe-University, 60438, Frankfurt/M., Germany
| | - Julio C Hechavarría
- Institute for Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Goethe-University, 60438, Frankfurt/M., Germany
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Kothari NB, Wohlgemuth MJ, Moss CF. Dynamic representation of 3D auditory space in the midbrain of the free-flying echolocating bat. eLife 2018; 7:e29053. [PMID: 29633711 PMCID: PMC5896882 DOI: 10.7554/elife.29053] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2017] [Accepted: 02/27/2018] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Essential to spatial orientation in the natural environment is a dynamic representation of direction and distance to objects. Despite the importance of 3D spatial localization to parse objects in the environment and to guide movement, most neurophysiological investigations of sensory mapping have been limited to studies of restrained subjects, tested with 2D, artificial stimuli. Here, we show for the first time that sensory neurons in the midbrain superior colliculus (SC) of the free-flying echolocating bat encode 3D egocentric space, and that the bat's inspection of objects in the physical environment sharpens tuning of single neurons, and shifts peak responses to represent closer distances. These findings emerged from wireless neural recordings in free-flying bats, in combination with an echo model that computes the animal's instantaneous stimulus space. Our research reveals dynamic 3D space coding in a freely moving mammal engaged in a real-world navigation task.
Collapse
|
21
|
Accomando AW, Vargas-Irwin CE, Simmons JA. Spike Train Similarity Space (SSIMS) Method Detects Effects of Obstacle Proximity and Experience on Temporal Patterning of Bat Biosonar. Front Behav Neurosci 2018; 12:13. [PMID: 29472848 PMCID: PMC5809465 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2017] [Accepted: 01/16/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Bats emit biosonar pulses in complex temporal patterns that change to accommodate dynamic surroundings. Efforts to quantify these patterns have included analyses of inter-pulse intervals, sonar sound groups, and changes in individual signal parameters such as duration or frequency. Here, the similarity in temporal structure between trains of biosonar pulses is assessed. The spike train similarity space (SSIMS) algorithm, originally designed for neural activity pattern analysis, was applied to determine which features of the environment influence temporal patterning of pulses emitted by flying big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus. In these laboratory experiments, bats flew down a flight corridor through an obstacle array. The corridor varied in width (100, 70, or 40 cm) and shape (straight or curved). Using a relational point-process framework, SSIMS was able to discriminate between echolocation call sequences recorded from flights in each of the corridor widths. SSIMS was also able to tell the difference between pulse trains recorded during flights where corridor shape through the obstacle array matched the previous trials (fixed, or expected) as opposed to those recorded from flights with randomized corridor shape (variable, or unexpected), but only for the flight path shape in which the bats had previous training. The results show that experience influences the temporal patterns with which bats emit their echolocation calls. It is demonstrated that obstacle proximity to the bat affects call patterns more dramatically than flight path shape.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alyssa W Accomando
- Department of Neuroscience, Division of Biology and Medicine, Brown University, Providence, RI, United States.,National Marine Mammal Foundation, San Diego, CA, United States
| | - Carlos E Vargas-Irwin
- Department of Neuroscience, Division of Biology and Medicine, Brown University, Providence, RI, United States
| | - James A Simmons
- Department of Neuroscience, Division of Biology and Medicine, Brown University, Providence, RI, United States
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Kothari NB, Wohlgemuth MJ, Moss CF. Adaptive sonar call timing supports target tracking in echolocating bats. J Exp Biol 2018; 221:jeb.176537. [DOI: 10.1242/jeb.176537] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/23/2017] [Accepted: 07/02/2018] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Echolocating bats dynamically adapt the features of their sonar calls as they approach obstacles and track targets. As insectivorous bats forage, they increase sonar call rate with decreasing prey distance, and often embedded in bat insect approach sequences are clusters of sonar sounds, termed sonar sound groups (SSGs). The bat's production of SSGs has been observed in both field and laboratory conditions, and is hypothesized to sharpen spatiotemporal sonar resolution. When insectivorous bats hunt insects, they may encounter erratically moving prey, which increases the demands on the bat's sonar imaging system. Here, we studied the bat's adaptive vocal behavior in an experimentally controlled insect tracking task, allowing us to manipulate the predictability of target trajectories and measure the prevalence of SSGs. With this system, we trained bats to remain stationary on a platform and track a moving prey item, whose trajectory was programmed either to approach the bat, or to move back and forth, before arriving at the bat. We manipulated target motion predictability by varying the order in which different target trajectories were presented to the bats. During all trials, we recorded the bat's sonar calls and later analyzed the incidence of SSG production during the different target tracking conditions. Our results demonstrate that bats increase the production of SSGs when target unpredictability increases, and decrease the production of SSGs when target motion predictability increases. Further, bats produce the same number of sonar vocalizations irrespective of the target motion predictability, indicating that the animal's temporal clustering of sonar call sequences to produce SSGs is purposeful, and therefore involves sensorimotor planning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ninad B. Kothari
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Krieger School of Arts and Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
| | - Melville J. Wohlgemuth
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Krieger School of Arts and Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
| | - Cynthia F. Moss
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Krieger School of Arts and Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
- The Solomon H. Snyder Department of Neuroscience, School of Medicine. Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
- Department of Mechanical Engineering, Whiting School of Engineering. Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
- Behavioral Biology Program Chair. Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Ros IG, Bhagavatula PS, Lin HT, Biewener AA. Rules to fly by: pigeons navigating horizontal obstacles limit steering by selecting gaps most aligned to their flight direction. Interface Focus 2017; 7:20160093. [PMID: 28163883 DOI: 10.1098/rsfs.2016.0093] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Flying animals must successfully contend with obstacles in their natural environments. Inspired by the robust manoeuvring abilities of flying animals, unmanned aerial systems are being developed and tested to improve flight control through cluttered environments. We previously examined steering strategies that pigeons adopt to fly through an array of vertical obstacles (VOs). Modelling VO flight guidance revealed that pigeons steer towards larger visual gaps when making fast steering decisions. In the present experiments, we recorded three-dimensional flight kinematics of pigeons as they flew through randomized arrays of horizontal obstacles (HOs). We found that pigeons still decelerated upon approach but flew faster through a denser array of HOs compared with the VO array previously tested. Pigeons exhibited limited steering and chose gaps between obstacles most aligned to their immediate flight direction, in contrast to VO navigation that favoured widest gap steering. In addition, pigeons navigated past the HOs with more variable and decreased wing stroke span and adjusted their wing stroke plane to reduce contact with the obstacles. Variability in wing extension, stroke plane and wing stroke path was greater during HO flight. Pigeons also exhibited pronounced head movements when negotiating HOs, which potentially serve a visual function. These head-bobbing-like movements were most pronounced in the horizontal (flight direction) and vertical directions, consistent with engaging motion vision mechanisms for obstacle detection. These results show that pigeons exhibit a keen kinesthetic sense of their body and wings in relation to obstacles. Together with aerodynamic flapping flight mechanics that favours vertical manoeuvring, pigeons are able to navigate HOs using simple rules, with remarkable success.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ivo G Ros
- Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, Concord Field Station, Harvard University, Bedford, MA 01730, USA; Division of Biology and Bioengineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA
| | - Partha S Bhagavatula
- Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, Concord Field Station , Harvard University , Bedford, MA 01730 , USA
| | - Huai-Ti Lin
- Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, Concord Field Station, Harvard University, Bedford, MA 01730, USA; HHMI Janelia Research Campus, Ashburn, VA 20147, USA
| | - Andrew A Biewener
- Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, Concord Field Station , Harvard University , Bedford, MA 01730 , USA
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Falk B, Kasnadi J, Moss CF. Tight coordination of aerial flight maneuvers and sonar call production in insectivorous bats. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2017; 218:3678-88. [PMID: 26582935 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.122283] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Echolocating bats face the challenge of coordinating flight kinematics with the production of echolocation signals used to guide navigation. Previous studies of bat flight have focused on kinematics of fruit and nectar-feeding bats, often in wind tunnels with limited maneuvering, and without analysis of echolocation behavior. In this study, we engaged insectivorous big brown bats in a task requiring simultaneous turning and climbing flight, and used synchronized high-speed motion-tracking cameras and audio recordings to quantify the animals' coordination of wing kinematics and echolocation. Bats varied flight speed, turn rate, climb rate and wingbeat rate as they navigated around obstacles, and they adapted their sonar signals in patterning, duration and frequency in relation to the timing of flight maneuvers. We found that bats timed the emission of sonar calls with the upstroke phase of the wingbeat cycle in straight flight, and that this relationship changed when bats turned to navigate obstacles. We also characterized the unsteadiness of climbing and turning flight, as well as the relationship between speed and kinematic parameters. Adaptations in the bats' echolocation call frequency suggest changes in beam width and sonar field of view in relation to obstacles and flight behavior. By characterizing flight and sonar behaviors in an insectivorous bat species, we find evidence of exquisitely tight coordination of sensory and motor systems for obstacle navigation and insect capture.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Benjamin Falk
- Johns Hopkins University, 7121 Riverdale Road, Lanham, MD 20706, USA
| | - Joseph Kasnadi
- Johns Hopkins University, 7121 Riverdale Road, Lanham, MD 20706, USA
| | - Cynthia F Moss
- Johns Hopkins University, 7121 Riverdale Road, Lanham, MD 20706, USA
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Gonzalez-Terrazas TP, Koblitz JC, Fleming TH, Medellín RA, Kalko EKV, Schnitzler HU, Tschapka M. How Nectar-Feeding Bats Localize their Food: Echolocation Behavior of Leptonycteris yerbabuenae Approaching Cactus Flowers. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0163492. [PMID: 27684373 PMCID: PMC5042408 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163492] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/18/2016] [Accepted: 09/10/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Nectar-feeding bats show morphological, physiological, and behavioral adaptations for feeding on nectar. How they find and localize flowers is still poorly understood. While scent cues alone allow no precise localization of a floral target, the spatial properties of flower echoes are very precise and could play a major role, particularly at close range. The aim of this study is to understand the role of echolocation for classification and localization of flowers. We compared the approach behavior of Leptonycteris yerbabuenae to flowers of a columnar cactus, Pachycereus pringlei, to that to an acrylic hollow hemisphere that is acoustically conspicuous to bats, but has different acoustic properties and, contrary to the cactus flower, present no scent. For recording the flight and echolocation behaviour we used two infrared video cameras under stroboscopic illumination synchronized with ultrasound recordings. During search flights all individuals identified both targets as a possible food source and initiated an approach flight; however, they visited only the cactus flower. In experiments with the acrylic hemisphere bats aborted the approach at ca. 40-50 cm. In the last instant before the flower visit the bats emitted a long terminal group of 10-20 calls. This is the first report of this behaviour for a nectar-feeding bat. Our findings suggest that L. yerbabuenae use echolocation for classification and localization of cactus flowers and that the echo-acoustic characteristics of the flower guide the bats directly to the flower opening.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Theodore H. Fleming
- Department of Biology, University of Miami, Miami, Florida, United States of America
| | - Rodrigo A. Medellín
- Instituto de Ecología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, México D. F., México
| | - Elisabeth K. V. Kalko
- Institute of Evolutionary Ecology and Conservation Genomics, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
- Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, Balboa, Panama
| | | | - Marco Tschapka
- Institute of Evolutionary Ecology and Conservation Genomics, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
- Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, Balboa, Panama
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Wohlgemuth MJ, Kothari NB, Moss CF. Action Enhances Acoustic Cues for 3-D Target Localization by Echolocating Bats. PLoS Biol 2016; 14:e1002544. [PMID: 27608186 PMCID: PMC5015854 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002544] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2016] [Accepted: 08/04/2016] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Under natural conditions, animals encounter a barrage of sensory information from which they must select and interpret biologically relevant signals. Active sensing can facilitate this process by engaging motor systems in the sampling of sensory information. The echolocating bat serves as an excellent model to investigate the coupling between action and sensing because it adaptively controls both the acoustic signals used to probe the environment and movements to receive echoes at the auditory periphery. We report here that the echolocating bat controls the features of its sonar vocalizations in tandem with the positioning of the outer ears to maximize acoustic cues for target detection and localization. The bat’s adaptive control of sonar vocalizations and ear positioning occurs on a millisecond timescale to capture spatial information from arriving echoes, as well as on a longer timescale to track target movement. Our results demonstrate that purposeful control over sonar sound production and reception can serve to improve acoustic cues for localization tasks. This finding also highlights the general importance of movement to sensory processing across animal species. Finally, our discoveries point to important parallels between spatial perception by echolocation and vision. As an echolocating bat tracks a moving target, it produces head waggles and adjusts the separation of the tips of its ears to enhance cues for target detection and localization. These findings suggest parallels in active sensing between echolocation and vision. As animals operate in the natural environment, they must detect and process relevant sensory information embedded in complex and noisy signals. One strategy to overcome this challenge is to use active sensing or behavioral adjustments to extract sensory information from a selected region of the environment. We studied one of nature’s champions in auditory active sensing—the echolocating bat—to understand how this animal extracts task-relevant acoustic cues to detect and track a moving target. The bat produces high-frequency vocalizations and processes information carried by returning echoes to navigate and catch prey. This animal serves as an excellent model of active sensing because both sonar signal transmission and echo reception are under the animal’s active control. We used high-speed stereo video images of the bat’s head and ear movements, along with synchronized audio recordings, to study how the bat coordinates adaptive motor behaviors when detecting and tracking moving prey. We found that the bat synchronizes changes in sonar vocal production with changes in the movements of the head and ears to enhance acoustic cues for target detection and localization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melville J. Wohlgemuth
- Department of Psychology and Institute for Systems Research, Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland, United States of America
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Ninad B. Kothari
- Department of Psychology and Institute for Systems Research, Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland, United States of America
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland, United States of America
| | - Cynthia F. Moss
- Department of Psychology and Institute for Systems Research, Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland, United States of America
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Wheeler AR, Fulton KA, Gaudette JE, Simmons RA, Matsuo I, Simmons JA. Echolocating Big Brown Bats, Eptesicus fuscus, Modulate Pulse Intervals to Overcome Range Ambiguity in Cluttered Surroundings. Front Behav Neurosci 2016; 10:125. [PMID: 27445723 PMCID: PMC4916216 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00125] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2016] [Accepted: 06/03/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) emit trains of brief, wideband frequency-modulated (FM) echolocation sounds and use echoes of these sounds to orient, find insects, and guide flight through vegetation. They are observed to emit sounds that alternate between short and long inter-pulse intervals (IPIs), forming sonar sound groups. The occurrence of these strobe groups has been linked to flight in cluttered acoustic environments, but how exactly bats use sonar sound groups to orient and navigate is still a mystery. Here, the production of sound groups during clutter navigation was examined. Controlled flight experiments were conducted where the proximity of the nearest obstacles was systematically decreased while the extended scene was kept constant. Four bats flew along a corridor of varying widths (100, 70, and 40 cm) bounded by rows of vertically hanging plastic chains while in-flight echolocation calls were recorded. Bats shortened their IPIs for more rapid spatial sampling and also grouped their sounds more tightly when flying in narrower corridors. Bats emitted echolocation calls with progressively shorter IPIs over the course of a flight, and began their flights by emitting shorter starting IPI calls when clutter was denser. The percentage of sound groups containing 3 or more calls increased with increasing clutter proximity. Moreover, IPI sequences having internal structure become more pronounced when corridor width narrows. A novel metric for analyzing the temporal organization of sound sequences was developed, and the results indicate that the time interval between echolocation calls depends heavily on the preceding time interval. The occurrence of specific IPI patterns were dependent upon clutter, which suggests that sonar sound grouping may be an adaptive strategy for coping with pulse-echo ambiguity in cluttered surroundings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alyssa R Wheeler
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University Providence, RI, USA
| | - Kara A Fulton
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown UniversityProvidence, RI, USA; Circuit Dynamics and Connectivity Unit, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of HealthBethesda, MD, USA
| | - Jason E Gaudette
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown UniversityProvidence, RI, USA; Sensors and Sonar Systems Department, Naval Undersea Warfare CenterNewport, RI, USA
| | - Ryan A Simmons
- Department of Biostatistics and Bioinformatics, Duke University School of Medicine Durham, NC, USA
| | - Ikuo Matsuo
- Department of Information Science, Tohoku Gakuin University Sendai, Japan
| | - James A Simmons
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University Providence, RI, USA
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Warnecke M, Lee WJ, Krishnan A, Moss CF. Dynamic Echo Information Guides Flight in the Big Brown Bat. Front Behav Neurosci 2016; 10:81. [PMID: 27199690 PMCID: PMC4843091 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00081] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/03/2016] [Accepted: 04/08/2016] [Indexed: 12/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Animals rely on sensory feedback from their environment to guide locomotion. For instance, visually guided animals use patterns of optic flow to control their velocity and to estimate their distance to objects (e.g., Srinivasan et al., 1991, 1996). In this study, we investigated how acoustic information guides locomotion of animals that use hearing as a primary sensory modality to orient and navigate in the dark, where visual information is unavailable. We studied flight and echolocation behaviors of big brown bats as they flew under infrared illumination through a corridor with walls constructed from a series of individual vertical wooden poles. The spacing between poles on opposite walls of the corridor was experimentally manipulated to create dense/sparse and balanced/imbalanced spatial structure. The bats' flight trajectories and echolocation signals were recorded with high-speed infrared motion-capture cameras and ultrasound microphones, respectively. As bats flew through the corridor, successive biosonar emissions returned cascades of echoes from the walls of the corridor. The bats flew through the center of the corridor when the pole spacing on opposite walls was balanced and closer to the side with wider pole spacing when opposite walls had an imbalanced density. Moreover, bats produced shorter duration echolocation calls when they flew through corridors with smaller spacing between poles, suggesting that clutter density influences features of the bat's sonar signals. Flight speed and echolocation call rate did not, however, vary with dense and sparse spacing between the poles forming the corridor walls. Overall, these data demonstrate that bats adapt their flight and echolocation behavior dynamically when flying through acoustically complex environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michaela Warnecke
- Comparative Neural Systems and Behavior Lab, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, BaltimoreMD, USA
| | | | | | - Cynthia F. Moss
- Comparative Neural Systems and Behavior Lab, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, BaltimoreMD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Luo J, Goerlitz HR, Brumm H, Wiegrebe L. Linking the sender to the receiver: vocal adjustments by bats to maintain signal detection in noise. Sci Rep 2015; 5:18556. [PMID: 26692325 PMCID: PMC4686984 DOI: 10.1038/srep18556] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/18/2015] [Accepted: 11/20/2015] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Short-term adjustments of signal characteristics allow animals to maintain reliable communication in noise. Noise-dependent vocal plasticity often involves simultaneous changes in multiple parameters. Here, we quantified for the first time the relative contributions of signal amplitude, duration, and redundancy for improving signal detectability in noise. To this end, we used a combination of behavioural experiments on pale spear-nosed bats (Phyllostomus discolor) and signal detection models. In response to increasing noise levels, all bats raised the amplitude of their echolocation calls by 1.8-7.9 dB (the Lombard effect). Bats also increased signal duration by 13%-85%, corresponding to an increase in detectability of 1.0-5.3 dB. Finally, in some noise conditions, bats increased signal redundancy by producing more call groups. Assuming optimal cognitive integration, this could result in a further detectability improvement by up to 4 dB. Our data show that while the main improvement in signal detectability was due to the Lombard effect, increasing signal duration and redundancy can also contribute markedly to improving signal detectability. Overall, our findings demonstrate that the observed adjustments of signal parameters in noise are matched to how these parameters are processed in the receiver's sensory system, thereby facilitating signal transmission in fluctuating environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jinhong Luo
- Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Acoustic and Functional Ecology Group, Eberhard-Gwinner-Straße, 82319 Seewiesen, Germany
- Division of Neurobiology, Department Biology II, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Großhaderner Straße 2, 82152 Planegg-Martinsried, Germany
| | - Holger R. Goerlitz
- Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Acoustic and Functional Ecology Group, Eberhard-Gwinner-Straße, 82319 Seewiesen, Germany
| | - Henrik Brumm
- Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Communication and Social Behaviour Group, Eberhard-Gwinner-Straße, 82319 Seewiesen, Germany
| | - Lutz Wiegrebe
- Division of Neurobiology, Department Biology II, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Großhaderner Straße 2, 82152 Planegg-Martinsried, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Knowles JM, Barchi JR, Gaudette JE, Simmons JA. Effective biosonar echo-to-clutter rejection ratio in a complex dynamic scene. THE JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA 2015; 138:1090-101. [PMID: 26328724 PMCID: PMC4552698 DOI: 10.1121/1.4915001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2014] [Revised: 02/06/2015] [Accepted: 02/23/2015] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
Biosonar guidance in a rapidly changing complex scene was examined by flying big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) through a Y-shaped maze composed of rows of strongly reflective vertical plastic chains that presented the bat with left and right corridors for passage. Corridors were 80-100 cm wide and 2-4 m long. Using the two-choice Y-shaped paradigm to compensate for left-right bias and spatial memory, a moveable, weakly reflective thin-net barrier randomly blocked the left or right corridor, interspersed with no-barrier trials. Flight path and beam aim were tracked using an array of 24 microphones surrounding the flight room. Each bat flew on a path centered in the entry corridor (base of Y) and then turned into the left or right passage, to land on the far wall or to turn abruptly, reacting to avoid a collision. Broadcasts were broadly beamed in the direction of flight, smoothly leading into an upcoming turn. Duration of broadcasts decreased slowly from 3 to 2 ms during flights to track the chains' progressively closer ranges. Broadcast features and flight velocity changed abruptly about 1 m from the barrier, indicating that echoes from the net were perceived even though they were 18-35 dB weaker than overlapping echoes from surrounding chains.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey M Knowles
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, 185 Meeting Street, Providence, Rhode Island 02912, USA
| | - Jonathan R Barchi
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, 185 Meeting Street, Providence, Rhode Island 02912, USA
| | | | - James A Simmons
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, 185 Meeting Street, Providence, Rhode Island 02912, USA
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Hulgard K, Moss CF, Jakobsen L, Surlykke A. Big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) emit intense search calls and fly in stereotyped flight paths as they forage in the wild. J Exp Biol 2015; 219:334-40. [DOI: 10.1242/jeb.128983] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2015] [Accepted: 11/09/2015] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
The big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus, uses echolocation for orientation and foraging, and scans its surroundings by aiming its sonar beam at obstacles and prey. All call parameters are highly adaptable and determine the bat's acoustic field of view and hence its perception of the echo scene. The intensity (source level) and directionality of the emitted calls directly contribute to the bat's acoustic field of view; however, the source level and directionality of the big brown bat's sonar signals have not been measured in the field. In addition, for bats, navigation and prey capture require that they process several streams of acoustic information. By using stereotypic flight paths in known areas, bats may be able to reduce the sensory processing load for orientation and therefore allocate echo processing resources to prey. Here we recorded the echolocation calls from foraging E. fuscus, in the field with a microphone array and estimated call intensity and directionality, based on reconstructed flight trajectories. The source levels were intense with an average max SL of 138 dB (rms re 20 µPa at 0.1 m.). Further, measurements taken from a subset of calls indicate that the echolocation signals in the field may be more directional than estimated in the lab (half amplitude angle=30° at 35 kHz). We also observed that E. fuscus appear to follow stereotypic flight paths, and propose that this could be a strategy to optimize foraging efficiency by minimizing the sensory processing load
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katrine Hulgard
- Department of Biology, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark
| | - Cynthia F. Moss
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | - Lasse Jakobsen
- Department of Biology, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark
| | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Falk B, Jakobsen L, Surlykke A, Moss CF. Bats coordinate sonar and flight behavior as they forage in open and cluttered environments. J Exp Biol 2014; 217:4356-64. [PMID: 25394632 PMCID: PMC4375838 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.114132] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/17/2014] [Accepted: 11/02/2014] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Echolocating bats use active sensing as they emit sounds and listen to the returning echoes to probe their environment for navigation, obstacle avoidance and pursuit of prey. The sensing behavior of bats includes the planning of 3D spatial trajectory paths, which are guided by echo information. In this study, we examined the relationship between active sonar sampling and flight motor output as bats changed environments from open space to an artificial forest in a laboratory flight room. Using high-speed video and audio recordings, we reconstructed and analyzed 3D flight trajectories, sonar beam aim and acoustic sonar emission patterns as the bats captured prey. We found that big brown bats adjusted their sonar call structure, temporal patterning and flight speed in response to environmental change. The sonar beam aim of the bats predicted the flight turn rate in both the open room and the forest. However, the relationship between sonar beam aim and turn rate changed in the forest during the final stage of prey pursuit, during which the bat made shallower turns. We found flight stereotypy developed over multiple days in the forest, but did not find evidence for a reduction in active sonar sampling with experience. The temporal patterning of sonar sound groups was related to path planning around obstacles in the forest. Together, these results contribute to our understanding of how bats coordinate echolocation and flight behavior to represent and navigate their environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Benjamin Falk
- Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA.
| | - Lasse Jakobsen
- Centre for Animal Movement, Evolutionary Ecology, Lund University, Ecology Building, SE-22362 Lund, Sweden
| | - Annemarie Surlykke
- Department of Biology, University of Southern Denmark, DK-5230 Odense M., Denmark
| | - Cynthia F Moss
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
| |
Collapse
|