1
|
Zhang X, Li Z, Ji J, Ma Y, Sun G, Chen X, Zhang L, Zhang T, Zhang Y, Zhang Y. CD8 +T cells and monocytes were associated with brain alterations in human immunodeficiency virus-infected individuals with cognitive impairment. Brain Res Bull 2025; 222:111231. [PMID: 39892581 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2025.111231] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2024] [Revised: 12/23/2024] [Accepted: 01/25/2025] [Indexed: 02/04/2025]
Abstract
Cognitive impairment (CI) continues to be a concern for people living with HIV-1 (PLWH) in the era of antiretroviral therapy (ART), yet the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. We aimed to elucidate the structural and functional brain alterations and peripheral immune profile of PLWH with CI, as well as the correlation between them. PLWH were divided into CI (n = 30) and cognitive normal (CN, n = 59) groups based on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment, and underwent multi-modal magnetic resonance imaging. Mass cytometry was utilized to profile immune cells, while the liquid chip technique was employed to measure plasma levels of cytokines and chemokines. Spearman correlation analyses were conducted for correlation analysis. Here, we found that the gray matter volume in left supramarginal gyrus was reduced, and the ReHo in the right middle frontal gyrus and the functional connectivity between right middle frontal gyrus and left postcentral gyrus were enhanced in CI group compared to CN group. Additionally, the frequencies of naïve CD8+T cells (Tn) and CD31lowCD8+ Tn were significantly correlated with gray matter volume in the left supramarginal gyrus. The amplitude of low frequency fluctuations in a specific brain region of frontal-middle lobe was negatively correlated with the frequencies of non-classical monocytes (nCM) and their subpopulations (CCR2lownCM, CD57lownCM and CD127+nCM), and positively associated with the plasma interleukin 25 and transforming growth factor-α levels. These findings suggest the association between peripheral immunity and the brain abnormalities in PLWH, highlighting a potential role of the immune-brain-cognition axis in the pathogenesis of CI in Chinese PLWH.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xin Zhang
- Department of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, 100069, China; Center for Infectious Diseases, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, 100069, China; Beijing Institute of Hepatology, 100069, China
| | - Zhen Li
- Beijing Key Laboratory for HIV/AIDS Research, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, 100069, China
| | - Jiahao Ji
- Department of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, 100069, China
| | - Yundong Ma
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders & National Center for Mental Disorders, Beijing Anding Hospital, Capital Medical University, 100088, China; Advanced Innovation Center for Human Brain Protection, Capital Medical University, 100088, China
| | - Guangqiang Sun
- Center for Infectious Diseases, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, 100069, China
| | - Xue Chen
- Center for Infectious Diseases, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, 100069, China
| | - Ling Zhang
- Department of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, 100069, China
| | - Tong Zhang
- Center for Infectious Diseases, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, 100069, China; Beijing Key Laboratory for HIV/AIDS Research, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, 100069, China.
| | - Yulin Zhang
- Department of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, 100069, China; Beijing Institute of Hepatology, 100069, China.
| | - Yang Zhang
- Department of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, 100069, China.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Roheger M, Riemann S, Brauer A, McGowan E, Grittner U, Flöel A, Meinzer M. Non-pharmacological interventions for improving language and communication in people with primary progressive aphasia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2024; 5:CD015067. [PMID: 38808659 PMCID: PMC11134511 DOI: 10.1002/14651858.cd015067.pub2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/30/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) accounts for approximately 43% of frontotemporal dementias and is mainly characterised by a progressive impairment of speech and communication abilities. Three clinical variants have been identified: (a) non-fluent/agrammatic, (b) semantic, and (c) logopenic/phonological PPA variants. There is currently no curative treatment for PPA, and the disease progresses inexorably over time, with devastating effects on speech and communication ability, functional status, and quality of life. Several non-pharmacological interventions that may improve symptoms (e.g. different forms of language training and non-invasive brain stimulation) have been investigated in people with PPA. OBJECTIVES To assess the effects of non-pharmacological interventions for people with PPA on word retrieval (our primary outcome), global language functions, cognition, quality of life, and adverse events. SEARCH METHODS We searched the Cochrane Dementia and Cognitive Improvement Group's trial register, MEDLINE (Ovid SP), Embase (Ovid SP), four other databases and two other trial registers. The latest searches were run on 26 January 2024. SELECTION CRITERIA We included randomised controlled trials (RCTs) evaluating the effects of non-pharmacological interventions in people with PPA. DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS We used standard methodological procedures expected by Cochrane. MAIN RESULTS There were insufficient data available to conduct the network meta-analyses that we had originally planned (due to trial data being insufficiently reported or not reported at all, as well as the heterogeneous content of the included interventions). Therefore, we provide a descriptive summary of the included studies and results. We included 10 studies, with a total of 132 participants, evaluating non-pharmacological interventions. These were: transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) or repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) as stand-alone treatments (used by two and one studies, respectively); tDCS combined with semantic and phonological word-retrieval training (five studies); tDCS combined with semantic word-retrieval training (one study); and tDCS combined with phonological word-retrieval training (one study). Results for our primary outcome of word retrieval were mixed. For the two studies that investigated the effects of tDCS as stand-alone treatment compared to placebo ("sham") tDCS, we rated the results as having very low-certainty evidence. One study found a significant beneficial effect on word retrieval after active tDCS; one study did not report any significant effects in favour of the active tDCS group. Five studies investigated tDCS administered to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, inferior frontal cortex, left frontotemporal region, or the temporoparietal cortex, combined with semantic and phonological word-retrieval training. The most consistent finding was enhancement of word-retrieval ability for trained items immediately after the intervention, when behavioural training was combined with active tDCS compared to behavioural training plus sham tDCS. We found mixed effects for untrained items and maintenance of treatment effects during follow-up assessments. We rated the certainty of the evidence as very low in all studies. One study investigated tDCS combined with semantic word-retrieval training. Training was provided across 15 sessions with a frequency of three to five sessions per week, depending on the personal preferences of the participants. tDCS targeted the left frontotemporal region. The study included three participants: two received 1 mA stimulation and one received 2 mA stimulation. The study showed mixed results. We rated it as very low-certainty evidence. One study investigated tDCS combined with phonological word-retrieval training. Training was again provided across 15 sessions over a period of three weeks. tDCS targeted the left inferior frontal gyrus. This study showed a significantly more pronounced improvement for trained and untrained words in favour of the group that had received active tDCS, but we rated the certainty of the evidence as very low. One study compared active rTMS applied to an individually determined target site to active rTMS applied to a control site (vertex) for effects on participants' word retrieval. This study demonstrated better word retrieval for active rTMS administered to individually determined target brain regions than in the control intervention, but we rated the results as having a very low certainty of evidence. Four studies assessed overall language ability, three studies assessed cognition, five studies assessed potential adverse effects of brain stimulation, and one study investigated quality of life. AUTHORS' CONCLUSIONS There is currently no high-certainty evidence to inform clinical decision-making regarding non-pharmacological treatment selection for people with PPA. Preliminary evidence suggests that the combination of active tDCS with specific language therapy may improve impaired word retrieval for specifically trained items beyond the effects of behavioural treatment alone. However, more research is needed, including high-quality RCTs with detailed descriptions of participants and methods, and consideration of outcomes such as quality of life, depressive symptoms, and overall cognitive functioning. Moreover, studies assessing optimal treatments (i.e. behavioural interventions, brain stimulation interventions, and their combinations) for individual patients and PPA subtypes are needed. We were not able to conduct the planned (network) meta-analyses due to missing data that could not be obtained from most of the authors, a general lack of RCTs in the field, and heterogeneous interventions in eligible trials. Journals should implement a mandatory data-sharing requirement to assure transparency and accessibility of data from clinical trials.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mandy Roheger
- Department of Psychology, Carl von Ossietzky University Oldenburg, Oldenburg, Germany
| | - Steffen Riemann
- Department of Neurology, University Medicine Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
| | - Andreas Brauer
- Department of Neurology, University Medicine Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
| | - Ellen McGowan
- Speech and Language Therapy, Older People's Mental Health, Stockport, Pennine Care NHS Foundation Trust, Pennine Care NHS Foundation Trust, Stockport, UK
| | - Ulrike Grittner
- Berlin Institute of Health at Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Institute of Biometry and Clinical Epidemiology, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Agnes Flöel
- Department of Neurology, University Medicine Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
| | - Marcus Meinzer
- Department of Neurology, University Medicine Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Wang Z, Gallegos J, Tippett D, Onyike CU, Desmond JE, Hillis AE, Frangakis CE, Caffo B, Tsapkini K. Baseline functional connectivity predicts who will benefit from neuromodulation: evidence from primary progressive aphasia. MEDRXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR HEALTH SCIENCES 2024:2024.04.19.24305354. [PMID: 38699365 PMCID: PMC11065007 DOI: 10.1101/2024.04.19.24305354] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/05/2024]
Abstract
Background Identifying the characteristics of individuals who demonstrate response to an intervention allows us to predict who is most likely to benefit from certain interventions. Prediction is challenging in rare and heterogeneous diseases, such as primary progressive aphasia (PPA), that have varying clinical manifestations. We aimed to determine the characteristics of those who will benefit most from transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) using a novel heterogeneity and group identification analysis. Methods We compared the predictive ability of demographic and clinical patient characteristics (e.g., PPA variant and disease progression, baseline language performance) vs. functional connectivity alone (from resting-state fMRI) in the same cohort. Results Functional connectivity alone had the highest predictive value for outcomes, explaining 62% and 75% of tDCS effect of variance in generalization (semantic fluency) and in the trained outcome of the clinical trial (written naming), contrasted with <15% predicted by clinical characteristics, including baseline language performance. Patients with higher baseline functional connectivity between the left IFG (opercularis and triangularis), and between the middle temporal pole and posterior superior temporal gyrus, were most likely to benefit from tDCS. Conclusions We show the importance of a baseline 7-minute functional connectivity scan in predicting tDCS outcomes, and point towards a precision medicine approach in neuromodulation studies. The study has important implications for clinical trials and practice, providing a statistical method that addresses heterogeneity in patient populations and allowing accurate prediction and enrollment of those who will most likely benefit from specific interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zeyi Wang
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Jessica Gallegos
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Donna Tippett
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Chiadi U Onyike
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - John E Desmond
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Neuroscience Program, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Argye E Hillis
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Constantine E Frangakis
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Radiology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Brian Caffo
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Kyrana Tsapkini
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Akhmadullina DR, Konovalov RN, Shpilyukova YA, Fedotova EY, Illarioshkin SN. Anomia: Deciphering Functional Neuroanatomy in Primary Progressive Aphasia Variants. Brain Sci 2023; 13:1703. [PMID: 38137151 PMCID: PMC10741652 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci13121703] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2023] [Revised: 11/12/2023] [Accepted: 12/05/2023] [Indexed: 12/24/2023] Open
Abstract
Naming decline is one of the most common symptoms of primary progressive aphasia (PPA). Most studies on anomia in PPA are performed without taking into account PPA variants, especially for action naming. Only limited data are available for the neuroanatomical basis of anomia considering differences in the pathogenesis of PPAs. The aim of our study is to investigate the associations between anomia severity for both noun and verb naming and gray matter (GM) atrophy, as well as accompanying functional connectivity (FC) changes in three PPA variants. A total of 17 patients with non-fluent (nfvPPA), 11 with semantic (svPPA), and 9 with logopenic (lvPPA) PPA variants were included in the study and underwent cognitive/naming assessments and brain MRIs. Voxel-based morphometry was performed to evaluate GM volume. A resting-state functional MRI was applied to investigate FC changes in the identified GM areas. The study shows that different brain regions are involved in naming decline in each PPA variant with a predominantly temporal lobe involvement in svPPA, parietal lobe involvement in lvPPA, and frontal lobe involvement in nfvPPA. Separate data for object and action naming in PPA variants are provided. The obtained results mainly correspond to the current understanding of language processing and indicate that the evaluation of language impairments is preferable for each PPA variant separately. A further analysis of larger cohorts of patients is necessary to confirm these preliminary results.
Collapse
|
5
|
Jacobs M, Evans E, Ellis C. Intersectional sociodemographic and neurological relationships in the naming ability of persons with post-stroke aphasia. JOURNAL OF COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2023; 105:106352. [PMID: 37331326 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2023.106352] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/24/2022] [Revised: 05/16/2023] [Accepted: 06/08/2023] [Indexed: 06/20/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Significant attention has been given to the role of brain function and disruption in determining performance on naming tasks among individuals with aphasia. However, scholarly pursuit of a neurological explanation has overlooked the fundamental cornerstone of individual health-the underlying social, economic, and environmental factors that shape how they live, work, and age, also known as the social determinants of health (SDOH). This study examines the correlation between naming performance and these underlying factors. METHODS Individual level data from the 2010 Moss Aphasia Psycholinguistic Project Database (MAPPD) was matched with the 2009-2011 Medical Expenditure Panel Survey (MEPS) using a propensity score algorithm based on functional, health, and demographic characteristics. Multilevel, generalized, nonlinear regression models were applied to the resulting data set to assess the correlation between the Boston Naming Test (BNT) percentile score and age, income, sex, race, household size, marital status, aphasia type, and region of residence. Poisson regression models with bootstrapped standard errors were used to estimate these relationships RESULTS: Discrete dependent variable estimation with non-normal prior specification included individual level (age, marital status, years of education), socioeconomic (family income), health (aphasia type), household (family size), and environmental (region of residence) characteristics. Regression results indicated that, relative to individuals with Wernicke's, individuals with Anomic (0.74, SE = 0.0008) and Conduction (0.42, SE = 0.0009) aphasia performed better on the BNT. While age at the time of testing was not significantly correlated, higher income level (0.15, SE = 0.0003) and larger family size (0.002, SE = 0.002) was associated with higher BNT score percentiles. Finally, Black persons with aphasia (PWA) (-0.0124, SE = 0.0007) had lower average score percentiles when other factors were held constant. CONCLUSIONS The findings reported here suggest higher income and larger family size are associated with better outcomes. As expected, aphasia type was significantly associated with naming outcomes. However, poorer performance by Black PWA and individuals with low income suggests that SDOH can play a critical role (positive and negative) in naming impairment in some populations with aphasia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Molly Jacobs
- Health Services Research, Management and Policy, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, United States
| | - Elizabeth Evans
- Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, United States
| | - Charles Ellis
- Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Wang Z, Ficek BN, Webster KT, Herrmann O, Frangakis CE, Desmond JE, Onyike CU, Caffo B, Hillis AE, Tsapkini K. Specificity in Generalization Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Over the Left Inferior Frontal Gyrus in Primary Progressive Aphasia. Neuromodulation 2023; 26:850-860. [PMID: 37287321 PMCID: PMC10250817 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurom.2022.09.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/11/2022] [Revised: 08/30/2022] [Accepted: 09/01/2022] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Generalization (or near-transfer) effects of an intervention to tasks not explicitly trained are the most desirable intervention outcomes. However, they are rarely reported and even more rarely explained. One hypothesis for generalization effects is that the tasks improved share the same brain function/computation with the intervention task. We tested this hypothesis in this study of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) that is claimed to be involved in selective semantic retrieval of information from the temporal lobes. MATERIALS AND METHODS In this study, we examined whether tDCS over the left IFG in a group of patients with primary progressive aphasia (PPA), paired with a lexical/semantic retrieval intervention (oral and written naming), may specifically improve semantic fluency, a nontrained near-transfer task that relies on selective semantic retrieval, in patients with PPA. RESULTS Semantic fluency improved significantly more in the active tDCS than in the sham tDCS condition immediately after and two weeks after treatment. This improvement was marginally significant two months after treatment. We also found that the active tDCS effect was specific to tasks that require this IFG computation (selective semantic retrieval) but not to other tasks that may require different computations of the frontal lobes. CONCLUSIONS We provided interventional evidence that the left IFG is critical for selective semantic retrieval, and tDCS over the left IFG may have a near-transfer effect on tasks that depend on the same computation, even if they are not specifically trained. CLINICAL TRIAL REGISTRATION The Clinicaltrials.gov registration number for the study is NCT02606422.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zeyi Wang
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Bronte N Ficek
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Kimberly T Webster
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA; Department of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Olivia Herrmann
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Constantine E Frangakis
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA; Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA; Department of Radiology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - John E Desmond
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA; Neuroscience Program, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Chiadi U Onyike
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Brian Caffo
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Argye E Hillis
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA; Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA; Department of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Kyrana Tsapkini
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA; Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Meyer AM, Snider SF, Tippett DC, Saloma R, Turkeltaub PE, Hillis AE, Friedman RB. Baseline Conceptual-Semantic Impairment Predicts Longitudinal Treatment Effects for Anomia in Primary Progressive Aphasia and Alzheimer's Disease. APHASIOLOGY 2023; 38:205-236. [PMID: 38283767 PMCID: PMC10809875 DOI: 10.1080/02687038.2023.2183075] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2022] [Accepted: 02/16/2023] [Indexed: 01/30/2024]
Abstract
Background An individual's diagnostic subtype may fail to predict the efficacy of a given type of treatment for anomia. Classification by conceptual-semantic impairment may be more informative. Aims This study examined the effects of conceptual-semantic impairment and diagnostic subtype on anomia treatment effects in primary progressive aphasia (PPA) and Alzheimer's disease (AD). Methods & Procedures At baseline, the picture and word versions of the Pyramids and Palm Trees and Kissing and Dancing tests were used to measure conceptual-semantic processing. Based on norming that was conducted with unimpaired older adults, participants were classified as being impaired on both the picture and word versions (i.e., modality-general conceptual-semantic impairment), the picture version (Objects or Actions) only (i.e., visual-conceptual impairment), the word version (Nouns or Verbs) only (i.e., lexical-semantic impairment), or neither the picture nor the word version (i.e., no impairment). Following baseline testing, a lexical treatment and a semantic treatment were administered to all participants. The treatment stimuli consisted of nouns and verbs that were consistently named correctly at baseline (Prophylaxis items) and/or nouns and verbs that were consistently named incorrectly at baseline (Remediation items). Naming accuracy was measured at baseline, and it was measured at three, seven, eleven, fourteen, eighteen, and twenty-one months. Outcomes & Results Compared to baseline naming performance, lexical and semantic treatments both improved naming accuracy for treated Remediation nouns and verbs. For Prophylaxis items, lexical treatment was effective for both nouns and verbs, and semantic treatment was effective for verbs, but the pattern of results was different for nouns -- the effect of semantic treatment was initially nonsignificant or marginally significant, but it was significant beginning at 11 Months, suggesting that the effects of prophylactic semantic treatment may become more apparent as the disorder progresses. Furthermore, the interaction between baseline Conceptual-Semantic Impairment and the Treatment Condition (Lexical vs. Semantic) was significant for verb Prophylaxis items at 3 and 18 Months, and it was significant for noun Prophylaxis items at 14 and 18 Months. Conclusions The pattern of results suggested that individuals who have modality-general conceptual-semantic impairment at baseline are more likely to benefit from lexical treatment, while individuals who have unimpaired conceptual-semantic processing at baseline are more likely to benefit from semantic treatment as the disorder progresses. In contrast to conceptual-semantic impairment, diagnostic subtype did not typically predict the treatment effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron M. Meyer
- Center for Aphasia Research and Rehabilitation, Georgetown University Medical Center
| | - Sarah F. Snider
- Center for Aphasia Research and Rehabilitation, Georgetown University Medical Center
| | | | - Ryan Saloma
- Center for Aphasia Research and Rehabilitation, Georgetown University Medical Center
| | - Peter E. Turkeltaub
- Center for Aphasia Research and Rehabilitation, Georgetown University Medical Center
| | | | - Rhonda B. Friedman
- Center for Aphasia Research and Rehabilitation, Georgetown University Medical Center
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Breining BL, Faria AV, Tippett DC, Stockbridge MD, Meier EL, Caffo B, Hermann O, Friedman R, Meyer A, Tsapkini K, Hillis AE. Association of Regional Atrophy With Naming Decline in Primary Progressive Aphasia. Neurology 2023; 100:e582-e594. [PMID: 36319108 PMCID: PMC9946192 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000201491] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2022] [Accepted: 09/14/2022] [Indexed: 11/05/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) is a neurodegenerative condition that predominantly impairs language. Most investigations of how focal atrophy affects language consider 1 time point compared with healthy controls. However, true atrophy quantification requires comparing individual brains over time. In this observational cohort study, we identified areas where focal atrophy was associated with contemporaneous decline in naming in the same individuals. METHODS Cross-sectional analyses-related Boston Naming Test (BNT) performance and volume in 22 regions of interests (ROIs) at each time point using Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO) regression. Longitudinal analysis evaluated changes in BNT performance and change in volume in the same ROIs. RESULTS Participants (N = 62; 50% female; mean age = 66.8 ± 7.4 years) with PPA completed the BNT and MRI twice (mean = 343.9 ± 209.0 days apart). In cross-sectional left inferior frontal gyrus pars opercularis, superior temporal pole, middle temporal gyrus, and inferior temporal gyrus were identified as critical for naming at all time points. Longitudinal analysis revealed that increasing atrophy in the left supramarginal gyrus and middle temporal pole predicted greater naming decline, as did female sex and longer intervals between time points. DISCUSSION Although cross-sectional analyses identified classic language areas that were consistently related to poor performance at multiple time points, it was not increasing atrophy in these areas that lead to further decline: longitudinal analysis of each person's atrophy over time instead identified nearby but distinct regions where increased atrophy was related to decreasing performance. The results demonstrate that directly examining atrophy (in each individual) over time furthers understanding of decline in PPA and reveal the importance of left supramarginal gyrus and middle temporal pole in maintaining naming when areas normally critical for language degenerate. The novel results provide insight into how the underlying disease progresses to result in the clinical decline in naming, the deficit most common among all 3 PPA variants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bonnie L Breining
- From the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (B.L.B., A.V.F., D.C.T., M.D.S., E.L.M., O.H., K.T., A.E.H.), Baltimore, MD; Johns Hopkins University (B.C.), Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD; and Georgetown University (R.F., A.M.), Washington, DC
| | - Andreia Vasconcellos Faria
- From the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (B.L.B., A.V.F., D.C.T., M.D.S., E.L.M., O.H., K.T., A.E.H.), Baltimore, MD; Johns Hopkins University (B.C.), Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD; and Georgetown University (R.F., A.M.), Washington, DC
| | - Donna Clark Tippett
- From the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (B.L.B., A.V.F., D.C.T., M.D.S., E.L.M., O.H., K.T., A.E.H.), Baltimore, MD; Johns Hopkins University (B.C.), Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD; and Georgetown University (R.F., A.M.), Washington, DC
| | - Melissa Dawn Stockbridge
- From the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (B.L.B., A.V.F., D.C.T., M.D.S., E.L.M., O.H., K.T., A.E.H.), Baltimore, MD; Johns Hopkins University (B.C.), Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD; and Georgetown University (R.F., A.M.), Washington, DC
| | - Erin L Meier
- From the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (B.L.B., A.V.F., D.C.T., M.D.S., E.L.M., O.H., K.T., A.E.H.), Baltimore, MD; Johns Hopkins University (B.C.), Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD; and Georgetown University (R.F., A.M.), Washington, DC
| | - Brian Caffo
- From the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (B.L.B., A.V.F., D.C.T., M.D.S., E.L.M., O.H., K.T., A.E.H.), Baltimore, MD; Johns Hopkins University (B.C.), Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD; and Georgetown University (R.F., A.M.), Washington, DC
| | - Olivia Hermann
- From the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (B.L.B., A.V.F., D.C.T., M.D.S., E.L.M., O.H., K.T., A.E.H.), Baltimore, MD; Johns Hopkins University (B.C.), Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD; and Georgetown University (R.F., A.M.), Washington, DC
| | - Rhonda Friedman
- From the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (B.L.B., A.V.F., D.C.T., M.D.S., E.L.M., O.H., K.T., A.E.H.), Baltimore, MD; Johns Hopkins University (B.C.), Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD; and Georgetown University (R.F., A.M.), Washington, DC
| | - Aaron Meyer
- From the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (B.L.B., A.V.F., D.C.T., M.D.S., E.L.M., O.H., K.T., A.E.H.), Baltimore, MD; Johns Hopkins University (B.C.), Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD; and Georgetown University (R.F., A.M.), Washington, DC
| | - Kyrana Tsapkini
- From the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (B.L.B., A.V.F., D.C.T., M.D.S., E.L.M., O.H., K.T., A.E.H.), Baltimore, MD; Johns Hopkins University (B.C.), Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD; and Georgetown University (R.F., A.M.), Washington, DC
| | - Argye Elizabeth Hillis
- From the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (B.L.B., A.V.F., D.C.T., M.D.S., E.L.M., O.H., K.T., A.E.H.), Baltimore, MD; Johns Hopkins University (B.C.), Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD; and Georgetown University (R.F., A.M.), Washington, DC.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Herrmann O, Ficek B, Webster KT, Frangakis C, Spira AP, Tsapkini K. Sleep as a predictor of tDCS and language therapy outcomes. Sleep 2022; 45:zsab275. [PMID: 34875098 PMCID: PMC8919198 DOI: 10.1093/sleep/zsab275] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2021] [Revised: 10/01/2021] [Indexed: 12/17/2022] Open
Abstract
STUDY OBJECTIVES To determine whether sleep at baseline (before therapy) predicted improvements in language following either language therapy alone or coupled with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in individuals with primary progressive aphasia (PPA). METHODS Twenty-three participants with PPA (mean age 68.13 ± 6.21) received written naming/spelling therapy coupled with either anodal tDCS over the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) or sham condition in a crossover, sham-controlled, double-blind design (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT02606422). The outcome measure was percent of letters spelled correctly for trained and untrained words retrieved in a naming/spelling task. Given its particular importance as a sleep parameter in older adults, we calculated sleep efficiency (total sleep time/time in bed x100) based on subjective responses on the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). We grouped individuals based on a median split: high versus low sleep efficiency. RESULTS Participants with high sleep efficiency benefited more from written naming/spelling therapy than participants with low sleep efficiency in learning therapy materials (trained words). There was no effect of sleep efficiency in generalization of therapy materials to untrained words. Among participants with high sleep efficiency, those who received tDCS benefitted more from therapy than those who received sham condition. There was no additional benefit from tDCS in participants with low sleep efficiency. CONCLUSION Sleep efficiency modified the effects of language therapy and tDCS on language in participants with PPA. These results suggest sleep is a determinant of neuromodulation effects.Clinical Trial: tDCS Intervention in Primary Progressive Aphasia https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02606422.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Olivia Herrmann
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Bronte Ficek
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Kimberly T Webster
- Department of Otolaryngology, Head & Neck Surgery, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Constantine Frangakis
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Radiology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Adam P Spira
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Johns Hopkins Center on Aging and Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Kyrana Tsapkini
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Cognitive Science, The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Afthinos A, Themistocleous C, Herrmann O, Fan H, Lu H, Tsapkini K. The Contribution of Working Memory Areas to Verbal Learning and Recall in Primary Progressive Aphasia. Front Neurol 2022; 13:698200. [PMID: 35250797 PMCID: PMC8892377 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2022.698200] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/20/2021] [Accepted: 01/04/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent evidence of domain-specific working memory (WM) systems has identified the areas and networks which are involved in phonological, orthographic, and semantic WM, as well as in higher level domain-general WM functions. The contribution of these areas throughout the process of verbal learning and recall is still unclear. In the present study, we asked, what is the contribution of domain-specific specialized WM systems in the course of verbal learning and recall? To answer this question, we regressed the perfusion data from pseudo-continuous arterial spin labeling (pCASL) MRI with all the immediate, consecutive, and delayed recall stages of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) from a group of patients with Primary Progressive Aphasia (PPA), a neurodegenerative syndrome in which language is the primary deficit. We found that the early stages of verbal learning involve the areas with subserving phonological processing (left superior temporal gyrus), as well as semantic WM memory (left angular gyrus, AG_L). As learning unfolds, areas with subserving semantic WM (AG_L), as well as lexical/semantic (inferior temporal and fusiform gyri, temporal pole), and episodic memory (hippocampal complex) become more involved. Finally, a delayed recall depends entirely on semantic and episodic memory areas (hippocampal complex, temporal pole, and gyri). Our results suggest that AG_L subserving domain-specific (semantic) WM is involved only during verbal learning, but a delayed recall depends only on medial and cortical temporal areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandros Afthinos
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | | | - Olivia Herrmann
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | - Hongli Fan
- The Russell H. Morgan Department of Radiology & Radiological Science, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | - Hanzhang Lu
- The Russell H. Morgan Department of Radiology & Radiological Science, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
- F.M. Kirby Research Center for Functional Brain Imaging, Kennedy Krieger Research Institute, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | - Kyrana Tsapkini
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
- Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, United States
- *Correspondence: Kyrana Tsapkini
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Tippett DC, Keser Z. Clinical and neuroimaging characteristics of primary progressive aphasia. HANDBOOK OF CLINICAL NEUROLOGY 2022; 185:81-97. [PMID: 35078612 PMCID: PMC9951770 DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-823384-9.00016-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/31/2023]
Abstract
The chapter covers the clinical syndrome of a primary progressive aphasia (PPA), the demographics of this rare neurodegenerative disease, defining clinical and neuroanatomic characteristics of each PPA variant, disease progression, and behavioral features. The chapter begins with a brief introduction that includes references to seminal papers that defined this clinical syndrome and its three variants. The classic PPA subtypes discussed in the chapter are semantic variant PPA (svPPA), nonfluent/agrammatic PPA (nfaPPA), and logopenic variant PPA (lvPPA). The key language and cognitive characteristics, and language tasks that can elicit these language impairments, are detailed. Overlap in the clinical profiles of the PPA variants, which make differential diagnosis challenging, are explained. Disease progression is described, revealing that the PPA variants become more similar over time. Although PPA is language-predominant dementia, there are behavioral manifestations, particularly in svPPA. Changes in behavior in this variant are addressed as well as behavioral changes in nfaPPA and lvPPA that are less well recognized. The patterns of atrophy in the left temporal, parietal, and/or frontal cortices unique to each PPA variant are described. The underlying neuropathologies of the PPA variants are discussed, specifically tauopathies and non-tauopathies associated with svPPA and nfaPPA and Alzheimer's disease pathology in lvPPA.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Donna C. Tippett
- Departments of Neurology, Otolaryngology—Head and Neck Surgery, and Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland, United States
| | - Zafer Keser
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland, United States
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Riello M, Frangakis CE, Ficek B, Webster KT, Desmond JE, Faria AV, Hillis AE, Tsapkini K. Neural Correlates of Letter and Semantic Fluency in Primary Progressive Aphasia. Brain Sci 2021; 12:1. [PMID: 35053745 PMCID: PMC8773895 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci12010001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/22/2021] [Revised: 12/15/2021] [Accepted: 12/16/2021] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Verbal fluency (VF) is an informative cognitive task. Lesion and functional imaging studies implicate distinct cerebral areas that support letter versus semantic fluency and the understanding of neural and cognitive mechanisms underlying task performance. Most lesion studies include chronic stroke patients. People with primary progressive aphasia (PPA) provide complementary evidence for lesion-deficit associations, as different brain areas are affected in stroke versus PPA. In the present study we sought to determine imaging, clinical and demographic correlates of VF in PPA. Thirty-five patients with PPA underwent an assessment with letter and category VF tasks, evaluation of clinical features and an MRI scan for volumetric analysis. We used stepwise regression models to determine which brain areas are associated with VF performance while acknowledging the independent contribution of clinical and demographic factors. Letter fluency was predominantly associated with language severity (R2 = 38%), and correlated with the volume of the left superior temporal regions (R2 = 12%) and the right dorsolateral prefrontal area (R2 = 5%). Semantic fluency was predominantly associated with dementia severity (R2 = 47%) and correlated with the volume of the left inferior temporal gyrus (R2 = 7%). No other variables were significantly associated with performance in the two VF tasks. We concluded that, independently of disease severity, letter fluency is significantly associated with the volume of frontal and temporal areas whereas semantic fluency is associated mainly with the volume of temporal areas. Furthermore, our findings indicated that clinical severity plays a critical role in explaining VF performance in PPA, compared to the other clinical and demographic factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marianna Riello
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA; (M.R.); (B.F.); (K.T.W.); (J.E.D.); (A.E.H.)
| | - Constantine E. Frangakis
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD 21227, USA;
- Department of Radiology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21227, USA;
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21227, USA
| | - Bronte Ficek
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA; (M.R.); (B.F.); (K.T.W.); (J.E.D.); (A.E.H.)
| | - Kimberly T. Webster
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA; (M.R.); (B.F.); (K.T.W.); (J.E.D.); (A.E.H.)
- Department of Otolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21227, USA
| | - John E. Desmond
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA; (M.R.); (B.F.); (K.T.W.); (J.E.D.); (A.E.H.)
| | - Andreia V. Faria
- Department of Radiology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21227, USA;
| | - Argye E. Hillis
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA; (M.R.); (B.F.); (K.T.W.); (J.E.D.); (A.E.H.)
- Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA
| | - Kyrana Tsapkini
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA; (M.R.); (B.F.); (K.T.W.); (J.E.D.); (A.E.H.)
- Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
The Impact of Primary Progressive Aphasia on Picture Naming and General Language Ability. Cogn Behav Neurol 2021; 34:188-199. [PMID: 34473670 DOI: 10.1097/wnn.0000000000000275] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/22/2020] [Accepted: 12/23/2020] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) is a clinical syndrome that is characterized by progressive deterioration of language while other cognitive domains remain relatively intact. The extent to which print exposure and cortical volume atrophy jointly influence picture naming and general language ability in individuals with PPA remains underexplored. OBJECTIVE To investigate the language performance of individuals with the nonfluent variant of primary progressive aphasia (nfvPPA) and to explore the impact of print exposure and cortical volume atrophy on their language ability. METHOD We compared 14 Greek individuals with nfvPPA and similar age, education, disease duration, and cognitive ability with age-, gender- and education-matched Greek controls on picture naming and on language tasks of the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination-Short Form, including oral word reading, word and sentence repetition, complex ideational material, and reading comprehension. The effects of print exposure and left-hemisphere cortical volume on the individuals' language performance were estimated through stepwise regression models. RESULTS The language performance of the individuals with nfvPPA was affected by print exposure and cortical volume atrophy. Picture naming and word reading were affected by print exposure. The highest contributions of cortical volume atrophy were found for the repetition, complex ideational material, and reading comprehension tasks. CONCLUSION Print exposure and cortical volume atrophy may help explain variability in the language performance of nfvPPA individuals with similar age, education, disease duration, and cognitive ability.
Collapse
|
14
|
Lukic S, Borghesani V, Weis E, Welch A, Bogley R, Neuhaus J, Deleon J, Miller ZA, Kramer JH, Miller BL, Dronkers NF, Gorno-Tempini ML. Dissociating nouns and verbs in temporal and perisylvian networks: Evidence from neurodegenerative diseases. Cortex 2021; 142:47-61. [PMID: 34182153 PMCID: PMC8556704 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.05.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2020] [Revised: 02/27/2021] [Accepted: 05/08/2021] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
Naming of nouns and verbs can be selectively impaired in neurological disorders, but the specificity of the neural and cognitive correlates of such dissociation remains unclear. Functional imaging and stroke research sought to identify cortical regions selectively recruited for nouns versus verbs, yet findings are inconsistent. The present study investigated this issue in neurodegenerative diseases known to selectively affect different brain networks, thus providing new critical evidence of network specificity. We examined naming performances on nouns and verbs in 146 patients with different neurodegenerative syndromes (Primary Progressive Aphasia - PPA, Alzheimer's disease - AD, and behavioral variant Frontotemporal Dementia - FTD) and 30 healthy adults. We then correlated naming scores with MRI-derived cortical thickness values as well as with performances in semantic and syntactic tasks, across all subjects. Results indicated that patients with the semantic variant PPA named significantly fewer nouns than verbs. Instead, nonfluent/agrammatic PPA patients named fewer verbs than nouns. Across all subjects, performance on nouns (adjusted for verbs) specifically correlated with cortical atrophy in left anterior temporal regions, and performance on verbs (adjusted for nouns) with atrophy in left inferior and middle frontal, inferior parietal and posterior temporal regions. Furthermore, lower lexical-semantic abilities correlated with deficits in naming both nouns and verbs, while lower syntactic abilities only correlated with naming verbs. Our results show that different neural and cognitive mechanisms underlie naming of specific grammatical categories in neurodegenerative diseases. Importantly, our findings showed that verb processing depends on a widespread perisylvian networks, suggesting that some regions might be involved in processing different types of action knowledge. These findings have important implications for early differential diagnosis of neurodegenerative disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sladjana Lukic
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA.
| | - Valentina Borghesani
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Elizabeth Weis
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Ariane Welch
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Rian Bogley
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - John Neuhaus
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Jessica Deleon
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Zachary A Miller
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Joel H Kramer
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Bruce L Miller
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Nina F Dronkers
- University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA; University of California, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Maria L Gorno-Tempini
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Breining BL, Faria AV, Caffo B, Meier EL, Sheppard SM, Sebastian R, Tippett DC, Hillis AE. Neural regions underlying object and action naming: Complementary evidence from acute stroke and primary progressive aphasia. APHASIOLOGY 2021; 36:732-760. [PMID: 35832655 PMCID: PMC9272983 DOI: 10.1080/02687038.2021.1907291] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2020] [Accepted: 03/11/2021] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Naming impairment is commonly noted in individuals with aphasia. However, object naming receives more attention than action naming. Furthermore, most studies include participants with aphasia due to only one aetiology, commonly stroke. We developed a new assessment, the Hopkins Action Naming Assessment (HANA), to evaluate action naming impairments. AIMS Our aims were to show that the HANA is a useful tool that can (1) identify action naming impairments and (2) be used to investigate the neural substrates underlying naming. We paired the HANA with the Boston Naming Test (BNT) to compare action and object naming. We considered participants with aphasia due to primary progressive aphasia (PPA) or acute left hemisphere stroke to provide a more comprehensive picture of brain-behaviour relationships critical for naming. Behaviourally, we hypothesised that there would be a double dissociation between object and action naming performance. Neuroanatomically, we hypothesised that different neural substrates would be implicated in object vs. action naming and that different lesion-deficit associations would be identified in participants with PPA vs. acute stroke. METHODS & PROCEDURES Participants (N=138 with PPA, N=37 with acute stroke) completed the BNT and HANA. Behavioural performance was compared. A subset of participants (N=31 with PPA, N=37 with acute stroke) provided neuroimaging data. The whole brain was automatically segmented into regions of interest (ROIs). For participants with PPA, the image variables were the ROI volumes, normalised by the brain volume. For participants with acute stroke, the image variables were the percentage of each ROI affected by the lesion. The relationship between ROIs likely to be involved in naming performance was modelled with LASSO regression. OUTCOMES & RESULTS Behavioural results showed a double dissociation in performance: in each group, some participants displayed intact performance relative to healthy controls on actions but not objects and/or significantly better performance on actions than objects, while others showed the opposite pattern. These results support the need to assess both objects and actions when evaluating naming deficits. Neuroimaging results identified different regions associated with object vs. action naming, implicating overlapping but distinct networks of regions. Furthermore, results differed for participants with PPA vs. acute stroke, indicating that critical information may be missed when only one aetiology is considered. CONCLUSIONS Overall, the study provides a more comprehensive picture of the neural bases of naming, underscoring the importance of assessing both objects and actions and considering different aetiologies of damage. It demonstrates the utility of the HANA.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bonnie L. Breining
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
| | - Andreia V. Faria
- Department of Radiology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
| | - Brian Caffo
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
| | - Erin L. Meier
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
| | - Shannon M. Sheppard
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
- Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders, Chapman University, Irvine, CA 92618, USA
| | - Rajani Sebastian
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
| | - Donna C. Tippett
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
- Department of Otolaryngology - Head and Neck Surgery, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
| | - Argye E. Hillis
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
- Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Cho S, Nevler N, Ash S, Shellikeri S, Irwin DJ, Massimo L, Rascovsky K, Olm C, Grossman M, Liberman M. Automated analysis of lexical features in frontotemporal degeneration. Cortex 2021; 137:215-231. [PMID: 33640853 PMCID: PMC8044033 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.01.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/26/2020] [Revised: 07/26/2020] [Accepted: 01/22/2021] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
We implemented an automated analysis of lexical aspects of semi-structured speech produced by healthy elderly controls (n = 37) and three patient groups with frontotemporal degeneration (FTD): behavioral variant FTD (n = 74), semantic variant primary progressive aphasia (svPPA, n = 42), and nonfluent/agrammatic PPA (naPPA, n = 22). Based on previous findings, we hypothesized that the three patient groups and controls would differ in the counts of part-of-speech (POS) categories and several lexical measures. With a natural language processing program, we automatically tagged POS categories of all words produced during a picture description task. We further counted the number of wh-words, and we rated nouns for abstractness, ambiguity, frequency, familiarity, and age of acquisition. We also computed the cross-entropy estimation, where low cross-entropy indicates high predictability, and lexical diversity for each description. We validated a subset of the POS data that were automatically tagged with the Google Universal POS scheme using gold-standard POS data tagged by a linguist, and we found that the POS categories from our automated methods were more than 90% accurate. For svPPA patients, we found fewer unique nouns than in naPPA and more pronouns and wh-words than in the other groups. We also found high abstractness, ambiguity, frequency, and familiarity for nouns and the lowest cross-entropy estimation among all groups. These measures were associated with cortical thinning in the left temporal lobe. In naPPA patients, we found increased speech errors and partial words compared to controls, and these impairments were associated with cortical thinning in the left middle frontal gyrus. bvFTD patients' adjective production was decreased compared to controls and was correlated with their apathy scores. Their adjective production was associated with cortical thinning in the dorsolateral frontal and orbitofrontal gyri. Our results demonstrate distinct language profiles in subgroups of FTD patients and validate our automated method of analyzing FTD patients' speech.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sunghye Cho
- Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
| | - Naomi Nevler
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Sharon Ash
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Sanjana Shellikeri
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - David J Irwin
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Lauren Massimo
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Katya Rascovsky
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Christopher Olm
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA; Department of Radiology and Penn Image Computing and Science Laboratory, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Murray Grossman
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Mark Liberman
- Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Themistocleous C, Webster K, Afthinos A, Tsapkini K. Part of Speech Production in Patients With Primary Progressive Aphasia: An Analysis Based on Natural Language Processing. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2021; 30:466-480. [PMID: 32697669 PMCID: PMC8702871 DOI: 10.1044/2020_ajslp-19-00114] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2019] [Revised: 02/14/2020] [Accepted: 05/13/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Background Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) is a neurodegenerative disorder characterized by a progressive decline of language functions. Its symptoms are grouped into three PPA variants: nonfluent PPA, logopenic PPA, and semantic PPA. Grammatical deficiencies differ depending on the PPA variant. Aims This study aims to determine the differences between PPA variants with respect to part of speech (POS) production and to identify morphological markers that classify PPA variants using machine learning. By fulfilling these aims, the overarching goal is to provide objective measures that can facilitate clinical diagnosis, evaluation, and prognosis. Method and Procedure Connected speech productions from PPA patients produced in a picture description task were transcribed, and the POS class of each word was estimated using natural language processing, namely, POS tagging. We then implemented a twofold analysis: (a) linear regression to determine how patients with nonfluent PPA, semantic PPA, and logopenic PPA variants differ in their POS productions and (b) a supervised classification analysis based on POS using machine learning models (i.e., random forests, decision trees, and support vector machines) to subtype PPA variants and generate feature importance (FI). Outcome and Results Using an automated analysis of a short picture description task, this study showed that content versus function words can distinguish patients with nonfluent PPA, semantic PPA, and logopenic PPA variants. Verbs were less important as distinguishing features of patients with different PPA variants than earlier thought. Finally, the study showed that among the most important distinguishing features of PPA variants were elaborative speech elements, such as adjectives and adverbs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Kimberly Webster
- Department of Otolaryngology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore MD
| | | | - Kyrana Tsapkini
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD
- Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore MD
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Cho S, Nevler N, Ash S, Shellikeri S, Irwin DJ, Massimo L, Rascovsky K, Olm C, Grossman M, Liberman M. Automated analysis of lexical features in Frontotemporal Degeneration. MEDRXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR HEALTH SCIENCES 2020:2020.09.10.20192054. [PMID: 33173922 PMCID: PMC7654918 DOI: 10.1101/2020.09.10.20192054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
We implemented an automated analysis of lexical aspects of semi-structured speech produced by healthy elderly controls (n=37) and three patient groups with frontotemporal degeneration (FTD): behavioral variant FTD (n=74), semantic variant primary progressive aphasia (svPPA, n=42), and nonfluent/agrammatic PPA (naPPA, n=22). Based on previous findings, we hypothesized that the three patient groups and controls would differ in the counts of part-of-speech (POS) categories and several lexical measures. With a natural language processing program, we automatically tagged POS categories of all words produced during a picture description task. We further counted the number of wh -words, and we rated nouns for abstractness, ambiguity, frequency, familiarity, and age of acquisition. We also computed the cross-entropy estimation, which is a measure of word predictability, and lexical diversity for each description. We validated a subset of the POS data that were automatically tagged with the Google Universal POS scheme using gold-standard POS data tagged by a linguist, and we found that the POS categories from our automated methods were more than 90% accurate. For svPPA patients, we found fewer unique nouns than in naPPA and more pronouns and wh -words than in the other groups. We also found high abstractness, ambiguity, frequency, and familiarity for nouns and the lowest cross-entropy estimation among all groups. These measures were associated with cortical thinning in the left temporal lobe. In naPPA patients, we found increased speech errors and partial words compared to controls, and these impairments were associated with cortical thinning in the left middle frontal gyrus. bvFTD patients' adjective production was decreased compared to controls and was correlated with their apathy scores. Their adjective production was associated with cortical thinning in the dorsolateral frontal and orbitofrontal gyri. Our results demonstrate distinct language profiles in subgroups of FTD patients and validate our automated method of analyzing FTD patients' speech.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sunghye Cho
- Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Naomi Nevler
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Sharon Ash
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Sanjana Shellikeri
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - David J. Irwin
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Lauren Massimo
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Katya Rascovsky
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Christopher Olm
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
- Department of Radiology and Penn Image Computing and Science Laboratory, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Murray Grossman
- Department of Neurology and Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Mark Liberman
- Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Faroqi-Shah Y, Treanor A, Ratner NB, Ficek B, Webster K, Tsapkini K. Using narratives in differential diagnosis of neurodegenerative syndromes. JOURNAL OF COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2020; 85:105994. [PMID: 32388191 PMCID: PMC7304645 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2020.105994] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2019] [Revised: 03/25/2020] [Accepted: 03/29/2020] [Indexed: 05/16/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Language decline has been associated with healthy aging and with various neurodegenerative conditions, making it challenging to differentiate among these conditions. This study examined the utility of linguistic measures derived from a short narrative language sample for 1) identifying language characteristics and cut-off scores to differentiate between healthy aging, Primary Progressive Aphasia (PPA), Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), and Alzheimer's dementia (AD); and 2) differentiating among PPA variants in which language is the primary impairment. METHOD Participants were 25 neurologically healthy English speakers, 20 individuals with MCI, 20 with AD, and 26 with PPA (non-fluent/agrammatic N = 10, logopenic N = 9, semantic N = 7). Narrative language samples of the Cookie Theft Picture of persons with healthy aging, MCI and AD were retrospectively obtained from the DementiaBank database (https://talkbank.org/DementiaBank/) and PPA samples were obtained from an ongoing research study. The language samples were analyzed for fluency, word retrieval success, grammatical accuracy, and errors using automated and manual analysis methods. The sensitivity and specificity of various language measures was computed. RESULTS Participants with PPA scored lower than neurologically healthy and MCI groups on fluency (words per minute and disfluencies), word retrieval (Correct Information Units and number of errors), and sentence grammaticality. PPA and AD groups did not differ on language measures. Agrammatic PPA participants scored lower than logopenic and semantic PPA groups on several measures, while logopenic and semantic PPA did not differ on any measures. CONCLUSION Measures derived from brief language samples and analyzed using mostly automated methods are clinically useful in differentiating PPA from healthy aging and MCI, and agrammatic PPA from other variants. The sensitivity and specificity of these measures is modest and can be improved when coupled with clinical presentation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yasmeen Faroqi-Shah
- University of Maryland, Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, United States.
| | - Ashlyn Treanor
- University of Maryland, Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, United States
| | - Nan Bernstein Ratner
- University of Maryland, Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, United States
| | - Bronte Ficek
- Johns Hopkins University, Department of Neurology, United States
| | - Kimberly Webster
- Johns Hopkins University, Department of Neurology, United States
| | - Kyrana Tsapkini
- Johns Hopkins University, Department of Neurology, United States
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Meyer AM, Snider SF, McGowan SA, Tippett DC, Hillis AE, Friedman RB. Grammatical Ability Predicts Relative Action Naming Impairment in Primary Progressive Aphasia. APHASIOLOGY 2020; 34:664-674. [PMID: 33716376 PMCID: PMC7954137 DOI: 10.1080/02687038.2020.1734527] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2019] [Accepted: 02/20/2020] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Findings from several studies have indicated that participants with nfvPPA and participants with svPPA exhibit different patterns on action and object naming tasks, while other recent studies have found that neither participants with nfvPPA nor participants with svPPA show a significant difference in accuracy between object naming and action naming. AIMS The goal of this study was to test the hypothesis that relative action naming impairment is associated with grammatical ability in PPA, rather than a specific subtype of PPA. METHODS & PROCEDURES Thirty-four participants with PPA completed the Boston Naming Test, the Action Naming subtest of the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, and the Northwestern Anagram Test, which was used to measure grammatical ability. Z-scores for the two naming tasks were calculated based on normative data from unimpaired controls. For each participant with PPA, the relative action naming impairment was calculated by subtracting the object naming z-score from the action naming z-score. Linear regression analysis was then used to evaluate the role of grammatical ability as a predictor of relative action naming impairment, while controlling for age, education, cognitive ability (as measured by the Montreal Cognitive Assessment), and semantic ability (as measured by the Pyramids and Palm Trees test). The interaction between grammatical ability and each control variable was also examined. OUTCOMES & RESULTS The main effect of grammatical ability was a significant predictor of relative action naming impairment, while none of the control variables was a significant predictor. However, the interaction between grammatical ability and semantic ability was also significant. CONCLUSIONS Individuals who have both grammatical impairment and semantic impairment have the largest relative action naming impairment. These individuals may benefit from a treatment that focuses on the retrieval of verbs and their arguments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron M. Meyer
- Center for Aphasia Research and Rehabilitation, Georgetown University Medical Center
| | - Sarah F. Snider
- Center for Aphasia Research and Rehabilitation, Georgetown University Medical Center
| | - Shelby A. McGowan
- Center for Aphasia Research and Rehabilitation, Georgetown University Medical Center
| | | | | | - Rhonda B. Friedman
- Center for Aphasia Research and Rehabilitation, Georgetown University Medical Center
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Bruffaerts R, Schaeverbeke J, De Weer AS, Nelissen N, Dries E, Van Bouwel K, Sieben A, Bergmans B, Swinnen C, Pijnenburg Y, Sunaert S, Vandenbulcke M, Vandenberghe R. Multivariate analysis reveals anatomical correlates of naming errors in primary progressive aphasia. Neurobiol Aging 2019; 88:71-82. [PMID: 31955981 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.12.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/24/2019] [Revised: 12/10/2019] [Accepted: 12/15/2019] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) is an overarching term for a heterogeneous group of neurodegenerative diseases which affect language processing. Impaired picture naming has been linked to atrophy of the anterior temporal lobe in the semantic variant of PPA. Although atrophy of the anterior temporal lobe proposedly impairs picture naming by undermining access to semantic knowledge, picture naming also entails object recognition and lexical retrieval. Using multivariate analysis, we investigated whether cortical atrophy relates to different types of naming errors generated during picture naming in 43 PPA patients (13 semantic, 9 logopenic, 11 nonfluent, and 10 mixed variant). Omissions were associated with atrophy of the anterior temporal lobes. Semantic errors, for example, mistaking a rhinoceros for a hippopotamus, were associated with atrophy of the left mid and posterior fusiform cortex and the posterior middle and inferior temporal gyrus. Semantic errors and atrophy in these regions occurred in each PPA subtype, without major between-subtype differences. We propose that pathological changes to neural mechanisms associated with semantic errors occur across the PPA spectrum.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rose Bruffaerts
- Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium; Neurology Department, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.
| | - Jolien Schaeverbeke
- Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - An-Sofie De Weer
- Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Natalie Nelissen
- Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Eva Dries
- Neurology Department, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Karen Van Bouwel
- Neurology Department, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Anne Sieben
- Neurology Department, University Hospital Ghent, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Bruno Bergmans
- Neurology Department, University Hospital Ghent, Ghent, Belgium; Neurology Department, AZ Sint-Jan Brugge-Oostende AV, Bruges, Belgium
| | | | - Yolande Pijnenburg
- Neurology Department, Alzheimer Center Amsterdam, Amsterdam Neuroscience, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam UMC, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Stefan Sunaert
- Radiology Department, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | | | - Rik Vandenberghe
- Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium; Neurology Department, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Fenner AS, Webster KT, Ficek BN, Frangakis CE, Tsapkini K. Written Verb Naming Improves After tDCS Over the Left IFG in Primary Progressive Aphasia. Front Psychol 2019; 10:1396. [PMID: 31249546 PMCID: PMC6582664 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01396] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/16/2018] [Accepted: 05/29/2019] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), a non-invasive neuromodulation technique, is an effective adjunct to naming treatments in post-stroke aphasia and primary progressive aphasia (PPA). Enhanced performance in oral and written naming and spelling of nouns with tDCS has been quantified in detail, but it is not known whether it is effective for verb treatment in PPA. We addressed the question of whether performance in naming and spelling of verbs can be augmented with anodal tDCS over the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). We compared tDCS coupled with oral and written verb naming/spelling treatment with oral and written verb naming/spelling treatment alone. In a double-blind, sham-controlled, crossover design, 11 participants with logopenic or non-fluent variant PPA received approximately 15 consecutive sessions of anodal tDCS and sham over the left IFG coupled with oral and written verb-naming + spelling treatment. Written verb-naming performance improved significantly more for trained verbs in the tDCS than the sham condition. Importantly, tDCS effects generalized to untrained items for written verb naming and were significant even at 2 months post-treatment. We conclude that tDCS over the left IFG can improve written verb naming and spelling in PPA.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amberlynn S. Fenner
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | - Kimberly T. Webster
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
- Department of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | - Bronte N. Ficek
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | - Constantine E. Frangakis
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
- Department of Radiology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | - Kyrana Tsapkini
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
- Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, United States
| |
Collapse
|