51
|
Multani N, Taghdiri F, Anor CJ, Varriano B, Misquitta K, Tang-Wai DF, Keren R, Fox S, Lang AE, Vijverman AC, Marras C, Tartaglia MC. Association Between Social Cognition Changes and Resting State Functional Connectivity in Frontotemporal Dementia, Alzheimer's Disease, Parkinson's Disease, and Healthy Controls. Front Neurosci 2019; 13:1259. [PMID: 31824254 PMCID: PMC6883726 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01259] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/11/2019] [Accepted: 11/06/2019] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To determine the relationship between alterations in resting state functional connectivity and social cognition dysfunction among patients with frontotemporal dementia (FTD), Alzheimer's disease (AD), Parkinson's disease (PD), and healthy controls (HC). METHODS Fifty-seven participants (FTD = 10, AD = 18, PD = 19, and HC = 10) underwent structural and functional imaging and completed the Awareness of Social Inference Test-Emotion Evaluation Test (TASIT-EET), Behavioral Inhibition System/Behavioral Activation System (BIS/BAS) scale, Revised Self-Monitoring Scale (RSMS), Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI), and Social Norms Questionnaire (SNQ). A multi-variate pattern analysis (MVPA) was carried out to determine activation differences between the groups. The clusters from the MVPA were used as seeds for the ROI-to-voxel analysis. Relationship between social cognition deficits and uncinate integrity was also investigated. RESULTS BOLD signal activation differed among the four groups of AD, PD, FTD, and HC in the left inferior temporal gyrus-anterior division [L-ITG (ant)], right central opercular cortex (R-COp), right supramarginal gyrus, posterior division (R-SMG, post), right angular gyrus (R-AG), and R-ITG. The BOLD co-activation of the L-ITG (ant) with bilateral frontal pole (FP) and paracingulate gyrus was positively associated with IRI-perspective taking (PT) (r = 0.38, p = 0.007), SNQ total (r = 0.37, p = 0.009), and TASIT-EET (r = 0.47, p < 0.001). CONCLUSION Patients with neurodegenerative diseases showed alterations in connectivity in brain regions important for social cognition compared with HCs. Functional connectivity correlated with performance on social cognition tasks and alterations could be responsible for some of the social cognition deficits observed in all neurodegenerative diseases.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Namita Multani
- Tanz Centre for Research in Neurodegenerative Diseases, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Foad Taghdiri
- Tanz Centre for Research in Neurodegenerative Diseases, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Cassandra J. Anor
- Tanz Centre for Research in Neurodegenerative Diseases, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Brenda Varriano
- Tanz Centre for Research in Neurodegenerative Diseases, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Karen Misquitta
- Tanz Centre for Research in Neurodegenerative Diseases, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - David F. Tang-Wai
- Memory Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital, University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Ron Keren
- Memory Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital, University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Susan Fox
- The Edmond J. Safra Program for Parkinson Disease, Movement Disorder Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital, University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Anthony E. Lang
- The Edmond J. Safra Program for Parkinson Disease, Movement Disorder Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital, University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Anne Catherine Vijverman
- The Edmond J. Safra Program for Parkinson Disease, Movement Disorder Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital, University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Connie Marras
- The Edmond J. Safra Program for Parkinson Disease, Movement Disorder Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital, University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Maria Carmela Tartaglia
- Tanz Centre for Research in Neurodegenerative Diseases, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
- Memory Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital, University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
52
|
Stewart E, Lah S, Smith ML. Patterns of impaired social cognition in children and adolescents with epilepsy: The borders between different epilepsy phenotypes. Epilepsy Behav 2019; 100:106146. [PMID: 30894295 DOI: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.01.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2018] [Revised: 01/22/2019] [Accepted: 01/23/2019] [Indexed: 01/06/2023]
Abstract
Over the past decade, a growing number of studies have shown that children and adolescents with focal and generalized epilepsies have marked impairments in social cognition, including deficits in facial emotion perception (FEP) and Theory of Mind (ToM). At present, it remains unclear whether FEP and ToM impairments are comparable in children with focal and generalized epilepsies or whether distinct syndrome-specific deficits have emerged. This question of whether unique or overlapping social cognitive profiles exist in epilepsy is of interest, given that the revised International League Against Epilepsy (ILAE) classification guidelines propose that seizures arise from a diseased network (i.e., network account), rather than being confined to discrete regions near the site of seizure foci (i.e., localization account). The purpose of this review was as follows: (1) to summarize studies examining FEP and ToM in pediatric patients with epilepsy, (2) to examine epilepsy and psychosocial correlates of these difficulties, and (3) to determine whether patterns of sociocognitive impairment better support a localization or neural network account of epilepsy. Twelve studies were reviewed examining FEP (N = 5) and/or ToM (N = 8). Findings revealed significant FEP and ToM impairments across the studied subgroups with epilepsy, which did not differ between children with generalized and focal (localization-related) epilepsies nor among children with different subtypes of localization-related epilepsy. Similarly, other epilepsy variables (i.e., seizure frequency, side of seizure focus, number of antiepileptic drugs (AEDs) or surgical status) were not related to FEP or ToM, with the exception of younger age at seizure onset and longer duration of epilepsy. Several studies documented a significant relationship between impaired ToM and reduced social competence in pediatric patients with epilepsy, whereas evidence for a relationship between FEP and psychosocial functioning is currently weak. In conclusion, findings suggest that social cognitive impairments represent a shared feature of epilepsy in childhood. The results support a neural network account of epilepsy, in which a shared neural network of dysfunction may be underlying social cognitive deficits in this group. Further research is needed to examine the functional correlates of social cognitive impairments, as well as to evaluate screening tools and treatment methods to identify and address significant social and emotional difficulties in this patient group. This article is part of the Special Issue "Epilepsy and social cognition across the lifespan".
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth Stewart
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW 2007, Australia
| | - Suncica Lah
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW 2007, Australia
| | - Mary Lou Smith
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga, ON L5L 1C6, Canada; Neurosciences and Mental Health Program, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON M5G 1X8, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Stewart E, Catroppa C, Gonzalez L, Gill D, Webster R, Lawson J, Sabaz M, Mandalis A, Barton B, McLean S, Lah S. Facial emotion perception and social competence in children (8 to 16 years old) with genetic generalized epilepsy and temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 2019; 100:106301. [PMID: 31133510 DOI: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.04.054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2019] [Revised: 04/01/2019] [Accepted: 04/27/2019] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Facial emotion perception (FEP) impairments are common in adults with epilepsy and associated with impaired psychosocial functioning. Research into the presence of FEP deficits in children with epilepsy and the functional implications of these deficits is limited. The primary aims of this study were to assess FEP abilities in children (8 to 16 years old) with genetic generalized epilepsy (GGE) and temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) and examine whether FEP is related to everyday social functioning. Forty-four children (8 to 16 years) with epilepsy (22 GGE, 22 TLE) and 22 typically developing controls completed the Pictures of Facial Affect (POFA) battery to assess FEP and a brief test of intellectual functioning (intelligence quotient [IQ]). Parents completed questionnaires assessing social competence of their child. Neurologists completed the Global Assessment of Severity of Epilepsy (GASE) scale as a measure of overall epilepsy severity. Demographic and clinical information was obtained from medical records and clinical interviews with parents. Findings revealed significant, overall FEP impairments and reduced social competence in children with GGE and TLE compared to controls. The magnitude of FEP impairment (i.e., across all emotions) was comparable in the two epilepsy groups, yet different emotions were impaired in each group: children with GGE were impaired in recognizing anger and disgust, whereas children with TLE were impaired in sadness and disgust, compared to controls. Contrary to expectations, total FEP accuracy was not significantly correlated with social competence in either epilepsy group. In conclusion, children with GGE and TLE have significant impairments recognizing emotional expressions on faces. Further research is needed to examine whether underlying FEP impairments relate to social and emotional functioning in children with epilepsy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth Stewart
- School of Psychology, The University of Sydney, 94 - 100 Mallett Street, Camperdown, Sydney, Australia; ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Sydney, Australia
| | - Cathy Catroppa
- Murdoch Children's Research Institute, 50 Flemington Road, Parkville, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Linda Gonzalez
- Murdoch Children's Research Institute, 50 Flemington Road, Parkville, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Deepak Gill
- T.Y Nelson Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, The Children's Hospital at Westmead, Corner Hawkesbury Road and Hainsworth Street, Westmead, Sydney, Australia
| | - Richard Webster
- T.Y Nelson Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, The Children's Hospital at Westmead, Corner Hawkesbury Road and Hainsworth Street, Westmead, Sydney, Australia
| | - John Lawson
- Department of Neurology, Sydney Children's Hospital, High Street Randwick, Sydney, Australia
| | - Mark Sabaz
- Department of Psychology, Sydney Children's Hospital, High Street Randwick, Sydney, Australia
| | - Anna Mandalis
- Department of Psychology, Sydney Children's Hospital, High Street Randwick, Sydney, Australia
| | - Belinda Barton
- Children's Hospital Education Research Institute, The Children's Hospital at Westmead, Corner Hawkesbury Road and Hainsworth Street, Westmead, Sydney, Australia
| | - Samantha McLean
- T.Y Nelson Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, The Children's Hospital at Westmead, Corner Hawkesbury Road and Hainsworth Street, Westmead, Sydney, Australia
| | - Suncica Lah
- School of Psychology, The University of Sydney, 94 - 100 Mallett Street, Camperdown, Sydney, Australia; ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Sydney, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Falagiarda F, Collignon O. Time-resolved discrimination of audio-visual emotion expressions. Cortex 2019; 119:184-194. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.04.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2018] [Revised: 04/05/2019] [Accepted: 04/29/2019] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
55
|
Rymarczyk K, Żurawski Ł, Jankowiak-Siuda K, Szatkowska I. Empathy in Facial Mimicry of Fear and Disgust: Simultaneous EMG-fMRI Recordings During Observation of Static and Dynamic Facial Expressions. Front Psychol 2019; 10:701. [PMID: 30971997 PMCID: PMC6445885 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00701] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/26/2018] [Accepted: 03/13/2019] [Indexed: 01/18/2023] Open
Abstract
Real-life faces are dynamic by nature, particularly when expressing emotion. Increasing evidence suggests that the perception of dynamic displays enhances facial mimicry and induces activation in widespread brain structures considered to be part of the mirror neuron system, a neuronal network linked to empathy. The present study is the first to investigate the relations among facial muscle responses, brain activity, and empathy traits while participants observed static and dynamic (videos) facial expressions of fear and disgust. During display presentation, blood-oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal as well as muscle reactions of the corrugator supercilii and levator labii were recorded simultaneously from 46 healthy individuals (21 females). It was shown that both fear and disgust faces caused activity in the corrugator supercilii muscle, while perception of disgust produced facial activity additionally in the levator labii muscle, supporting a specific pattern of facial mimicry for these emotions. Moreover, individuals with higher, compared to individuals with lower, empathy traits showed greater activity in the corrugator supercilii and levator labii muscles; however, these responses were not differentiable between static and dynamic mode. Conversely, neuroimaging data revealed motion and emotional-related brain structures in response to dynamic rather than static stimuli among high empathy individuals. In line with this, there was a correlation between electromyography (EMG) responses and brain activity suggesting that the Mirror Neuron System, the anterior insula and the amygdala might constitute the neural correlates of automatic facial mimicry for fear and disgust. These results revealed that the dynamic property of (emotional) stimuli facilitates the emotional-related processing of facial expressions, especially among whose with high trait empathy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Krystyna Rymarczyk
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Institute of Cognitive and Behavioural Neuroscience, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland
| | - Łukasz Żurawski
- Laboratory of Psychophysiology, Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of Sciences (PAS), Warsaw, Poland
| | - Kamila Jankowiak-Siuda
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Institute of Cognitive and Behavioural Neuroscience, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland
| | - Iwona Szatkowska
- Laboratory of Psychophysiology, Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of Sciences (PAS), Warsaw, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
56
|
Electrophysiological assessment methodology of sensory processing dysfunction in schizophrenia and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2019; 97:70-84. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/20/2017] [Revised: 09/04/2018] [Accepted: 09/05/2018] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
|
57
|
Likova LT, Mei M, Mineff KN, Nicholas SC. Learning face perception without vision: Rebound learning effect and hemispheric differences in congenital vs late-onset blindness. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2019; 2019:2371-23713. [PMID: 31633079 DOI: 10.2352/issn.2470-1173.2019.12.hvei-237] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/01/2022]
Abstract
To address the longstanding questions of whether the blind-from-birth have an innate face-schema, what plasticity mechanisms underlie non-visual face learning, and whether there are interhemispheric face processing differences in face processing in the blind, we used a unique non-visual drawing-based training in congenitally blind (CB), late-blind (LB) and blindfolded-sighted (BF) groups of adults. This Cognitive-Kinesthetic Drawing approach previously developed by Likova (e.g., 2010, 2012, 2013) enabled us to rapidly train and study training-driven neuroplasticity in both the blind and sighted groups. The five-day two-hour training taught participants to haptically explore, recognize, memorize raised-line images, and draw them free-hand from memory, in detail, including the fine facial characteristics of the face stimuli. Such drawings represent an externalization of the formed memory. Functional MRI was run before and after the training. Tactile-face perception activated the occipito-temporal cortex in all groups. However, the training led to a strong, predominantly left-hemispheric reorganization in the two blind groups, in contrast to right-hemispheric in blindfolded-sighted, i.e., the post-training response-change was stronger in the left hemisphere in the blind, but in the right in the blindfolded. This is the first study to discover interhemispheric differences in non-visual face processing. Remarkably, for face perception this learning-based change was positive in the CB and BF groups, but negative in the LB-group. Both the lateralization and inversed-sign learning effects were specific to face perception, but absent for the control nonface categories of small objects and houses. The unexpected inversed-sign training effect in CB vs LB suggests different stages of brain plasticity in the ventral pathway specific to the face category. Importantly, the fact that only after a very few days of our training, the totally-blind-from-birth CB manifested a very good (haptic) face perception, and even developed strong empathy to the explored faces, implies a preexisting face schema that can be "unmasked" and "tuned up" by a proper learning procedure. The Likova Cognitive-Kinesthetic Training is a powerful tool for driving brain plasticity, and providing deeper insights into non-visual learning, including emergence of perceptual categories. A rebound learning model and a neuro-Bayesian economy principle are proposed to explain the multidimensional learning effects. The results provide new insights into the Nature-vs-Nurture interplay in rapid brain plasticity and neurorehabilitation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lora T Likova
- The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, CA
| | - Ming Mei
- The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, CA
| | - Kris N Mineff
- The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, CA
| | | |
Collapse
|
58
|
The cerebellum and cognition. Neurosci Lett 2019; 688:62-75. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.07.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 425] [Impact Index Per Article: 70.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/03/2018] [Revised: 07/03/2018] [Accepted: 07/04/2018] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
|
59
|
Berthold-Losleben M, Habel U, Brehl AK, Freiherr J, Losleben K, Schneider F, Amunts K, Kohn N. Implicit Affective Rivalry: A Behavioral and fMRI Study Combining Olfactory and Auditory Stimulation. Front Behav Neurosci 2018; 12:313. [PMID: 30618666 PMCID: PMC6305346 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00313] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/28/2018] [Accepted: 11/30/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Aversive odors are highly salient stimuli that serve a protective function. Thus, emotional reactions elicited by negative odors may be hardly influenceable. We aim to elucidate if negative mood induced by negative odors can be modulated automatically by positively valenced stimuli. We included 32 healthy participants (16 men) in an fMRI design combining aversive and neutral olfactory stimuli with positive and neutral auditory stimuli to test the influence of aversive olfactory stimuli on subjective emotional state and brain activation when combined with positive and neutral auditory stimuli. The behavioral results show an interaction of negative olfactory stimuli on ratings of disgust, perceived valence of music, and subjective affective state, while positive auditory stimulation did not show this interaction. On a neuronal level, we observed main effects for auditory and olfactory stimulation, which are largely congruent with previous literature. However, the pairing of both stimuli was associated with attenuated brain activity in a set of brain areas (supplementary motor area, temporal pole, superior frontal gyrus) which overlaps with multisensory processing areas and pave the way for automatic emotion regulation. Our behavioral results and the integrated neural patterns provide evidence of predominance of olfaction in processing of affective rivalry from multiple sensory modalities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark Berthold-Losleben
- Division of Mental Healthcare, St. Olavs University Hospital, Trondheim, Norway.,Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Trondheim, Norway
| | - Ute Habel
- Uniklinik RWTH Aachen, Aachen, Germany
| | - Anne-Kathrin Brehl
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Jessica Freiherr
- Uniklinik RWTH Aachen, Aachen, Germany.,Fraunhofer Institute for Process Engineering and Packaging (IVV), Freising, Germany
| | | | - Frank Schneider
- Uniklinik RWTH Aachen, Aachen, Germany.,Düsseldorf University Hospital, Düsseldorf, Germany
| | - Katrin Amunts
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, Jülich Research Centre, Jülich, Germany
| | - Nils Kohn
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
60
|
Garrigan B, Adlam AL, Langdon PE. Moral decision-making and moral development: Toward an integrative framework. DEVELOPMENTAL REVIEW 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2018.06.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
|
61
|
Cao W, Sun X, Dong D, Yao S, Huang B. Sex Differences in Spontaneous Brain Activity in Adolescents With Conduct Disorder. Front Psychol 2018; 9:1598. [PMID: 30214422 PMCID: PMC6126420 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01598] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2018] [Accepted: 08/10/2018] [Indexed: 02/05/2023] Open
Abstract
Purpose: Sex differences in conduct disorder (CD) pathophysiology have yet to be resolved. In this study, we applied the amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations (ALFF) and fractional ALFF (fALFF) to compare spontaneous brain activity in male versus female adolescents diagnosed with CD in light of the gender paradox hypothesis. Materials and Methods: Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) examinations were conducted with 51 CD patients (35 males) and 53 age-matched healthy controls (HCs; 35 males). Pearson analysis was conducted to detect relationship between ALFF/fALFF values in gender-differentiated regions and clinical characteristics. Results: We observed that male CD patients showed significant increased ALFF in the bilateral superior temporal gyrus (STG)/insula, and significant decreased ALFF in the left anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), left middle frontal gyrus (BA8 andBA11), left middle temporal gyrus and left inferior/middle temporal gyrus relative to female CD patients. The fALFF in male CD patients was significantly increased in the right STG/insula, decreased in the right superior frontal gyrus, left middle frontal gyrus, right inferior frontal gyrus, and right postcentral gyrus relative to female CD patients. Considering the sex-by-diagnosis interactions in CD patients, the male CD patients had significantly higher fALFF in the left putamen, lower fALFF in the right postcentral gyrus relative to the female CD patients. Conclusion: The brain regions whose activity index values differed in relation to sex should be further explored in CD pathophysiology studies, particularly with respect to sex differences in clinical symptoms, emotional features, cognitive features, and prevalence rates in CD. The present findings are consistent with the gender paradox hypothesis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wanyi Cao
- Medical Psychological Center, The Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
| | - Xiaoqiang Sun
- Medical Psychological Center, The Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
| | - Daifeng Dong
- Medical Psychological Center, The Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
| | - Shuqiao Yao
- Medical Psychological Center, The Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
| | - Bingsheng Huang
- Medical Psychological Center, The Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
- School of Biomedical Engineering, Health Science Center, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
| |
Collapse
|
62
|
Oaten M, Stevenson RJ, Williams MA, Rich AN, Butko M, Case TI. Moral Violations and the Experience of Disgust and Anger. Front Behav Neurosci 2018; 12:179. [PMID: 30214400 PMCID: PMC6125610 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00179] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/19/2018] [Accepted: 07/30/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Disgust is a natural defensive emotion that has evolved to protect against potential sources of contamination and has been recently linked to moral judgements in many studies. However, that people often report feelings of disgust when thinking about feces or moral transgressions alike does not necessarily mean that the same mechanisms mediate these reactions. The present study used functional magnetic resonance imaging (n = 22) to investigate whether core and moral disgusts entrain common neural systems. We provide evidence that: (i) activation of overlapping brain regions between core and moral disgust is the result of content overlap in the vignettes—core disgust elicitors—across conditions, and not from moral violations per se, and (ii) moral residue (i.e., the remaining or “residual” activation after the influence of core disgust elicitors have been taken into account) produced a pattern of activation that is more consistent with moral anger, than one of “residual disgust.” These findings run contrary to the premise that our “moral center” is connected to the area of the brain in which physical revulsion is located.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Megan Oaten
- Menzies Health Institute Queensland, Griffith University, Southport, QLD, Australia
| | | | - Mark A Williams
- Department of Cognitive Science & ARC Centre for Cognition and its Disorders, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia.,Perception in Action Research Centre, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Anina N Rich
- Department of Cognitive Science & ARC Centre for Cognition and its Disorders, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia.,Perception in Action Research Centre, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Marina Butko
- Department of Cognitive Science & ARC Centre for Cognition and its Disorders, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia.,Perception in Action Research Centre, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Trevor I Case
- Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
63
|
Wang Y, Zhu Z, Chen B, Fang F. Perceptual learning and recognition confusion reveal the underlying relationships among the six basic emotions. Cogn Emot 2018; 33:754-767. [PMID: 29962270 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2018.1491831] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
The six basic emotions (disgust, anger, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise) have long been considered discrete categories that serve as the primary units of the emotion system. Yet recent evidence indicated underlying connections among them. Here we tested the underlying relationships among the six basic emotions using a perceptual learning procedure. This technique has the potential of causally changing participants' emotion detection ability. We found that training on detecting a facial expression improved the performance not only on the trained expression but also on other expressions. Such a transfer effect was consistently demonstrated between disgust and anger detection as well as between fear and surprise detection in two experiments (Experiment 1A, n = 70; Experiment 1B, n = 42). Notably, training on any of the six emotions could improve happiness detection, while sadness detection could only be improved by training on sadness itself, suggesting the uniqueness of happiness and sadness. In an emotion recognition test using a large sample of Chinese participants (n = 1748), the confusion between disgust and anger as well as between fear and surprise was further confirmed. Taken together, our study demonstrates that the "basic" emotions share some common psychological components, which might be the more basic units of the emotion system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yingying Wang
- a Academy for Advanced Interdisciplinary Studies, Peking-Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences, Peking University , Beijing , P. R. People's Republic of China.,b IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking University , Beijing , P. R. People's Republic of China
| | - Zijian Zhu
- a Academy for Advanced Interdisciplinary Studies, Peking-Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences, Peking University , Beijing , P. R. People's Republic of China.,b IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking University , Beijing , P. R. People's Republic of China
| | - Biqing Chen
- a Academy for Advanced Interdisciplinary Studies, Peking-Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences, Peking University , Beijing , P. R. People's Republic of China.,b IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking University , Beijing , P. R. People's Republic of China
| | - Fang Fang
- a Academy for Advanced Interdisciplinary Studies, Peking-Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences, Peking University , Beijing , P. R. People's Republic of China.,b IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking University , Beijing , P. R. People's Republic of China.,c School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Peking University , Beijing , P. R. People's Republic of China.,d Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University , Beijing , P. R. People's Republic of China.,e Key Laboratory of Machine Perception (Ministry of Education) , Peking University , Beijing , P. R. People's Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
64
|
Zhang H, Chen X, Chen S, Li Y, Chen C, Long Q, Yuan J. Facial Expression Enhances Emotion Perception Compared to Vocal Prosody: Behavioral and fMRI Studies. Neurosci Bull 2018; 34:801-815. [PMID: 29740753 DOI: 10.1007/s12264-018-0231-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/16/2017] [Accepted: 03/13/2018] [Indexed: 02/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Facial and vocal expressions are essential modalities mediating the perception of emotion and social communication. Nonetheless, currently little is known about how emotion perception and its neural substrates differ across facial expression and vocal prosody. To clarify this issue, functional MRI scans were acquired in Study 1, in which participants were asked to discriminate the valence of emotional expression (angry, happy or neutral) from facial, vocal, or bimodal stimuli. In Study 2, we used an affective priming task (unimodal materials as primers and bimodal materials as target) and participants were asked to rate the intensity, valence, and arousal of the targets. Study 1 showed higher accuracy and shorter response latencies in the facial than in the vocal modality for a happy expression. Whole-brain analysis showed enhanced activation during facial compared to vocal emotions in the inferior temporal-occipital regions. Region of interest analysis showed a higher percentage signal change for facial than for vocal anger in the superior temporal sulcus. Study 2 showed that facial relative to vocal priming of anger had a greater influence on perceived emotion for bimodal targets, irrespective of the target valence. These findings suggest that facial expression is associated with enhanced emotion perception compared to equivalent vocal prosodies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Heming Zhang
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of the Ministry of Education, School of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, 400715, China
| | - Xuhai Chen
- Key Laboratory of Behavior and Cognitive Psychology in Shaanxi Province, School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, 710062, China.,Key Laboratory of Modern Teaching Technology of the Ministry of Education, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, 710062, China
| | - Shengdong Chen
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of the Ministry of Education, School of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, 400715, China
| | - Yansong Li
- Department of Psychology, School of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Nanjing University, Nanjing, 210023, China
| | - Changming Chen
- School of Educational Sciences, Xinyang Normal University, Xinyang, 464000, China
| | - Quanshan Long
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of the Ministry of Education, School of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, 400715, China
| | - Jiajin Yuan
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of the Ministry of Education, School of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, 400715, China.
| |
Collapse
|
65
|
Trait anger modulates neural activity in the fronto-parietal attention network. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0194444. [PMID: 29672547 PMCID: PMC5908080 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0194444] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2017] [Accepted: 03/02/2018] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Anger is considered a unique high-arousal and approach-related negative emotion. The influence of individual differences in trait anger on the processing of visual stimuli is relevant to questions about emotional processing and remains to be explored. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we explored the neural responses to standardized images, selected based on valence and arousal ratings in a group of men with high trait anger compared to those with normative to low anger scores (controls). Results show increased activation in the left-lateralized ventral fronto-parietal attention network to unpleasant images by individuals with high trait anger. There was also a group by arousal interaction in the left thalamus/pulvinar such that individuals with high trait anger had increased pulvinar activation to the high-arousal (versus low arousal) unpleasant images as compared to controls. Thus, individual differences in trait anger in men are associated with brain regions subserving executive attentional and sensory integration during the processing of unpleasant emotional stimuli, particularly to high arousal images.
Collapse
|
66
|
Ziegler JC, Montant M, Briesemeister BB, Brink TT, Wicker B, Ponz A, Bonnard M, Jacobs AM, Braun M. Do Words Stink? Neural Reuse as a Principle for Understanding Emotions in Reading. J Cogn Neurosci 2018; 30:1023-1032. [PMID: 29668395 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01268] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
How do we understand the emotional content of written words? Here, we investigate the hypothesis that written words that carry emotions are processed through phylogenetically ancient neural circuits that are involved in the processing of the very same emotions in nonlanguage contexts. This hypothesis was tested with respect to disgust. In an fMRI experiment, it was found that the same region of the left anterior insula responded whether people observed facial expressions of disgust or whether they read words with disgusting content. In a follow-up experiment, it was found that repetitive TMS over the left insula in comparison with a control site interfered with the processing of disgust words to a greater extent than with the processing of neutral words. Together, the results support the hypothesis that the affective processes we experience when reading rely on the reuse of phylogenetically ancient brain structures that process basic emotions in other domains and species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Mario Braun
- University of Salzburg and Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience
| |
Collapse
|
67
|
Adamaszek M, D’Agata F, Steele CJ, Sehm B, Schoppe C, Strecker K, Woldag H, Hummelsheim H, Kirkby KC. Comparison of visual and auditory emotion recognition in patients with cerebellar and Parkinson´s disease. Soc Neurosci 2018; 14:195-207. [DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2018.1434089] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Adamaszek
- Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neurorehabilitation, Klinik Bavaria Kreischa, Kreischa, Germany
| | - Federico D’Agata
- Department of Neuroscience, AOU San Giovanni Battista Turin, Turin, Italy
| | - Christopher J. Steele
- Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Bernhard Sehm
- Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Cornelia Schoppe
- Department of Neurology, Neuropsychology, Sachsenklinik Bad Lausick, Bad Lausick, Germany
| | - Karl Strecker
- Department of Neurology, Neuropsychology, Sachsenklinik Bad Lausick, Bad Lausick, Germany
- Center for Neurological Rehabilitation, Leipzig University, Bennewitz, Germany
| | - Hartwig Woldag
- Department of Neurology, Neuropsychology, Sachsenklinik Bad Lausick, Bad Lausick, Germany
- Center for Neurological Rehabilitation, Leipzig University, Bennewitz, Germany
| | - Horst Hummelsheim
- Department of Neurology, Neuropsychology, Sachsenklinik Bad Lausick, Bad Lausick, Germany
- Center for Neurological Rehabilitation, Leipzig University, Bennewitz, Germany
| | - Kenneth C. Kirkby
- Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
68
|
Roberts G, Perry A, Lord A, Frankland A, Leung V, Holmes-Preston E, Levy F, Lenroot RK, Mitchell PB, Breakspear M. Structural dysconnectivity of key cognitive and emotional hubs in young people at high genetic risk for bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2018; 23:413-421. [PMID: 27994220 PMCID: PMC5794888 DOI: 10.1038/mp.2016.216] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2016] [Revised: 09/30/2016] [Accepted: 10/04/2016] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Emerging evidence suggests that psychiatric disorders are associated with disturbances in structural brain networks. Little is known, however, about brain networks in those at high risk (HR) of bipolar disorder (BD), with such disturbances carrying substantial predictive and etiological value. Whole-brain tractography was performed on diffusion-weighted images acquired from 84 unaffected HR individuals with at least one first-degree relative with BD, 38 young patients with BD and 96 matched controls (CNs) with no family history of mental illness. We studied structural connectivity differences between these groups, with a focus on highly connected hubs and networks involving emotional centres. HR participants showed lower structural connectivity in two lateralised sub-networks centred on bilateral inferior frontal gyri and left insular cortex, as well as increased connectivity in a right lateralised limbic sub-network compared with CN subjects. BD was associated with weaker connectivity in a small right-sided sub-network involving connections between fronto-temporal and temporal areas. Although these sub-networks preferentially involved structural hubs, the integrity of the highly connected structural backbone was preserved in both groups. Weaker structural brain networks involving key emotional centres occur in young people at genetic risk of BD and those with established BD. In contrast to other psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia, the structural core of the brain remains intact, despite the local involvement of network hubs. These results add to our understanding of the neurobiological correlates of BD and provide predictions for outcomes in young people at high genetic risk for BD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G Roberts
- School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Randwick, NSW, Australia,Black Dog Institute, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, NSW, Australia
| | - A Perry
- School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Randwick, NSW, Australia,Program of Mental Health Research, QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, QLD, Australia,Metro North Mental Health Service, Brisbane, QLD, Australia,Centre for Healthy Brain Ageing, Randwick, NSW, Australia
| | - A Lord
- Program of Mental Health Research, QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
| | - A Frankland
- School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Randwick, NSW, Australia,Black Dog Institute, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, NSW, Australia
| | - V Leung
- School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Randwick, NSW, Australia,Black Dog Institute, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, NSW, Australia
| | - E Holmes-Preston
- School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Randwick, NSW, Australia,Black Dog Institute, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, NSW, Australia
| | - F Levy
- School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Randwick, NSW, Australia,Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, NSW, Australia
| | - R K Lenroot
- School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Randwick, NSW, Australia,Neuroscience Research Australia, Randwick, NSW, Australia
| | - P B Mitchell
- School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Randwick, NSW, Australia,Black Dog Institute, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, NSW, Australia,Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, NSW, Australia
| | - M Breakspear
- Program of Mental Health Research, QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, QLD, Australia,Metro North Mental Health Service, Brisbane, QLD, Australia,Systems Neuroscience Group, QIMR Berghofer Institute of Medical Research, 300 Herston Road, Herston, QLD, Australia. E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
69
|
Herman AM, Critchley HD, Duka T. The role of emotions and physiological arousal in modulating impulsive behaviour. Biol Psychol 2018; 133:30-43. [PMID: 29391179 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.01.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 67] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2017] [Revised: 09/29/2017] [Accepted: 01/23/2018] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
Impulsivity received considerable attention in the context of drug misuse and certain neuropsychiatric conditions. Because of its great health and well-being importance, it is crucial to understand factors which modulate impulsive behaviour. As a growing body of literature indicates the role of emotional and physiological states in guiding our actions and decisions, we argue that current affective state and physiological arousal exert a significant influence on behavioural impulsivity. As 'impulsivity' is a heterogeneous concept, in this paper, we review key theories of the topic and summarise information about distinct impulsivity subtypes and their methods of assessment, pointing out to the differences between the various components of the construct. Moreover, we review existing literature on the relationship between emotional states, arousal and impulsive behaviour and suggest directions for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aleksandra M Herman
- Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience, School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom.
| | - Hugo D Critchley
- Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom; Brighton and Sussex Medical School, Brighton, United Kingdom
| | - Theodora Duka
- Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience, School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom.
| |
Collapse
|
70
|
Doretto V, Scivoletto S. Effects of Early Neglect Experience on Recognition and Processing of Facial Expressions: A Systematic Review. Brain Sci 2018; 8:brainsci8010010. [PMID: 29316648 PMCID: PMC5789341 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci8010010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/25/2017] [Revised: 12/29/2017] [Accepted: 12/29/2017] [Indexed: 02/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Background: Child neglect is highly prevalent and associated with a series of biological and social consequences. Early neglect may alter the recognition of emotional faces, but its precise impact remains unclear. We aim to review and analyze data from recent literature about recognition and processing of facial expressions in individuals with history of childhood neglect. Methods: We conducted a systematic review using PubMed, PsycINFO, ScIELO and EMBASE databases in the search of studies for the past 10 years. Results: In total, 14 studies were selected and critically reviewed. A heterogeneity was detected across methods and sample frames. Results were mixed across studies. Different forms of alterations to perception of facial expressions were found across 12 studies. There was alteration to the recognition and processing of both positive and negative emotions, but for emotional face processing there was predominance in alteration toward negative emotions. Conclusions: This is the first review to examine specifically the effects of early neglect experience as a prevalent condition of child maltreatment. The results of this review are inconclusive due to methodological diversity, implement of distinct instruments and differences in the composition of the samples. Despite these limitations, some studies support our hypothesis that individuals with history of early negligence may present alteration to the ability to perceive face expressions of emotions. The article brings relevant information that can help in the development of more effective therapeutic strategies to reduce the impact of neglect on the cognitive and emotional development of the child.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Victoria Doretto
- Departamento de Psiquiatria, Hospital das Clinicas HCFMUSP, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de Sao Paulo, 05403-010 Sao Paulo, SP, Brazil.
| | - Sandra Scivoletto
- Departamento de Psiquiatria, Hospital das Clinicas HCFMUSP, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de Sao Paulo, 05403-010 Sao Paulo, SP, Brazil.
| |
Collapse
|
71
|
Nickel K, Tebartz van Elst L, Manko J, Unterrainer J, Rauh R, Klein C, Endres D, Kaller CP, Mader I, Riedel A, Biscaldi M, Maier S. Inferior Frontal Gyrus Volume Loss Distinguishes Between Autism and (Comorbid) Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder-A FreeSurfer Analysis in Children. Front Psychiatry 2018; 9:521. [PMID: 30405459 PMCID: PMC6206215 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00521] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/04/2018] [Accepted: 10/02/2018] [Indexed: 01/23/2023] Open
Abstract
Objective: Autism spectrum (ASD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are neurodevelopmental disorders with a high rate of comorbidity. To date, diagnosis is based on clinical presentation and distinct reliable biomarkers have been identified neither for ASD nor ADHD. Most previous neuroimaging studies investigated ASD and ADHD separately. Method: To address the question of structural brain differences between ASD and ADHD, we performed FreeSurfer analysis in a sample of children with ADHD (n = 30), with high-functioning ASD (n = 14), with comorbid high-functioning ASD and ADHD (n = 15), and of typically developed controls (TD; n = 36). With FreeSurfer, an automated brain imaging processing and analyzing suite, we reconstructed the cerebral cortex and calculated gray matter volumes as well as cortical surface parameters in terms of cortical thickness and mean curvature. Results: A significant main effect of the factor ADHD was detected for the left inferior frontal gyrus (Pars orbitalis) volume, with the ADHD group exhibiting smaller Pars orbitalis volumes. Dimensional measures of autism (SRS total raw score) and ADHD (DISYPS-II FBB-ADHD score) had no significant influence on the left Pars orbitalis volume. Both, ASD and ADHD tended to have an effect on cortical thickness or mean curvature, which did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. Conclusion: Our results underline that ADHD rather than ASD is associated with volume loss in the left inferior frontal gyrus (Pars orbitalis). This area might play a relevant role in modulating symptoms of inattention and/or impulsivity in ADHD. The effect of comorbid ADHD in ASD samples and vice versa, on cortical thickness and mean curvature, requires further investigation in larger samples.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathrin Nickel
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Medicine, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Ludger Tebartz van Elst
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Medicine, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Jacek Manko
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Faculty of Medicine, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Josef Unterrainer
- Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Reinhold Rauh
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Faculty of Medicine, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Christoph Klein
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Faculty of Medicine, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Dominique Endres
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Medicine, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Christoph P Kaller
- Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Irina Mader
- Department of Neuroradiology, Faculty of Medicine, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Andreas Riedel
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Medicine, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Monica Biscaldi
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Faculty of Medicine, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Simon Maier
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Medicine, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
72
|
Park S, Kim T, Shin SA, Kim YK, Sohn BK, Park HJ, Youn JH, Lee JY. Behavioral and Neuroimaging Evidence for Facial Emotion Recognition in Elderly Korean Adults with Mild Cognitive Impairment, Alzheimer's Disease, and Frontotemporal Dementia. Front Aging Neurosci 2017; 9:389. [PMID: 29249960 PMCID: PMC5714934 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00389] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/24/2017] [Accepted: 11/13/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Background: Facial emotion recognition (FER) is impaired in individuals with frontotemporal dementia (FTD) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) when compared to healthy older adults. Since deficits in emotion recognition are closely related to caregiver burden or social interactions, researchers have fundamental interest in FER performance in patients with dementia. Purpose: The purpose of this study was to identify the performance profiles of six facial emotions (i.e., fear, anger, disgust, sadness, surprise, and happiness) and neutral faces measured among Korean healthy control (HCs), and those with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), AD, and FTD. Additionally, the neuroanatomical correlates of facial emotions were investigated. Methods: A total of 110 (33 HC, 32 MCI, 32 AD, 13 FTD) older adult participants were recruited from two different medical centers in metropolitan areas of South Korea. These individuals underwent an FER test that was used to assess the recognition of emotions or absence of emotion (neutral) in 35 facial stimuli. Repeated measures two-way analyses of variance were used to examine the distinct profiles of emotional recognition among the four groups. We also performed brain imaging and voxel-based morphometry (VBM) on the participants to examine the associations between FER scores and gray matter volume. Results: The mean score of negative emotion recognition (i.e., fear, anger, disgust, and sadness) clearly discriminated FTD participants from individuals with MCI and AD and HC [F(3,106) = 10.829, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.235], whereas the mean score of positive emotion recognition (i.e., surprise and happiness) did not. A VBM analysis showed negative emotions were correlated with gray matter volume of anterior temporal regions, whereas positive emotions were related to gray matter volume of fronto-parietal regions. Conclusion: Impairment of negative FER in patients with FTD is cross-cultural. The discrete neural correlates of FER indicate that emotional recognition processing is a multi-modal system in the brain. Focusing on the negative emotion recognition is a more effective way to discriminate healthy aging, MCI, and AD from FTD in older Korean adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Soowon Park
- Department of Education, Sejong University, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Taehoon Kim
- Department of Psychiatry, Neuroscience Research Institute, Seoul National University and SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Seong A Shin
- Department of Biomedical Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea.,Department of Nuclear Medicine, SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Yu Kyeong Kim
- Department of Nuclear Medicine, SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Bo Kyung Sohn
- Department of Psychiatry, Inje Univiersity Sanggye Paik Hospital, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Hyeon-Ju Park
- Department of Psychiatry, Neuroscience Research Institute, Seoul National University and SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Jung-Hae Youn
- Graduate School of Clinical Counseling Psychology, CHA University, Pocheon, South Korea
| | - Jun-Young Lee
- Department of Psychiatry, Neuroscience Research Institute, Seoul National University and SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Seoul, South Korea
| |
Collapse
|
73
|
Hoche F, Guell X, Sherman JC, Vangel MG, Schmahmann JD. Cerebellar Contribution to Social Cognition. THE CEREBELLUM 2017; 15:732-743. [PMID: 26585120 DOI: 10.1007/s12311-015-0746-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 140] [Impact Index Per Article: 17.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
Emotion attribution (EA) from faces is key to social cognition, and deficits in perception of emotions from faces underlie neuropsychiatric disorders in which cerebellar pathology is reported. Here, we test the hypothesis that the cerebellum contributes to social cognition through EA from faces. We examined 57 patients with cerebellar disorders and 57 healthy controls. Thirty-one patients had complex cerebrocerebellar disease (complex cerebrocerebellar disease group (CD)); 26 had disease isolated to cerebellum (isolated cerebellar disease group (ID)). EA was measured with the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test (RMET), and informants were administered a novel questionnaire, the Cerebellar Neuropsychiatric Rating Scale (CNRS). EA was impaired in all patients (CD p < 0.001, ID p < 0.001). When analyzed for valence categories, both CD and ID missed more positive and negative stimuli. Positive targets produced the highest deficit (CD p < 0.001, ID p = 0.004). EA impairments correlated with CNRS measures of deficient social skills (p < 0.05) and autism spectrum behaviors (p < 0.005). Patients had difficulties with emotion regulation (CD p < 0.001, ID p < 0.001), autism spectrum behaviors (CD p < 0.049, ID p < 0.001), and psychosis spectrum symptoms (CD p < 0.021, ID p < 0.002). ID informants endorsed deficient social skills (CD p < 0.746, ID p < 0.003) and impaired attention regulation (CD p < 0.144, ID p < 0.001). Within the psychosis spectrum domain, CD patients were worse than controls for lack of empathy (CD p = 0.05; ID p = 0.49). Thus, patients with cerebellar damage were impaired on an EA task associated with deficient social skills and autism spectrum behaviors and experienced psychosocial difficulties on the CNRS. This has relevance for ataxias, the cerebellar cognitive affective/Schmahmann syndrome, and neuropsychiatric disorders with cerebellar pathology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Franziska Hoche
- Ataxia Unit, Cognitive Behavioral Neurology Unit, Laboratory for Neuroanatomy and Cerebellar Neurobiology, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, 175 Cambridge Street, 02114, Boston, MA, USA.
| | - Xavier Guell
- Ataxia Unit, Cognitive Behavioral Neurology Unit, Laboratory for Neuroanatomy and Cerebellar Neurobiology, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, 175 Cambridge Street, 02114, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Janet C Sherman
- Psychology Assessment Center, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Mark G Vangel
- Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General, Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Jeremy D Schmahmann
- Ataxia Unit, Cognitive Behavioral Neurology Unit, Laboratory for Neuroanatomy and Cerebellar Neurobiology, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, 175 Cambridge Street, 02114, Boston, MA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
74
|
De Risi M, Di Gennaro G, Picardi A, Casciato S, Grammaldo LG, D'Aniello A, Lanni D, Meletti S, Modugno N. Facial emotion decoding in patients with Parkinson's disease. Int J Neurosci 2017; 128:71-78. [DOI: 10.1080/00207454.2017.1366475] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Angelo Picardi
- Mental Health Unit, National Centre of Epidemiology, Surveillance and Health Promotion, Italian National Institute of Health, Rome, Italy
| | | | | | | | | | - Stefano Meletti
- Department of Biomedical, Metabolic, and Neural Sciences, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Modena, Italy; N.O.C.S.A.E. Hospital, AUSL Modena, Modena, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
75
|
Celeghin A, Diano M, Bagnis A, Viola M, Tamietto M. Basic Emotions in Human Neuroscience: Neuroimaging and Beyond. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1432. [PMID: 28883803 PMCID: PMC5573709 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01432] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2017] [Accepted: 08/07/2017] [Indexed: 01/17/2023] Open
Abstract
The existence of so-called ‘basic emotions’ and their defining attributes represents a long lasting and yet unsettled issue in psychology. Recently, neuroimaging evidence, especially related to the advent of neuroimaging meta-analytic methods, has revitalized this debate in the endeavor of systems and human neuroscience. The core theme focuses on the existence of unique neural bases that are specific and characteristic for each instance of basic emotion. Here we review this evidence, outlining contradictory findings, strengths and limits of different approaches. Constructionism dismisses the existence of dedicated neural structures for basic emotions, considering that the assumption of a one-to-one relationship between neural structures and their functions is central to basic emotion theories. While these critiques are useful to pinpoint current limitations of basic emotions theories, we argue that they do not always appear equally generative in fostering new testable accounts on how the brain relates to affective functions. We then consider evidence beyond PET and fMRI, including results concerning the relation between basic emotions and awareness and data from neuropsychology on patients with focal brain damage. Evidence from lesion studies are indeed particularly informative, as they are able to bring correlational evidence typical of neuroimaging studies to causation, thereby characterizing which brain structures are necessary for, rather than simply related to, basic emotion processing. These other studies shed light on attributes often ascribed to basic emotions, such as automaticity of perception, quick onset, and brief duration. Overall, we consider that evidence in favor of the neurobiological underpinnings of basic emotions outweighs dismissive approaches. In fact, the concept of basic emotions can still be fruitful, if updated to current neurobiological knowledge that overcomes traditional one-to-one localization of functions in the brain. In particular, we propose that the structure-function relationship between brain and emotions is better described in terms of pluripotentiality, which refers to the fact that one neural structure can fulfill multiple functions, depending on the functional network and pattern of co-activations displayed at any given moment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alessia Celeghin
- Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology, Center of Research on Psychology in Somatic Diseases, Tilburg UniversityTilburg, Netherlands.,Department of Psychology, University of TurinTurin, Italy
| | - Matteo Diano
- Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology, Center of Research on Psychology in Somatic Diseases, Tilburg UniversityTilburg, Netherlands.,Department of Psychology, University of TurinTurin, Italy
| | - Arianna Bagnis
- Department of Psychology, University of TurinTurin, Italy
| | - Marco Viola
- Centre for Neurocognition, Epistemology and Theoretical Syntax, Scuola di Studi Superiori PaviaPavia, Italy.,Faculty of Philosophy, Vita-Salute San Raffaele UniversityMilan, Italy
| | - Marco Tamietto
- Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology, Center of Research on Psychology in Somatic Diseases, Tilburg UniversityTilburg, Netherlands.,Department of Psychology, University of TurinTurin, Italy.,Department of Experimental Psychology, University of OxfordOxford, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
76
|
Icer S, Benli SG, Gumus K, Demirci E, Ozmen S, Doganay S. Can Functional Connectivity at Resting Brain in ADHD Indicate the Impairments in Sensory-Motor Functions and Face/Emotion Recognition? J Med Biol Eng 2017. [DOI: 10.1007/s40846-017-0289-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
77
|
Sedda A, Petito S, Guarino M, Stracciari A. Identification and intensity of disgust: Distinguishing visual, linguistic and facial expressions processing in Parkinson disease. Behav Brain Res 2017; 330:30-36. [PMID: 28476571 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.05.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2017] [Revised: 04/27/2017] [Accepted: 05/02/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES & METHODS Most of the studies since now show an impairment for facial displays of disgust recognition in Parkinson disease. A general impairment in disgust processing in patients with Parkinson disease might adversely affect their social interactions, given the relevance of this emotion for human relations. However, despite the importance of faces, disgust is also expressed through other format of visual stimuli such as sentences and visual images. The aim of our study was to explore disgust processing in a sample of patients affected by Parkinson disease, by means of various tests tackling not only facial recognition but also other format of visual stimuli through which disgust can be recognized. RESULTS Our results confirm that patients are impaired in recognizing facial displays of disgust. Further analyses show that patients are also impaired and slower for other facial expressions, with the only exception of happiness. Notably however, patients with Parkinson disease processed visual images and sentences as controls. CONCLUSIONS Our findings show a dissociation within different formats of visual stimuli of disgust, suggesting that Parkinson disease is not characterized by a general compromising of disgust processing, as often suggested. The involvement of the basal ganglia-frontal cortex system might spare some cognitive components of emotional processing, related to memory and culture, at least for disgust.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna Sedda
- Psychology, School of Social Sciences, Heriot Watt University, Edinburgh Campus, UK.
| | - Sara Petito
- School of Psychology and Education Sciences, Bologna University, Cesena Campus, Italy
| | - Maria Guarino
- Neurology Unit, S. Orsola, Malpighi University Hospital, Bologna, Italy
| | - Andrea Stracciari
- School of Psychology and Education Sciences, Bologna University, Cesena Campus, Italy; Neurology Unit, S. Orsola, Malpighi University Hospital, Bologna, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
78
|
Trujillo SP, Valencia S, Trujillo N, Ugarriza JE, Rodríguez MV, Rendón J, Pineda DA, López JD, Ibañez A, Parra MA. Atypical Modulations of N170 Component during Emotional Processing and Their Links to Social Behaviors in Ex-combatants. Front Hum Neurosci 2017; 11:244. [PMID: 28588462 PMCID: PMC5440593 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00244] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2017] [Accepted: 04/25/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Emotional processing (EP) is crucial for the elaboration and implementation of adaptive social strategies. EP is also necessary for the expression of social cognition and behavior (SCB) patterns. It is well-known that war contexts induce socio-emotional atypical functioning, in particular for those who participate in combats. Thus, ex-combatants represent an ideal non-clinical population to explore EP modulation and to evaluate its relation with SCB. The aim of this study was to explore EP and its relation with SCB dimensions such as empathy, theory of mind and social skills in a sample of 50 subjects, of which 30 were ex-combatants from illegally armed groups in Colombia, and 20 controls without combat experience. We adapted an Emotional Recognition Task for faces and words and synchronized it with electroencephalographic recording. Ex-combatants presented with higher assertion skills and showed more pronounced brain responses to faces than Controls. They did not show the bias toward anger observed in control participants whereby the latter group was more likely to misclassify neutral faces as angry. However, ex-combatants showed an atypical word valence processing. That is, words with different emotions yielded no differences in N170 modulations. SCB variables were successfully predicted by neurocognitive variables. Our results suggest that in ex-combatants the links between EP and SCB functions are reorganized. This may reflect neurocognitive modulations associated to chronic exposure to war experiences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sandra P Trujillo
- Doctoral Program in Psychology, Department of Experimental Psychology, Universidad de GranadaGranada, Spain.,GISAME, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia
| | - Stella Valencia
- GISAME, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia
| | - Natalia Trujillo
- GISAME, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia.,Neuroscience Group, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia
| | - Juan E Ugarriza
- Facultad de Jurisprudencia, Universidad del RosarioBogotá, Colombia
| | - Mónica V Rodríguez
- SISTEMIC, Facultad de Ingeniería, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia
| | - Jorge Rendón
- Neuroscience Group, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia.,Neuropsychology and Behavior Group, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia.,Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Maastricht UniversityMaastricht, Netherlands
| | - David A Pineda
- Neuropsychology and Behavior Group, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia
| | - José D López
- SISTEMIC, Facultad de Ingeniería, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia
| | - Agustín Ibañez
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma del CaribeBarranquilla, Colombia.,Center for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience (CSCN), School of Psychology, Universidad Adolfo IbañezSantiago, Chile.,National Scientific and Technical Research CouncilBuenos Aires, Argentina.,Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Translational and Cognitive Neuroscience, INECO Foundation, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina.,ACR Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, SydneyNSW, Australia
| | - Mario A Parra
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma del CaribeBarranquilla, Colombia.,Psychology, School of Social Sciences, Heriot-Watt UniversityEdinburgh, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
79
|
Trujillo S, Trujillo N, Lopez JD, Gomez D, Valencia S, Rendon J, Pineda DA, Parra MA. Social Cognitive Training Improves Emotional Processing and Reduces Aggressive Attitudes in Ex-combatants. Front Psychol 2017; 8:510. [PMID: 28428767 PMCID: PMC5382221 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00510] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/29/2016] [Accepted: 03/20/2017] [Indexed: 01/15/2023] Open
Abstract
Emotional processing (EP) is a complex cognitive function necessary to successfully adjust to social environments where we need to interpret and respond to cues that convey threat or reward signals. Ex-combatants have consistently shown atypical EP as well as poor social interactions. Available reintegration programs aim to facilitate the re-adaptation of ex-combatants to their communities. However, they do not incorporate actions to improve EP and to enhance cognitive-emotional regulation. The present study was aimed at evaluating the usefulness of an intervention focused on Social Cognitive Training (SCT), which was designed to equip ex-combatants enrolled in the Social Reintegration Route with EP and social cognition skills. A group of 31 ex-combatants (mean age of 37.2, 29 men) from Colombian illegal armed groups were recruited into this study. Of these, 16 were invited to take part in a SCT and the other continued with the conventional reintegration intervention. Both groups underwent 12 training sessions in a period 12–14 weeks. They were assessed with a comprehensive protocol which included Psychosocial, Behavioral, and Emotion Processing instruments. The scores on these instruments prior to and after the intervention were compared within and between groups. Both groups were matched at baseline. Ex-combatants receiving the SCT experienced significant improvements in EP and a reduction in aggressive attitudes, effects not observed in those continuing the conventional reintegration intervention. This is the first study that achieves such outcomes in such a population using SCT intervention. We discuss the implications of such results toward better social reintegration strategies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sandra Trujillo
- Grupo de Investigación en Salud Mental, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellín, Colombia.,Doctoral Program in Psychology, Department of Psychology, Universidad de GranadaGranada, España.,Department of Experimental Psychology, University of GranadaGranada, Spain
| | - Natalia Trujillo
- Grupo de Investigación en Salud Mental, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellín, Colombia.,Grupo de Neurociencias, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellin, Colombia
| | - Jose D Lopez
- SISTEMIC, Facultad de Ingeniería, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellín, Colombia
| | - Diana Gomez
- Grupo de Investigación en Salud Mental, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellín, Colombia
| | - Stella Valencia
- Grupo de Investigación en Salud Mental, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellín, Colombia
| | - Jorge Rendon
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Maastricht UniversityMaastricht, Netherlands
| | - David A Pineda
- Grupo de Neuropsicología y Conducta, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellin, Colombia
| | - Mario A Parra
- School of Social Sciences, Psychology, Heriot-Watt UniversityEdinburgh, UK.,Cerebro, Cognición y Procesos Sociales, Psicologia, Universidad Autónoma del CaribeBarranquilla, Colombia
| |
Collapse
|
80
|
Abstract
BACKGROUND Previous studies suggest that adults with Tourette syndrome (TS) can respond unconventionally on tasks involving social cognition. We therefore hypothesized that these patients would exhibit different neural responses to healthy controls in response to emotionally salient expressions of human eyes. METHOD Twenty-five adults with TS and 25 matched healthy controls were scanned using fMRI during the standard version of the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Task which requires mental state judgements, and a novel comparison version requiring judgements about age. RESULTS During prompted mental state recognition, greater activity was apparent in TS within left orbitofrontal cortex, posterior cingulate, right amygdala and right temporo-parietal junction (TPJ), while reduced activity was apparent in regions including left inferior parietal cortex. Age judgement elicited greater activity in TS within precuneus, medial prefrontal and temporal regions involved in mentalizing. The interaction between group and task revealed differential activity in areas including right inferior frontal gyrus. Task-related activity in the TPJ covaried with global ratings of the urge to tic. CONCLUSIONS While recognizing mental states, adults with TS exhibit greater activity than controls in brain areas involved in the processing of negative emotion, in addition to reduced activity in regions associated with the attribution of agency. In addition, increased recruitment of areas involved in mental state reasoning is apparent in these patients when mentalizing is not a task requirement. Our findings highlight differential neural reactivity in response to emotive social cues in TS, which may interact with tic expression.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C M Eddy
- BSMHFT National Centre for Mental Health,Birmingham,UK
| | - A E Cavanna
- BSMHFT National Centre for Mental Health,Birmingham,UK
| | - P C Hansen
- Birmingham University Imaging Centre and School of Psychology,College of Life and Environmental Sciences,University of Birmingham,UK
| |
Collapse
|
81
|
Grace SA, Buchanan BG, Maller JJ, Toh WL, Castle DJ, Rossell SL. Reduced cortical thickness in body dysmorphic disorder. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 2017; 259:25-28. [PMID: 27918913 DOI: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.11.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2016] [Revised: 09/28/2016] [Accepted: 11/21/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Recent neuroimaging studies in body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) have implicated abnormal structure and function of occipito-temporal and fronto-limbic regions in the potential pathophysiology of the disorder. To date, morphometric investigations have yielded inconsistent results, and have suggested that clinical symptoms may mediate structural brain abnormalities in BDD. We measured Grey Matter (GM) cortical thickness in 20 participants with BDD and 20 healthy control participants matched on age, gender, estimated IQ and handedness. We observed cortical thinning in BDD patients compared with healthy control participants within the left middle temporal and left inferior parietal gyrus. No significant relationships between cortical thickness and BDD symptom severity, insight, social anxiety and depression were observed within the BDD group. Thinning within left temporal and left inferior parietal regions supports the involvement of these regions in the pathophysiology of BDD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sally A Grace
- Brain and Psychological Sciences Research Centre, Swinburne University, Melbourne, Australia.
| | - Ben G Buchanan
- Monash University School of Psychological Science, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Jerome J Maller
- Monash Alfred Psychiatry Research Centre, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Wei Lin Toh
- Monash Alfred Psychiatry Research Centre, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
| | - David J Castle
- Psychiatry, St Vincent's Hospital, Melbourne, Australia; Psychiatry, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Susan L Rossell
- Monash Alfred Psychiatry Research Centre, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia; Psychiatry, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
82
|
Lesions of Left Basal Ganglia and Insula Structures Impair Executive Functions but not Emotion Recognition: A Case Report. BRAIN IMPAIR 2016. [DOI: 10.1017/brimp.2016.23] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
Increasing research evidence suggests that basal ganglia are an important part of frontal-subcortical circuit which is involved not only in motor control but also in affective, cognitive and executive functions. In this article, we describe the ability of facial emotion recognition and cognitive functioning in a patient with left basal ganglia and insula damage. The patient's ability to recognise facial emotional expressions was intact in spite of unilateral injury of the left insula and basal ganglia. He showed preserved intellectual function in general, but experienced difficulties on subsets of the executive functions: set-shifting and ability to activate or generate cognitive strategies, commonly found in patients with caudate lesions. This case contributes to evidence that striatal structures are important for executive functions.
Collapse
|
83
|
Self-Awareness After Brain Injury: Relation with Emotion Recognition and Effects of Treatment. BRAIN IMPAIR 2016. [DOI: 10.1017/brimp.2016.28] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
Self-awareness is often impaired after acquired brain injury (ABI) and this hampers rehabilitation, in general: unrealistic reports by patients about their functioning and poor motivation and compliance with treatment. We evaluated a self-awareness treatment that was part of a treatment protocol on executive dysfunction (Spikman, Boelen, Lamberts, Brouwer, & Fasotti, 2010). A total of 63 patients were included, aged 17–70, suffering non-progressive ABI, and minimum time post-onset of 3 months. Self-awareness was measured by comparing the patient's Dysexecutive Questionnaire (Wilson, Alderman, Burgess, Emslie, & Evans, 1996) score with that of an independent other. As emotion recognition is associated with self-awareness and influences the effect of rehabilitation treatment, we assessed this function using the Facial Expressions of Emotion-Stimuli and Tests (Young, Perrett, Calder, Sprengelmeyer, & Ekman, 2002). Results showed that patients in the experimental treatment group (n= 29) had better self-awareness after training than control patients (n= 34). Moreover, our results confirmed that the level of self-awareness before treatment was related to emotion recognition. Hence, self-awareness can improve after neuropsychological treatment fostering self-monitoring. Since neuropsychological treatment involves social learning, impairments in social cognition should be taken into account before starting and during treatment.
Collapse
|
84
|
Rizzolatti G, Sinigaglia C. The mirror mechanism: a basic principle of brain function. Nat Rev Neurosci 2016; 17:757-765. [PMID: 27761004 DOI: 10.1038/nrn.2016.135] [Citation(s) in RCA: 251] [Impact Index Per Article: 27.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
The mirror mechanism is a basic brain mechanism that transforms sensory representations of others' behaviour into one's own motor or visceromotor representations concerning that behaviour. According to its location in the brain, it may fulfil a range of cognitive functions, including action and emotion understanding. In each case, it may enable a route to knowledge of others' behaviour, which mainly depends on one's own motor or visceromotor representations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Giacomo Rizzolatti
- University of Parma, Department of Neuroscience, via Volturno 39, I-43100 Parma, Italy.,Brain Center for Motor and Social Cognition, Italian Institute of Technology, I-43100 Parma, Italy
| | - Corrado Sinigaglia
- University of Milan, Department of Philosophy, via Festa del Perdono 7, I-20122 Milano, Italy.,Center for the Study of Social Action, University of Milan, I-20122 Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
85
|
Royet JP, Meunier D, Torquet N, Mouly AM, Jiang T. The Neural Bases of Disgust for Cheese: An fMRI Study. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:511. [PMID: 27799903 PMCID: PMC5065955 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00511] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2016] [Accepted: 09/27/2016] [Indexed: 01/01/2023] Open
Abstract
The study of food aversion in humans by the induction of illness is ethically unthinkable, and it is difficult to propose a type of food that is disgusting for everybody. However, although cheese is considered edible by most people, it can also be perceived as particularly disgusting to some individuals. As such, the perception of cheese constitutes a good model to study the cerebral processes of food disgust and aversion. In this study, we show that a higher percentage of people are disgusted by cheese than by other types of food. Functional magnetic resonance imaging then reveals that the internal and external globus pallidus and the substantia nigra belonging to the basal ganglia are more activated in participants who dislike or diswant to eat cheese (Anti) than in other participants who like to eat cheese, as revealed following stimulation with cheese odors and pictures. We suggest that the aforementioned basal ganglia structures commonly involved in reward are also involved in the aversive motivated behaviors. Our results further show that the ventral pallidum, a core structure of the reward circuit, is deactivated in Anti subjects stimulated by cheese in the wanting task, highlighting the suppression of motivation-related activation in subjects disgusted by cheese.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jean-Pierre Royet
- Olfaction: From Coding to Memory Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U1028 - Université de Lyon 1 Lyon, France
| | - David Meunier
- Olfaction: From Coding to Memory Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U1028 - Université de Lyon 1 Lyon, France
| | - Nicolas Torquet
- Sorbonne Universités, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut de Biologie Paris Seine, UM 119, CNRS, UMR 8246, Neuroscience Paris Seine Paris, France
| | - Anne-Marie Mouly
- Olfaction: From Coding to Memory Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U1028 - Université de Lyon 1 Lyon, France
| | - Tao Jiang
- Olfaction: From Coding to Memory Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U1028 - Université de Lyon 1 Lyon, France
| |
Collapse
|
86
|
Aho-Özhan HEA, Keller J, Heimrath J, Uttner I, Kassubek J, Birbaumer N, Ludolph AC, Lulé D. Perception of Emotional Facial Expressions in Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis (ALS) at Behavioural and Brain Metabolic Level. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0164655. [PMID: 27741285 PMCID: PMC5065224 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0164655] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/26/2016] [Accepted: 09/28/2016] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) primarily impairs motor abilities but also affects cognition and emotional processing. We hypothesise that subjective ratings of emotional stimuli depicting social interactions and facial expressions is changed in ALS. It was found that recognition of negative emotions and ability to mentalize other's intentions is reduced. METHODS Processing of emotions in faces was investigated. A behavioural test of Ekman faces expressing six basic emotions was presented to 30 ALS patients and 29 age-, gender and education matched healthy controls. Additionally, a subgroup of 15 ALS patients that were able to lie supine in the scanner and 14 matched healthy controls viewed the Ekman faces during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Affective state and a number of daily social contacts were measured. RESULTS ALS patients recognized disgust and fear less accurately than healthy controls. In fMRI, reduced brain activity was seen in areas involved in processing of negative emotions replicating our previous results. During processing of sad faces, increased brain activity was seen in areas associated with social emotions in right inferior frontal gyrus and reduced activity in hippocampus bilaterally. No differences in brain activity were seen for any of the other emotional expressions. Inferior frontal gyrus activity for sad faces was associated with increased amount of social contacts of ALS patients. CONCLUSION ALS patients showed decreased brain and behavioural responses in processing of disgust and fear and an altered brain response pattern for sadness. The negative consequences of neurodegenerative processes in the course of ALS might be counteracted by positive emotional activity and positive social interactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Jürgen Keller
- Department of Neurology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
| | | | - Ingo Uttner
- Department of Neurology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
| | - Jan Kassubek
- Department of Neurology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
| | - Niels Birbaumer
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, Eberhard-Karls-University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
- The Wyss Center for Bio and Neuroengineering, Geneva, Switzerland
| | | | - Dorothée Lulé
- Department of Neurology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, Eberhard-Karls-University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
87
|
Sabharwal A, Szekely A, Kotov R, Mukherjee P, Leung HC, Barch DM, Mohanty A. Transdiagnostic neural markers of emotion-cognition interaction in psychotic disorders. JOURNAL OF ABNORMAL PSYCHOLOGY 2016; 125:907-922. [PMID: 27618279 PMCID: PMC5576592 DOI: 10.1037/abn0000196] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
Abstract
Deficits in working memory (WM) and emotion processing are prominent impairments in psychotic disorders, and have been linked to reduced quality of life and real-world functioning. Translation of knowledge regarding the neural circuitry implementing these deficits into improved diagnosis and targeted treatments has been slow, possibly because of categorical definitions of disorders. Using the dimensional Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework, we investigated the clinical and practical utility of transdiagnostic behavioral and neural measures of emotion-related WM disruption across psychotic disorders. Behavioral and functional MRI data were recorded while 53 participants with psychotic disorders and 29 participants with no history of psychosis performed a modified n-back task with fear and neutral distractors. Hierarchical regression analyses showed that psychotic symptoms entered after diagnosis accounted for unique variance in fear versus neutral accuracy and activation in the ventrolateral, dorsolateral, and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, but diagnostic group entered after psychotic symptoms did not. These results remained even after controlling for negative symptoms, disorganized symptoms, and dysphoria. Finally, worse accuracy and greater prefrontal activity were associated with poorer social functioning and unemployment across diagnostic groups. Present results support the transdiagnostic nature of behavioral and neuroimaging measures of emotion-related WM disruption as they relate to psychotic symptoms, irrespective of diagnosis. They also provide support for the practical utility of these markers in explaining real-world functioning. Overall, these results elucidate key aspects of the RDoC construct of WM maintenance by clarifying its transdiagnostic importance and clinical utility in psychotic disorders. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Akos Szekely
- Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University
| | - Roman Kotov
- Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University
| | | | | | - Deanna M. Barch
- Departments of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis
| | | |
Collapse
|
88
|
Manderino L, Spitznagel MB, Strain G, Devlin M, Cohen R, Crosby RD, Mitchell JE, Gunstad J. Cognitive dysfunction predicts poorer emotion recognition in bariatric surgery candidates. Obes Sci Pract 2016; 1:97-103. [PMID: 27668086 PMCID: PMC5019263 DOI: 10.1002/osp4.9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/02/2015] [Revised: 07/22/2015] [Accepted: 07/30/2015] [Indexed: 11/06/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Deficits in traditional cognitive domains (e.g. executive function and memory) are common in persons with severe obesity, but it is unclear if this pattern of dysfunction extends to social cognition. The present study examined whether cognitive impairment was associated with poorer emotion recognition in bariatric surgery candidates. METHODS One hundred sixteen bariatric surgery candidates (mean age = 43.62 ± 11.03; 81% female) completed the computerized Integneuro test battery as part of a larger study visit. In addition to assessing traditional cognitive domains, the Integneuro also includes an emotion recognition measure. This task presents 48 faces (eight different individuals depicting neutral, happiness, fear, sadness, anger and disgust), and participants must choose the correct verbal label from six expression options. Number of correct responses and average reaction time for correct responses served as primary dependent variables. RESULTS Stepwise multiple regression analyses revealed that older age, more maze errors, and history of hypertension predicted less accuracy in emotion recognition (adjusted R2 = .22, F[3, 111] = 11.86, p < .001) and that slower switching of attention-digits, worse long-delay recall, and older age predicted speed of responses (adjusted R2 = .26, F[3, 111] = 13.00, p < .001). DISCUSSION Results show that cognitive dysfunction is associated with poorer performance on a computerized test of emotion recognition, consistent with those in persons with a range of psychiatric and neurological disorders. Additional work is needed to clarify the mechanisms and functional impact of these impairments, especially in relation to weight loss following bariatric surgery.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - G Strain
- Weill Cornell Medical College New York NY USA
| | - M Devlin
- Columbia University Medical Center New York NY USA
| | - R Cohen
- University of Florida Institute on Aging Gainesville FL USA
| | - R D Crosby
- University of North Dakota School of Medicine and Health Sciences Neuropsychiatric Research Institute Fargo ND USA
| | - J E Mitchell
- University of North Dakota School of Medicine and Health Sciences Neuropsychiatric Research Institute Fargo ND USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
89
|
Atkinson AP, Dittrich WH, Gemmell AJ, Young AW. Emotion Perception from Dynamic and Static Body Expressions in Point-Light and Full-Light Displays. Perception 2016; 33:717-46. [PMID: 15330366 DOI: 10.1068/p5096] [Citation(s) in RCA: 376] [Impact Index Per Article: 41.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Research on emotion recognition has been dominated by studies of photographs of facial expressions. A full understanding of emotion perception and its neural substrate will require investigations that employ dynamic displays and means of expression other than the face. Our aims were: (i) to develop a set of dynamic and static whole-body expressions of basic emotions for systematic investigations of clinical populations, and for use in functional-imaging studies; (ii) to assess forced-choice emotion-classification performance with these stimuli relative to the results of previous studies; and (iii) to test the hypotheses that more exaggerated whole-body movements would produce (a) more accurate emotion classification and (b) higher ratings of emotional intensity. Ten actors portrayed 5 emotions (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, and sadness) at 3 levels of exaggeration, with their faces covered. Two identical sets of 150 emotion portrayals (full-light and point-light) were created from the same digital footage, along with corresponding static images of the ‘peak’ of each emotion portrayal. Recognition tasks confirmed previous findings that basic emotions are readily identifiable from body movements, even when static form information is minimised by use of point-light displays, and that full-light and even point-light displays can convey identifiable emotions, though rather less efficiently than dynamic displays. Recognition success differed for individual emotions, corroborating earlier results about the importance of distinguishing differences in movement characteristics for different emotional expressions. The patterns of misclassifications were in keeping with earlier findings on emotional clustering. Exaggeration of body movement (a) enhanced recognition accuracy, especially for the dynamic point-light displays, but notably not for sadness, and (b) produced higher emotional-intensity ratings, regardless of lighting condition, for movies but to a lesser extent for stills, indicating that intensity judgments of body gestures rely more on movement (or form-from-movement) than static form information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anthony P Atkinson
- Department of Psychology, University of Durham, Science Laboratories, South Road, Durham DH1 3LE, UK.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
90
|
Abstract
Social cognitive neuroscience is a rapidly emerging field that utilizes cognitive neuroscientific techniques (e.g., lesion studies, neuroimaging) to address concepts traditionally in the social psychological realm (e.g., attitudes, stereotypes). The purpose of this article is to review published neuroscientific and neuropsychological research into social cognition. The author focuses on the role of the prefrontal cortex in social behavior and presents a framework that provides cohesion of this research. The article proposes that this framework will be useful in guiding future social cognitive neuroscientific research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jacqueline N. Wood
- National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD
| |
Collapse
|
91
|
Verstaen A, Eckart JA, Muhtadie L, Otero MC, Sturm VE, Haase CM, Miller BL, Levenson RW. Insular atrophy and diminished disgust reactivity. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2016; 16:903-12. [PMID: 27148847 DOI: 10.1037/emo0000195] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Disgust is an emotion that helps us deal with potential contamination (Rozin & Fallon, 1987). It produces a distinctive facial expression (e.g., wrinkled nose) and a physiological response that is accompanied by strong visceral sensations (e.g., nausea). Given the important role that the anterior insula plays in processing and integrating visceral information (Craig, 2009), it is likely to be centrally involved in disgust. Despite this, few studies have examined the link between insular degeneration and the experience, physiology, and expression of disgust. We studied a group that was heterogeneous in terms of insular damage: 84 patients with neurodegenerative diseases (i.e., frontotemporal dementia, corticobasal syndrome, progressive supranuclear palsy, Alzheimer's disease) and 29 controls. Subjects viewed films that elicit high levels of disgust and sadness. Emotional reactivity was assessed using self-report, peripheral physiology, and facial behavior. Regional brain volumes (insula, putamen, pallidum, caudate, and amygdala) were determined from structural MRIs using the FreeSurfer method. Results indicated that smaller insular volumes were associated with reduced disgust responding in self-report and physiological reactivity, but not in facial behavior. In terms of the specificity of these findings, insular volume did not predict sadness reactivity, and disgust reactivity was not predicted by putamen, pallidum, and caudate volumes (lower self-reported disgust was associated with smaller amygdala volume). These findings underscore the central role of the insula in the experience and physiology of disgust. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alice Verstaen
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley
| | - Janet A Eckart
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley
| | - Luma Muhtadie
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley
| | - Marcela C Otero
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley
| | - Virginia E Sturm
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco
| | - Claudia M Haase
- School of Education and Social Policy, Northwestern University
| | - Bruce L Miller
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco
| | | |
Collapse
|
92
|
Somatosensory Representations Link the Perception of Emotional Expressions and Sensory Experience. eNeuro 2016; 3:eN-NWR-0090-15. [PMID: 27280154 PMCID: PMC4894916 DOI: 10.1523/eneuro.0090-15.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2015] [Revised: 04/08/2016] [Accepted: 04/11/2016] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Studies of human emotion perception have linked a distributed set of brain regions to the recognition of emotion in facial, vocal, and body expressions. In particular, lesions to somatosensory cortex in the right hemisphere have been shown to impair recognition of facial and vocal expressions of emotion. Although these findings suggest that somatosensory cortex represents body states associated with distinct emotions, such as a furrowed brow or gaping jaw, functional evidence directly linking somatosensory activity and subjective experience during emotion perception is critically lacking. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging and multivariate decoding techniques, we show that perceiving vocal and facial expressions of emotion yields hemodynamic activity in right somatosensory cortex that discriminates among emotion categories, exhibits somatotopic organization, and tracks self-reported sensory experience. The findings both support embodied accounts of emotion and provide mechanistic insight into how emotional expressions are capable of biasing subjective experience in those who perceive them.
Collapse
|
93
|
Specific disgust processing in the left insula: New evidence from direct electrical stimulation. Neuropsychologia 2016; 84:29-35. [PMID: 26836143 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2015] [Revised: 12/26/2015] [Accepted: 01/29/2016] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies yielded controversial results concerning the specific role of the insula in recognizing the facial expression of disgust. To verify whether the insula has a selective role in facial disgust processing, emotion recognition was studied in thirteen patients during intraoperative stimulation of the insula in awake surgery performed for removal of a glioma close to this structure. Direct electrical stimulation of the left insula produced a general decrease in emotion recognition but only in the case of disgust there was a statistically significant detrimental effect (p=0.004). Happiness and anger were the best and the worst recognized emotion, respectively. The worst baseline performance with anger and, partly, fear could be explained with the involvement of the left temporal regions, striatum, and the connection between the striatum and the frontal lobe, as suggested in previous studies. Therefore, upon these intra-operative evidences, we argue for a selective role of the left insula in disgust recognition, although a (non significant) decrease in the recognition of other negative emotions was found. However, additional networks can develop, as demonstrated by the fact that disgust recognition was not impaired after surgery even in patients with insular resection in the current as in previous studies.
Collapse
|
94
|
Senderecka M. Threatening visual stimuli influence response inhibition and error monitoring: An event-related potential study. Biol Psychol 2016; 113:24-36. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.11.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/25/2015] [Revised: 10/02/2015] [Accepted: 11/09/2015] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
95
|
Kucharska K, Wilkos E, Stefanski R, Makowicz G, Ryglewicz D, Slawinska K, Piatkowska-Janko E. Behavioural and Neurophysiological Effects of a Stroke Rehabilitation Program on Emotional Processing in Tuberothalamic Infarct—Case Study. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2016. [DOI: 10.4236/jbbs.2016.61006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
96
|
Berlin HA, Schulz KP, Zhang S, Turetzky R, Rosenthal D, Goodman W. Neural correlates of emotional response inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A preliminary study. Psychiatry Res 2015; 234:259-64. [PMID: 26456416 DOI: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.09.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2015] [Revised: 09/27/2015] [Accepted: 09/27/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Failure to inhibit recurrent anxiety-provoking thoughts is a central symptom of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Neuroimaging studies suggest inhibitory control and disgust processing abnormalities in patients with OCD. However, the emotional modulation of response inhibition deficits in OCD and their neural correlates remain to be elucidated. For this preliminary study we administered an adapted affective response inhibition paradigm, an emotional go/no-go task, during fMRI to characterize the neural systems underlying disgust-related and fear-related inhibition in nine adults with contamination-type OCD compared to ten matched healthy controls. Participants with OCD had significantly greater anterior insula cortex activation when inhibiting responses to both disgusting (bilateral), and fearful (right-sided) images, compared to healthy controls. They also had increased activation in several frontal, temporal, and parietal regions, but there was no evidence of amygdala activation in OCD or healthy participants and no significant between-group differences in performance on the emotion go/no-go task. The anterior insula appears to play a central role in the emotional modulation of response inhibition in contamination-type OCD to both fearful and disgusting images. The insula may serve as a potential treatment target for contamination-type OCD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Heather A Berlin
- Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA; Department of Neuroscience, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA.
| | - Kurt P Schulz
- Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA
| | - Sam Zhang
- Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA
| | - Rachel Turetzky
- Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA
| | - David Rosenthal
- Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA
| | - Wayne Goodman
- Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA; Department of Neuroscience, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
97
|
Vierck E, Joyce PR. Influence of personality and neuropsychological ability on social functioning and self-management in bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res 2015; 229:715-23. [PMID: 26282228 DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.08.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2015] [Revised: 07/17/2015] [Accepted: 08/10/2015] [Indexed: 01/13/2023]
Abstract
A majority of bipolar patients (BD) show functional difficulties even in remission. In recent years cognitive functions and personality characteristics have been associated with occupational and psychosocial outcomes, but findings are not consistent. We assessed personality and cognitive functioning through a range of tests in BD and control participants. Three cognitive domains-verbal memory, facial-executive, and spatial memory-were extracted by principal component analysis. These factors and selected personality dimensions were included in hierarchical regression analysis to predict psychosocial functioning and the use of self-management strategies while controlling for mood status. The best determinants of good psychosocial functioning were good verbal memory and high self-directedness. The use of self-management techniques was associated with a low level of harm-avoidance. Our findings indicate that strategies to improve memory and self-directedness may be useful for increasing functioning in individuals with bipolar disorder.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Esther Vierck
- Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Otago, Christchurch, PO Box 4345, Christchurch, New Zealand.
| | - Peter R Joyce
- Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Otago, Christchurch, PO Box 4345, Christchurch, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
98
|
Woolley JD, Strobl EV, Sturm VE, Shany-Ur T, Poorzand P, Grossman S, Nguyen L, Eckart JA, Levenson RW, Seeley WW, Miller BL, Rankin KP. Impaired Recognition and Regulation of Disgust Is Associated with Distinct but Partially Overlapping Patterns of Decreased Gray Matter Volume in the Ventroanterior Insula. Biol Psychiatry 2015; 78:505-14. [PMID: 25890642 PMCID: PMC4529378 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2014] [Revised: 12/02/2014] [Accepted: 12/03/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The ventroanterior insula is implicated in the experience, expression, and recognition of disgust; however, whether this brain region is required for recognizing disgust or regulating disgusting behaviors remains unknown. METHODS We examined the brain correlates of the presence of disgusting behavior and impaired recognition of disgust using voxel-based morphometry in a sample of 305 patients with heterogeneous patterns of neurodegeneration. Permutation-based analyses were used to determine regions of decreased gray matter volume at a significance level p <= .05 corrected for family-wise error across the whole brain and within the insula. RESULTS Patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and semantic variant primary progressive aphasia were most likely to exhibit disgusting behaviors and were, on average, the most impaired at recognizing disgust in others. Imaging analysis revealed that patients who exhibited disgusting behaviors had significantly less gray matter volume bilaterally in the ventral anterior insula. A region of interest analysis restricted to behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and semantic variant primary progressive aphasia patients alone confirmed this result. Moreover, impaired recognition of disgust was associated with decreased gray matter volume in the bilateral ventroanterior and ventral middle regions of the insula. There was an area of overlap in the bilateral anterior insula where decreased gray matter volume was associated with both the presence of disgusting behavior and impairments in recognizing disgust. CONCLUSIONS These findings suggest that regulating disgusting behaviors and recognizing disgust in others involve two partially overlapping neural systems within the insula. Moreover, the ventral anterior insula is required for both processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Josh D Woolley
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Francisco; San Francisco Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, San Francisco, California.
| | - Eric V Strobl
- University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
| | - Virginia E Sturm
- University of California San Francisco Memory and Aging Center, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California
| | - Tal Shany-Ur
- University of California San Francisco Memory and Aging Center, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California
| | - Pardis Poorzand
- University of California San Francisco Memory and Aging Center, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California
| | | | - Lauren Nguyen
- University of California San Francisco Memory and Aging Center, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California
| | | | | | - William W Seeley
- University of California San Francisco Memory and Aging Center, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California
| | - Bruce L Miller
- University of California San Francisco Memory and Aging Center, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California
| | - Katherine P Rankin
- University of California San Francisco Memory and Aging Center, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California
| |
Collapse
|
99
|
Marsh AA. Understanding amygdala responsiveness to fearful expressions through the lens of psychopathy and altruism. J Neurosci Res 2015; 94:513-25. [PMID: 26366635 DOI: 10.1002/jnr.23668] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2015] [Revised: 08/24/2015] [Accepted: 08/27/2015] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
Abstract
Because the face is the central focus of human social interactions, emotional facial expressions provide a unique window into the emotional lives of others. They play a particularly important role in fostering empathy, which entails understanding and responding to others' emotions, especially distress-related emotions such as fear. This Review considers how fearful facial as well as vocal and postural expressions are interpreted, with an emphasis on the role of the amygdala. The amygdala may be best known for its role in the acquisition and expression of conditioned fear, but it also supports the perception and recognition of others' fear. Various explanations have been supplied for the amygdala's role in interpreting and responding to fearful expressions. They include theories that amygdala responses to fearful expressions 1) reflect heightened vigilance in response to uncertain danger, 2) promote heightened attention to the eye region of faces, 3) represent a response to an unconditioned aversive stimulus, or 4) reflect the generation of an empathic fear response. Among these, only empathic fear explains why amygdala lesions would impair fear recognition across modalities. Supporting the possibility of a link between fundamental empathic processes and amygdala responses to fear is evidence that impaired fear recognition in psychopathic individuals results from amygdala dysfunction, whereas enhanced fear recognition in altruistic individuals results from enhanced amygdala function. Empathic concern and caring behaviors may be fostered by sensitivity to signs of acute distress in others, which relies on intact functioning of the amygdala.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Abigail A Marsh
- Department of Psychology, Georgetown University, Washington, DC
| |
Collapse
|
100
|
Wabnegger A, Ille R, Schwingenschuh P, Katschnig-Winter P, Kögl-Wallner M, Wenzel K, Schienle A. Facial Emotion Recognition in Parkinson's Disease: An fMRI Investigation. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0136110. [PMID: 26285212 PMCID: PMC4540566 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136110] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2015] [Accepted: 07/29/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Findings of behavioral studies on facial emotion recognition in Parkinson's disease (PD) are very heterogeneous. Therefore, the present investigation additionally used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in order to compare brain activation during emotion perception between PD patients and healthy controls. METHODS AND FINDINGS We included 17 nonmedicated, nondemented PD patients suffering from mild to moderate symptoms and 22 healthy controls. The participants were shown pictures of facial expressions depicting disgust, fear, sadness, and anger and they answered scales for the assessment of affective traits. The patients did not report lowered intensities for the displayed target emotions, and showed a comparable rating accuracy as the control participants. The questionnaire scores did not differ between patients and controls. The fMRI data showed similar activation in both groups except for a generally stronger recruitment of somatosensory regions in the patients. CONCLUSIONS Since somatosensory cortices are involved in the simulation of an observed emotion, which constitutes an important mechanism for emotion recognition, future studies should focus on activation changes within this region during the course of disease.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Albert Wabnegger
- Clinical Psychology, University of Graz, BioTechMedGraz, Graz, Austria
| | - Rottraut Ille
- Clinical Psychology, University of Graz, BioTechMedGraz, Graz, Austria
| | | | | | | | - Karoline Wenzel
- Department of Neurology, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria
| | - Anne Schienle
- Clinical Psychology, University of Graz, BioTechMedGraz, Graz, Austria
| |
Collapse
|