1
|
Mukunda CL, Sane SP. Encoding of antennal position and velocity by the Johnston's organ in hawkmoths. J Exp Biol 2025; 228:jeb249342. [PMID: 40099381 PMCID: PMC12079665 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.249342] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/29/2024] [Accepted: 03/06/2025] [Indexed: 03/19/2025]
Abstract
Insect antennae function as versatile, multimodal sensory probes in diverse behavioural contexts. In addition to their primary role as olfactory organs, they serve essential mechanosensory functions across insects, including auditory perception, vestibular feedback, airflow detection, gravity sensing and tactile sensation. These diverse functions are facilitated by the mechanosensory Johnston's organ (JO), located at the joint between the flagellum and the pedicel (second antennal segment). This joint lacks muscles, which means that JOs can perceive only passive deflections of the flagellum. Earlier work that characterized the sensitivity and short response time of the JO sensory units in hawkmoths showed that their sensitivity to a broad frequency range is range-fractionated. This vastly expands the functional repertoire of the JO. However, it is not clear what components of antennal kinematics are encoded by the JO. Here, we conducted experiments to test the hypothesis that JO neurons encode the position and velocity of angular movements of the flagellum. We recorded intracellularly from the axons of primary sensory neurons of the JO while stimulating it with ramp-and-hold stimuli in which either the antennal position or antennal angular velocity was maintained at various constant values. Our study shows that JO neurons encode angular velocity and position of the antenna in their response. We also characterized the neural adaptation of the responses to angular velocities and positions. The majority of neurons were sensitive to a movement in the ventrad direction, in the direction of gravity. The adaptation and directional response properties give rise to a nonlinear hysteresis-like response. Together, these findings highlight the neurophysiological basis underlying the functional versatility of the JO.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chinmayee L. Mukunda
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, GKVK Campus, Bellary Road, Bangalore 560065, India
| | - Sanjay P. Sane
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, GKVK Campus, Bellary Road, Bangalore 560065, India
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
McCarter MG, Loudon C. The spatial arrangement of Böhm's bristles and resolution of joint position in cricket antennae (Acheta domesticus) (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). JOURNAL OF INSECT SCIENCE (ONLINE) 2025; 25:22. [PMID: 40289659 PMCID: PMC12034924 DOI: 10.1093/jisesa/ieaf039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2024] [Revised: 03/03/2025] [Accepted: 03/11/2025] [Indexed: 04/30/2025]
Abstract
Böhm's bristles are mechanosensory structures located on the basal segments of antennae of most insects and provide sensory feedback for antennal positioning. The bristles are located near the head-scape and scape-pedicel joints, and evidence demonstrates that as the joints bend, the bristles are mechanically deflected by contact with the reconfiguring flexible cuticle of the joint, providing sensory information about joint angles. If the Böhm's bristles are only detecting joint position, and the full range of motion of that joint is detected by the array of Böhm's bristles, it follows that the extent of the array should correspond to the length of the segment covered by the flexible cuticle during bending. The spatial arrangement of the bristles should provide insight into the joint movement range, joint type, and the resolution of the joint angle available as sensory input. To evaluate this, we characterized the Böhm's bristles in Acheta domesticus (L.) (two hinge joints) using scanning electron microscopy (SEM). Our SEM analysis revealed one field of Böhm's bristles on the dorsal side and two on the ventral side of the scape, and two additional fields of Böhm's bristles on the lateral and medial sides of the pedicel. Bristles are positioned such that they are all deflected when the scape or pedicel is completely bent; thus, the arrangement of bristles matches the range of joint movement. In addition, we suggest that the number of bristles in the direction of joint movement may indicate the resolution of detection of the movement of the joint.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marlo G McCarter
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, U.C. Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA
| | - Catherine Loudon
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, U.C. Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Claverie N, Buvat P, Casas J. Active Sensing in Bees Through Antennal Movements Is Independent of Odor Molecule. Integr Comp Biol 2023; 63:315-331. [PMID: 36958852 DOI: 10.1093/icb/icad010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/17/2022] [Revised: 03/08/2023] [Accepted: 03/13/2023] [Indexed: 03/25/2023] Open
Abstract
When sampling odors, many insects are moving their antennae in a complex but repeatable fashion. Previous studies with bees have tracked antennal movements in only two dimensions, with a low sampling rate and with relatively few odorants. A detailed characterization of the multimodal antennal movement patterns as function of olfactory stimuli is thus wanted. The aim of this study is to test for a relationship between the scanning movements and the properties of the odor molecule. We tracked several key locations on the antennae of bumblebees at high frequency and in three dimensions while stimulating the insect with puffs of 11 common odorants released in a low-speed continuous flow. Water and paraffin were used as negative controls. Movement analysis was done with the neural network Deeplabcut. Bees use a stereotypical oscillating motion of their antennae when smelling odors, similar across all bees, independently of the identity of the odors and hence their diffusivity and vapor pressure. The variability in the movement amplitude among odors is as large as between individuals. The main type of oscillation at low frequencies and large amplitude is triggered by the presence of an odor and is in line with previous work, as is the speed of movement. The second oscillation mode at higher frequencies and smaller amplitudes is constantly present. Antennae are quickly deployed when a stimulus is perceived, decorrelate their movement trajectories rapidly, and oscillate vertically with a large amplitude and laterally with a smaller one. The cone of airspace thus sampled was identified through the 3D understanding of the motion patterns. The amplitude and speed of antennal scanning movements seem to be function of the internal state of the animal, rather than determined by the odorant. Still, bees display an active olfactory sampling strategy. First, they deploy their antennae when perceiving an odor. Second, fast vertical scanning movements further increase the odorant capture rate. Finally, lateral movements might enhance the likelihood to locate the source of odor, similarly to the lateral scanning movement of insects at odor plume boundaries.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicolas Claverie
- Institut de Recherche sur la Biologie de l'Insecte, Université de Tours, 37200 Tours, France
- CEA le Ripault, Centre d'études du Ripault, 37260 Monts, France
| | - Pierrick Buvat
- CEA le Ripault, Centre d'études du Ripault, 37260 Monts, France
| | - Jérôme Casas
- Institut de Recherche sur la Biologie de l'Insecte, Université de Tours, 37200 Tours, France
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Verbe A, Martinez D, Viollet S. Sensory fusion in the hoverfly righting reflex. Sci Rep 2023; 13:6138. [PMID: 37061548 PMCID: PMC10105705 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-33302-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2023] [Accepted: 04/11/2023] [Indexed: 04/17/2023] Open
Abstract
We study how falling hoverflies use sensory cues to trigger appropriate roll righting behavior. Before being released in a free fall, flies were placed upside-down with their legs contacting the substrate. The prior leg proprioceptive information about their initial orientation sufficed for the flies to right themselves properly. However, flies also use visual and antennal cues to recover faster and disambiguate sensory conflicts. Surprisingly, in one of the experimental conditions tested, hoverflies flew upside-down while still actively flapping their wings. In all the other conditions, flies were able to right themselves using two roll dynamics: fast ([Formula: see text]50ms) and slow ([Formula: see text]110ms) in the presence of consistent and conflicting cues, respectively. These findings suggest that a nonlinear sensory integration of the three types of sensory cues occurred. A ring attractor model was developed and discussed to account for this cue integration process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna Verbe
- Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, ISM, 13009, Marseille, France
- PNI, Princeton University, Washington Road, Princeton, NJ, 08540, USA
| | - Dominique Martinez
- Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, ISM, 13009, Marseille, France
- Université de Lorraine, CNRS, LORIA, 54000, Nancy, France
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Suver MP, Medina AM, Nagel KI. Active antennal movements in Drosophila can tune wind encoding. Curr Biol 2023; 33:780-789.e4. [PMID: 36731464 PMCID: PMC9992063 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.01.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/29/2022] [Revised: 12/16/2022] [Accepted: 01/10/2023] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
Insects use their antennae to smell odors,1,2 detect auditory cues,3,4 and sense mechanosensory stimuli such as wind5 and objects,6,7,8 frequently by combining sensory processing with active movements. Genetic access to antennal motor systems would therefore provide a powerful tool for dissecting the circuit mechanisms underlying active sensing, but little is known about how the most genetically tractable insect, Drosophila melanogaster, moves its antennae. Here, we use deep learning to measure how tethered Drosophila move their antennae in the presence of sensory stimuli and identify genetic reagents for controlling antennal movement. We find that flies perform both slow adaptive movements and fast flicking movements in response to wind-induced deflections, but not the attractive odor apple cider vinegar. Next, we describe four muscles in the first antennal segment that control antennal movements and identify genetic driver lines that provide access to two groups of antennal motor neurons and an antennal muscle. Through optogenetic inactivation, we provide evidence that antennal motor neurons contribute to active movements with different time courses. Finally, we show that activation of antennal motor neurons and muscles can adjust the gain and acuity of wind direction encoding by antennal displacement. Together, our experiments provide insight into the neural control of antennal movement and suggest that active antennal positioning in Drosophila may tune the precision of wind encoding.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marie P Suver
- Neuroscience Institute, NYU Langone Medical Center, 435 E 30(th) St., New York, NY 10016, USA
| | - Ashley M Medina
- Neuroscience Institute, NYU Langone Medical Center, 435 E 30(th) St., New York, NY 10016, USA
| | - Katherine I Nagel
- Neuroscience Institute, NYU Langone Medical Center, 435 E 30(th) St., New York, NY 10016, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Chatterjee P, Mohan U, Sane SP. Small-amplitude head oscillations result from a multimodal head stabilization reflex in hawkmoths. Biol Lett 2022; 18:20220199. [PMID: 36349580 PMCID: PMC9653261 DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2022.0199] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/22/2022] [Accepted: 10/11/2022] [Indexed: 11/11/2023] Open
Abstract
In flying insects, head stabilization is an essential reflex that helps to reduce motion blur during fast aerial manoeuvres. This reflex is multimodal and requires the integration of visual and antennal mechanosensory feedback in hawkmoths, each operating as a negative-feedback-control loop. As in any negative-feedback system, the head stabilization system possesses inherent oscillatory dynamics that depend on the rate at which the sensorimotor components of the reflex operate. Consistent with this expectation, we observed small-amplitude oscillations in the head motion (or head wobble) of the oleander hawkmoth, Daphnis nerii, which are accentuated when sensory feedback is aberrant. Here, we show that these oscillations emerge from the inherent dynamics of the multimodal reflex underlying gaze stabilization, and that the amplitude of head wobble is a function of both the visual feedback and antennal mechanosensory feedback from the Johnston's organs. Our data support the hypothesis that head wobble results from a multimodal, dynamically stabilized reflex loop that mediates head positioning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Payel Chatterjee
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, Bangalore 560065, India
| | - Umesh Mohan
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, Bangalore 560065, India
| | - Sanjay P. Sane
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, Bangalore 560065, India
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Chatterjee P, Prusty AD, Mohan U, Sane SP. Integration of visual and antennal mechanosensory feedback during head stabilization in hawkmoths. eLife 2022; 11:e78410. [PMID: 35758646 PMCID: PMC9259029 DOI: 10.7554/elife.78410] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2022] [Accepted: 06/21/2022] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
During flight maneuvers, insects exhibit compensatory head movements which are essential for stabilizing the visual field on their retina, reducing motion blur, and supporting visual self-motion estimation. In Diptera, such head movements are mediated via visual feedback from their compound eyes that detect retinal slip, as well as rapid mechanosensory feedback from their halteres - the modified hindwings that sense the angular rates of body rotations. Because non-Dipteran insects lack halteres, it is not known if mechanosensory feedback about body rotations plays any role in their head stabilization response. Diverse non-Dipteran insects are known to rely on visual and antennal mechanosensory feedback for flight control. In hawkmoths, for instance, reduction of antennal mechanosensory feedback severely compromises their ability to control flight. Similarly, when the head movements of freely flying moths are restricted, their flight ability is also severely impaired. The role of compensatory head movements as well as multimodal feedback in insect flight raises an interesting question: in insects that lack halteres, what sensory cues are required for head stabilization? Here, we show that in the nocturnal hawkmoth Daphnis nerii, compensatory head movements are mediated by combined visual and antennal mechanosensory feedback. We subjected tethered moths to open-loop body roll rotations under different lighting conditions, and measured their ability to maintain head angle in the presence or absence of antennal mechanosensory feedback. Our study suggests that head stabilization in moths is mediated primarily by visual feedback during roll movements at lower frequencies, whereas antennal mechanosensory feedback is required when roll occurs at higher frequency. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that control of head angle results from a multimodal feedback loop that integrates both visual and antennal mechanosensory feedback, albeit at different latencies. At adequate light levels, visual feedback is sufficient for head stabilization primarily at low frequencies of body roll. However, under dark conditions, antennal mechanosensory feedback is essential for the control of head movements at high frequencies of body roll.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Payel Chatterjee
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental ResearchBangaloreIndia
| | - Agnish Dev Prusty
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental ResearchBangaloreIndia
| | - Umesh Mohan
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental ResearchBangaloreIndia
| | - Sanjay P Sane
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental ResearchBangaloreIndia
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Burnett NP, Badger MA, Combes SA. Wind and route choice affect performance of bees flying above versus within a cluttered obstacle field. PLoS One 2022; 17:e0265911. [PMID: 35325004 PMCID: PMC8947135 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0265911] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2021] [Accepted: 03/09/2022] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Bees flying through natural landscapes frequently encounter physical challenges, such as wind and cluttered vegetation, but the influence of these factors on flight performance remains unknown. We analyzed 548 videos of wild-caught honeybees (Apis mellifera) flying through an enclosure containing a field of vertical obstacles that bees could choose to fly within (through open corridors, without maneuvering) or above. We varied obstacle field height and wind condition (still, headwinds or tailwinds), and examined how these factors affected bees’ flight altitude, ground speed, and side-to-side casting motions (lateral excursions). When obstacle fields were short, bees flew at altitudes near the midpoint between the tunnel floor and ceiling. When obstacle fields approached or exceeded this midpoint, bees tended to increase their altitude, but they did not always avoid flying through obstacles, despite having the freedom to do so. Bees that flew above the obstacles exhibited 40% faster ground speeds and 36% larger lateral excursions than bees that flew within the obstacle fields. Wind did not affect flight altitude, but bees flew 12–19% faster in tailwinds, and their lateral excursions were 19% larger when flying in headwinds or tailwinds, as compared to still air. Our results show that bees flying through complex environments display flexibility in their route choices (i.e., flying above obstacles in some trials and through them in others), which affects their overall flight performance. Similar choices in natural landscapes could have broad implications for foraging efficiency, pollination, and mortality in wild bees.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicholas P. Burnett
- Department of Neurobiology, Physiology, and Behavior, University of California at Davis, Davis, California, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Marc A. Badger
- Department of Neurobiology, Physiology, and Behavior, University of California at Davis, Davis, California, United States of America
- Department of Computer and Information Science, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| | - Stacey A. Combes
- Department of Neurobiology, Physiology, and Behavior, University of California at Davis, Davis, California, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Baird E, Boeddeker N, Srinivasan MV. The effect of optic flow cues on honeybee flight control in wind. Proc Biol Sci 2021; 288:20203051. [PMID: 33468001 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2020.3051] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
To minimize the risk of colliding with the ground or other obstacles, flying animals need to control both their ground speed and ground height. This task is particularly challenging in wind, where head winds require an animal to increase its airspeed to maintain a constant ground speed and tail winds may generate negative airspeeds, rendering flight more difficult to control. In this study, we investigate how head and tail winds affect flight control in the honeybee Apis mellifera, which is known to rely on the pattern of visual motion generated across the eye-known as optic flow-to maintain constant ground speeds and heights. We find that, when provided with both longitudinal and transverse optic flow cues (in or perpendicular to the direction of flight, respectively), honeybees maintain a constant ground speed but fly lower in head winds and higher in tail winds, a response that is also observed when longitudinal optic flow cues are minimized. When the transverse component of optic flow is minimized, or when all optic flow cues are minimized, the effect of wind on ground height is abolished. We propose that the regular sidewards oscillations that the bees make as they fly may be used to extract information about the distance to the ground, independently of the longitudinal optic flow that they use for ground speed control. This computationally simple strategy could have potential uses in the development of lightweight and robust systems for guiding autonomous flying vehicles in natural environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily Baird
- Department of Zoology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Norbert Boeddeker
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Burnett NP, Badger MA, Combes SA. Wind and obstacle motion affect honeybee flight strategies in cluttered environments. J Exp Biol 2020; 223:jeb222471. [PMID: 32561633 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.222471] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2020] [Accepted: 06/09/2020] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Bees often forage in habitats with cluttered vegetation and unpredictable winds. Navigating obstacles in wind presents a challenge that may be exacerbated by wind-induced motions of vegetation. Although wind-blown vegetation is common in natural habitats, we know little about how the strategies of bees for flying through clutter are affected by obstacle motion and wind. We filmed honeybees Apis mellifera flying through obstacles in a flight tunnel with still air, headwinds or tailwinds. We tested how their ground speeds and centering behavior (trajectory relative to the midline between obstacles) changed when obstacles were moving versus stationary, and how their approach strategies affected flight outcome (successful transit versus collision). We found that obstacle motion affects ground speed: bees flew slower when approaching moving versus stationary obstacles in still air but tended to fly faster when approaching moving obstacles in headwinds or tailwinds. Bees in still air reduced their chances of colliding with obstacles (whether moving or stationary) by reducing ground speed, whereas flight outcomes in wind were not associated with ground speed, but rather with improvement in centering behavior during the approach. We hypothesize that in challenging flight situations (e.g. navigating moving obstacles in wind), bees may speed up to reduce the number of wing collisions that occur if they pass too close to an obstacle. Our results show that wind and obstacle motion can interact to affect flight strategies in unexpected ways, suggesting that wind-blown vegetation may have important effects on foraging behaviors and flight performance of bees in natural habitats.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicholas P Burnett
- Department of Neurobiology, Physiology, and Behavior, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA
| | - Marc A Badger
- Department of Neurobiology, Physiology, and Behavior, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA
| | - Stacey A Combes
- Department of Neurobiology, Physiology, and Behavior, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Nakata T, Phillips N, Simões P, Russell IJ, Cheney JA, Walker SM, Bomphrey RJ. Aerodynamic imaging by mosquitoes inspires a surface detector for autonomous flying vehicles. Science 2020; 368:634-637. [PMID: 32381721 DOI: 10.1126/science.aaz9634] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/24/2019] [Accepted: 04/02/2020] [Indexed: 11/02/2022]
Abstract
Some flying animals use active sensing to perceive and avoid obstacles. Nocturnal mosquitoes exhibit a behavioral response to divert away from surfaces when vision is unavailable, indicating a short-range, mechanosensory collision-avoidance mechanism. We suggest that this behavior is mediated by perceiving modulations of their self-induced airflow patterns as they enter a ground or wall effect. We used computational fluid dynamics simulations of low-altitude and near-wall flights based on in vivo high-speed kinematic measurements to quantify changes in the self-generated pressure and velocity cues at the sensitive mechanosensory antennae. We validated the principle that encoding aerodynamic information can enable collision avoidance by developing a quadcopter with a sensory system inspired by the mosquito. Such low-power sensing systems have major potential for future use in safer rotorcraft control systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Toshiyuki Nakata
- Structure and Motion Laboratory, Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield AL9 7TA, UK
- Graduate School of Engineering, Chiba University, Chiba 263-8522, Japan
| | - Nathan Phillips
- Structure and Motion Laboratory, Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield AL9 7TA, UK
| | - Patrício Simões
- Pharmacy and Biomolecular Sciences, University of Brighton, Moulsecoomb, Brighton BN2 4GJ, UK
| | - Ian J Russell
- Pharmacy and Biomolecular Sciences, University of Brighton, Moulsecoomb, Brighton BN2 4GJ, UK
| | - Jorn A Cheney
- Structure and Motion Laboratory, Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield AL9 7TA, UK
| | - Simon M Walker
- Faculty of Biological Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
| | - Richard J Bomphrey
- Structure and Motion Laboratory, Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield AL9 7TA, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Natesan D, Saxena N, Ekeberg Ö, Sane SP. Tuneable reflexes control antennal positioning in flying hawkmoths. Nat Commun 2019; 10:5593. [PMID: 31811150 PMCID: PMC6898381 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-019-13595-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/24/2018] [Accepted: 11/12/2019] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Complex behaviours may be viewed as sequences of modular actions, each elicited by specific sensory cues in their characteristic timescales. From this perspective, we can construct models in which unitary behavioural modules are hierarchically placed in context of related actions. Here, we analyse antennal positioning reflex in hawkmoths as a tuneable behavioural unit. Mechanosensory feedback from two antennal structures, Böhm’s bristles (BB) and Johnston’s organs (JO), determines antennal position. At flight onset, antennae attain a specific position, which is maintained by feedback from BB. Simultaneously, JO senses deflections in flagellum-pedicel joint due to frontal airflow, to modulate its steady-state position. Restricting JO abolishes positional modulation but maintains stability against perturbations. Linear feedback models are sufficient to predict antennal dynamics at various set-points. We modelled antennal positioning as a hierarchical neural-circuit in which fast BB feedback maintains instantaneous set-point, but slow JO feedback modulates it, thereby elucidating mechanisms underlying its robustness and flexibility. Flying insects position their antennae by integrating multisensory inputs across different timescales. This study describes an underlying hierarchical neural circuit that maintains antennal position in a fast and robust manner, whilst retaining flexibility to incorporate slower feedback to modulate position.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dinesh Natesan
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, GKVK campus, Bellary road, Bangalore, 560065, India.,Division of Computational Science and Technology, School of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, KTH Royal Institute of Technology, 10044, Stockholm, Sweden.,Manipal Academy of Higher Education, Manipal, 576104, India
| | - Nitesh Saxena
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, GKVK campus, Bellary road, Bangalore, 560065, India
| | - Örjan Ekeberg
- Division of Computational Science and Technology, School of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, KTH Royal Institute of Technology, 10044, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Sanjay P Sane
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, GKVK campus, Bellary road, Bangalore, 560065, India.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Rauscher MJ, Fox JL. Inertial Sensing and Encoding of Self-Motion: Structural and Functional Similarities across Metazoan Taxa. Integr Comp Biol 2019; 58:832-843. [PMID: 29860381 DOI: 10.1093/icb/icy041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
To properly orient and navigate, moving animals must obtain information about the position and motion of their bodies. Animals detect inertial signals resulting from body accelerations and rotations using a variety of sensory systems. In this review, we briefly summarize current knowledge on inertial sensing across widely disparate animal taxa with an emphasis on neuronal coding and sensory transduction. We outline systems built around mechanosensory hair cells, including the chordate vestibular complex and the statocysts seen in many marine invertebrates. We next compare these to schemes employed by flying insects for managing inherently unstable aspects of flapping flight, built around comparable mechanosensory cells but taking unique advantage of the physics of rotating systems to facilitate motion encoding. Finally, we highlight fundamental similarities across taxa with respect to the partnering of inertial senses with visual senses and conclude with a discussion of the functional utility of maintaining a multiplicity of encoding schemes for self-motion information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael J Rauscher
- Department of Biology, Case Western Reserve University, 10900 Euclid Avenue, Cleveland, OH 44106, USA
| | - Jessica L Fox
- Department of Biology, Case Western Reserve University, 10900 Euclid Avenue, Cleveland, OH 44106, USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Dahake A, Stöckl AL, Foster JJ, Sane SP, Kelber A. The roles of vision and antennal mechanoreception in hawkmoth flight control. eLife 2018; 7:37606. [PMID: 30526849 PMCID: PMC6303104 DOI: 10.7554/elife.37606] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/16/2018] [Accepted: 12/08/2018] [Indexed: 01/28/2023] Open
Abstract
Flying animals need continual sensory feedback about their body position and orientation for flight control. The visual system provides essential but slow feedback. In contrast, mechanosensory channels can provide feedback at much shorter timescales. How the contributions from these two senses are integrated remains an open question in most insect groups. In Diptera, fast mechanosensory feedback is provided by organs called halteres and is crucial for the control of rapid flight manoeuvres, while vision controls manoeuvres in lower temporal frequency bands. Here, we have investigated the visual-mechanosensory integration in the hawkmoth Macroglossum stellatarum. They represent a large group of insects that use Johnston’s organs in their antennae to provide mechanosensory feedback on perturbations in body position. Our experiments show that antennal mechanosensory feedback specifically mediates fast flight manoeuvres, but not slow ones. Moreover, we did not observe compensatory interactions between antennal and visual feedback.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ajinkya Dahake
- Vision Group, Lund University, Lund, Sweden.,National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, Bangalore, India
| | | | | | - Sanjay P Sane
- National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, Bangalore, India
| | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Saxena N, Natesan D, Sane SP. Odor source localization in complex visual environments by fruit flies. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2018; 221:jeb.172023. [PMID: 29146771 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.172023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2017] [Accepted: 11/07/2017] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Flying insects routinely forage in complex and cluttered sensory environments. Their search for a food or a pheromone source typically begins with a whiff of odor, which triggers a flight response, eventually bringing the insect near the odor source. However, pinpointing the precise location of an odor source requires use of both visual and olfactory modalities, aided by odor plumes. Here, we investigated odor-tracking behavior in fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) presented with low- or high-contrast visual landmarks, either paired with or separate from an attractive odor cue. These experiments were conducted either in a gentle air stream which generated laminar odor plumes or in still air in which odor dissipates uniformly in all directions. Trajectories of flies revealed several novel features of their odor-tracking behavior in addition to those previously documented. First, in both moving and still air, odor-seeking flies rely on the co-occurrence of visual landmarks with olfactory cues to guide them to odorant objects. Second, flies abruptly decelerate upon encountering an odor plume, thereafter steering towards the nearest visual objects that had no inherent salience in the absence of odor. Thus, interception of an attractive odor increases their salience to nearby high-contrast visual landmarks. Third, flies adopt distinct odor-tracking strategies during flight in moving versus still air. Whereas they weave in and out of plumes towards an odor source in airflow, their approach is more incremental in still air. Both strategies are robust and flexible, and enable flies to reliably find odor sources under diverse visual and airflow environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nitesh Saxena
- National Centre for Biological Sciences (NCBS), Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, GKVK campus, Bellary Road, Bangalore 560065, India
| | - Dinesh Natesan
- National Centre for Biological Sciences (NCBS), Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, GKVK campus, Bellary Road, Bangalore 560065, India.,Department of Computational Science and Technology, School of Computer Science and Communication, KTH Royal Institute of Technology, Stockholm 10044, Sweden.,Manipal University, Manipal 576104, India
| | - Sanjay P Sane
- National Centre for Biological Sciences (NCBS), Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, GKVK campus, Bellary Road, Bangalore 560065, India
| |
Collapse
|