1
|
Fang W, Reddy OS, Lai WF. Nutritional awareness of pregnant women and the underlying influencing factors. Nutr Rev 2024; 82:561-569. [PMID: 37460112 DOI: 10.1093/nutrit/nuad074] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/12/2024] Open
Abstract
Nutritional awareness is described as having knowledge or understanding of nutrition. It is often related to the ability of an individual to make an accurate estimate of their food intake, which involves comparing their actual nutritional behavior with the recommended food consumption. Nutritional awareness of women during the various phases of pregnancy may vary significantly across countries due to cultural and lifestyle differences. There has been extensive research on nutritional awareness of pregnant women in selected countries or regions; however, relatively few studies have explored it during different stages of pregnancy. To fill this gap, this article reviews the existing literature and draws together insights into the following areas: changes in nutritional awareness during various phases of pregnancy, nutritional awareness of pregnant women and its underlying factors in various nations, and the research methods used to study nutritional awareness of pregnant women.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Weijie Fang
- Bartlett School of Sustainable Construction Department, University College London, London, UK
| | | | - Wing-Fu Lai
- School of Food Science and Nutrition, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Diehl K, Brassat A, Hilger-Kolb J. Comparative physical activity as a global question to assess physical activity among university students. BMC Sports Sci Med Rehabil 2021; 13:19. [PMID: 33653387 PMCID: PMC7923472 DOI: 10.1186/s13102-021-00247-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/10/2020] [Accepted: 02/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Background To assess physical activity (PA), a comparative measurement – evaluating one’s own PA compared to others – may be an appropriate method. In previous studies, the use of comparative measurements led to an effect known as unrealistic comparative optimism (UCO) – people being unrealistically optimistic about their behavior. Our aim was to use this comparative measurement in university students to quantify the prevalence of UCO at the group level and to draw conclusions on its validity. Methods We used data from the Nutrition and Physical Activity in Adolescence Study (NuPhA), a cross-sectional online survey that included only self-reports (n = 689). To assess PA among students, they were asked to rate their PA level compared to that of their same-aged fellow students. In addition, we used the Godin-Shephard leisure-time PA questionnaire and other questions on PA for comparisons. We used bivariate and cluster-based analyses to identify potential UCO. Results We found that UCO at the group level led to an uneven distribution, with a higher proportion of students who rated themselves as being more physically active than average. However, the individual assessment of PA with a single and simple comparative question seemed to be valid. Discussion A global single comparative question seems useful for studies where PA is measured as a covariate in university students. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s13102-021-00247-7.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katharina Diehl
- Mannheim Institute of Public Health, Social and Preventive Medicine, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Ludolf-Krehl-Straße 7-11, 68167, Mannheim, Germany.
| | - Alessia Brassat
- Mannheim Institute of Public Health, Social and Preventive Medicine, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Ludolf-Krehl-Straße 7-11, 68167, Mannheim, Germany
| | - Jennifer Hilger-Kolb
- Mannheim Institute of Public Health, Social and Preventive Medicine, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Ludolf-Krehl-Straße 7-11, 68167, Mannheim, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Lyles AA, Amresh A, Huberty J, Todd M, Lee RE. A Mobile, Avatar-Based App for Improving Body Perceptions Among Adolescents: A Pilot Test. JMIR Serious Games 2017; 5:e4. [PMID: 28254737 PMCID: PMC5355625 DOI: 10.2196/games.6354] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2016] [Revised: 01/06/2017] [Accepted: 02/03/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND One barrier to effectively treating weight issues among adolescents is that they tend to use social comparison instead of objective measures to evaluate their own health status. When adolescents correctly perceive themselves as overweight, they are more likely to adopt healthy lifestyle behaviors. OBJECTIVE The purpose of this pilot test was to develop and assess acceptability and usability of an avatar-based, theoretically derived mobile app entitled Monitor Your Avatar (MYA). METHODS The MYA app was engineered for high school adolescents to identify, using avatars, what they thought they looked like, what they wanted to look like, and what they actually looked like based on body measurements. RESULTS The MYA app was pilot-tested with male and female adolescents aged 15-18 years to assess for acceptability and usability. A total of 42 students created and viewed their avatars. The majority of the adolescents were female (28/42, 67%), age 16 years (16/42, 38%), white (35/42, 83%), non-Hispanic (36/42, 86%), in grade 10 (20/42, 48%), healthy weight for females (23/28, 82%), and obese for males (7/14, 50%). The adolescents had positive reactions to the avatar app and being able to view avatars that represented them. All but one student (41/42, 98%) indicated some level of comfort viewing the avatars and would use the app in the future to see how their bodies change over time. CONCLUSIONS Avatar-based mobile apps, such as the MYA app, provide immediate feedback and allow users to engage with images that are personalized to represent their perceptions and actual body images. This pilot study adds to the increasing but limited research of using games to improve health outcomes among high school adolescents. There is a need to further adapt the MYA app and gather feedback from a larger number of high school adolescents, including those from diverse backgrounds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Annmarie A Lyles
- College of Nursing and Health Innovation, Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ, United States
| | - Ashish Amresh
- Ira A Fulton School of Engineering, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, United States
| | - Jennifer Huberty
- School of Nutrition and Health Promotion, Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ, United States
| | - Michael Todd
- College of Nursing and Health Innovation, Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ, United States
| | - Rebecca E Lee
- Center for Health Promotion and Disease Prevention, College of Nursing and Health Innovation, Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ, United States
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Sproesser G, Klusmann V, Schupp HT, Renner B. Self-Other Differences in Perceiving Why People Eat What They Eat. Front Psychol 2017; 8:209. [PMID: 28261140 PMCID: PMC5309212 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00209] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2016] [Accepted: 02/01/2017] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
People often view themselves more favorably than others, displaying unrealistic optimism. In the present study, we investigated whether people perceive their reasons for eating as better than those of others. Furthermore, we investigated which mechanisms of inaccuracy might underlie a possible bias when perceiving why people eat what they eat. In Study 1, 117 participants rated the social desirability of eating motives. In Study 2, 772 participants provided information on their own and others’ motives for eating behavior. In Study 1, particularly desirable motives were eating because of hunger, health reasons, and liking. Particularly undesirable motives were eating to make a good impression, to comply with social norms, and to regulate negative affect. Study 2 revealed that for socially desirable motives, participants perceived their own motives to be stronger; for undesirable motives, the opposite pattern emerged, with others being attributed stronger motives. Moreover, the perception of others’ emotional and social motives varied with participants’ own healthy eating behavior. Since the perception of eating motives of others should be independent of one’s own behavior, this pattern of results indicates a relative inaccuracy in the perception of others’ eating motives. In conclusion, there is evidence for unrealistic optimism in eating motives. For social and emotional motives, this self-favoring view seems to be driven by a relatively inaccurate perception of others.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gudrun Sproesser
- Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz Konstanz, Germany
| | - Verena Klusmann
- Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz Konstanz, Germany
| | - Harald T Schupp
- Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz Konstanz, Germany
| | - Britta Renner
- Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz Konstanz, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Sproesser G, Klusmann V, Schupp HT, Renner B. Comparative optimism about healthy eating. Appetite 2015; 90:212-8. [DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.03.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2014] [Revised: 02/27/2015] [Accepted: 03/06/2015] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
6
|
Marques A, Martins J, Ramos M, Yazigi F, Carreiro da Costa F. Perception and reality - Portuguese adults' awareness of active lifestyle. Eur J Sport Sci 2013; 14:468-74. [PMID: 24041272 DOI: 10.1080/17461391.2013.837512] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
This study aims to assess awareness of physical activity levels among adults and to investigate the variables associated with different types of awareness. The participants were 1042 men and 1316 women aged 31-60 years old (43.3 ± 6.1). Data were collected on physical activity behaviour, physical activity awareness, perceptions and psychological factors. Awareness was assessed by comparing self-rated physical activity with achieving physical activity guidelines. Chi-square and logistic regression analyses were applied to the results. About 32.4% were considered active. Moreover, 61% accurately reported their physical activity (38.3% realistic inactive and 22.7% realistic active), 29.2% overestimated their physical activity (overestimators) and 9.7% incorrectly described themselves as inactive (underestimators). Perception of an excellent health status (odds ratio, OR = 4.07, 95% confidence interval, CI: 2.07-8.00, p < 0.001) was the strongest positive association with being realistic active, followed by having a high socio-economic status (SES) (OR = 1.53, 95% CI: 1.10-2.12, p < 0.05). Overestimator participants were more likely to have an excellent perception of health (OR = 2.58, 95% CI: 1.47-4.52, p < 0.01) and had a good experience in physical education (OR = 1.46, 95% CI:1.03-2.08, p < 0.05). Almost half of these participants erroneously perceived themselves as physically active. Gender, body mass index (BMI) and the quality of physical education at school were associated with those who misperceived their physical activity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adilson Marques
- a Interdisciplinary Centre for the Study of Human Performance (CIPER), Faculty of Human Kinetics , University of Lisbon , Lisbon , Portugal
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Szwajcer E, Hiddink GJ, Maas L, Koelen M, van Woerkum C. Nutrition awareness before and throughout different trimesters in pregnancy: a quantitative study among Dutch women. Fam Pract 2012; 29 Suppl 1:i82-i88. [PMID: 22399562 DOI: 10.1093/fampra/cmr107] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To examine the nutrition awareness of women before and during pregnancy in order to provide a greater understanding of the life course perspective (LCP) in relation to nutrition behaviours and pregnancy. METHOD Data were collected in a cross-sectional study with the aid of a face-to-face interview, based on our conceptualization of nutrition awareness and the 'rules of thumb' designed by the Dutch Nutrition Centre. The sample consisted of five groups each of ~100 Dutch nulliparous women: women not trying to conceive a child, women trying to conceive a child and women in their first, second or third trimesters of pregnancy. RESULTS The measurement tool based on our conceptualization of nutrition awareness resulted in a Cronbach's alpha of 0.84. Pregnant women are significantly more aware of their nutrition than women who are not trying to conceive. The scores on nutrition awareness do not differ significantly between the three trimester groups of pregnant women. Women who are trying to conceive do not have a significantly higher nutrition awareness than women who are not trying to conceive. CONCLUSIONS Our conceptualization of nutrition awareness has shown to be fruitful in obtaining a better understanding of behavioural changes in health. The study provided indications in favour of the LCP; pregnancy could indeed be an event in a woman's life that causes increased nutrition awareness. This should be kept in mind when healthy nutrition promotion activities are being developed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E Szwajcer
- Community Care & Youth, Expertise Centre of Health, Social Work and Technology, Saxion University of Applied Sciences, Enschede, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Chock TM. The influence of body mass index, sex, and race on college students' optimistic bias for lifestyle healthfulness. JOURNAL OF NUTRITION EDUCATION AND BEHAVIOR 2011; 43:331-338. [PMID: 21741319 DOI: 10.1016/j.jneb.2010.09.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2009] [Revised: 09/03/2010] [Accepted: 09/27/2010] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To examine the influence of body mass index (BMI), sex, and race on college students' optimistic bias (OB) concerning the healthiness of their own lifestyles relative to the "average college student" and best friends. DESIGN Cross-sectional survey. SETTING Large university campus. PARTICIPANTS College students recruited from first-year classes (1,398 female, 985 male). VARIABLES MEASURED Student height, weight, sex, race, and assessments of lifestyle healthfulness for self, best friends, and the "average college student." ANALYSIS Paired 2-way t tests compared lifestyle ratings for self and others. One-way analyses of variance compared differences between BMI categories. Tukey Honestly Significant Difference tests tested all pairwise comparisons. Analyses of covariance controlling for BMI scores tested sex and race differences (P < .05). RESULTS College students in all BMI categories were optimistically biased in comparisons to the average college student. Optimistic bias differences were significantly smaller, however, for self and close friends. Sex and race also influenced the extent and nature of OB judgments. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS Optimistic bias may limit students' willingness to adopt and maintain more healthful behaviors. Optimistic bias can be reduced or even eliminated if close friends are specified as comparative targets. Health education campaigns, particularly those using socially normative approaches, should consider the impact of comparative target, sex, and race on OB.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- T Makana Chock
- S. I. Newhouse School of Public Communications, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY 13244-2100, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Rossiter M, Glanville T, Taylor J, Blum I. School food practices of prospective teachers. THE JOURNAL OF SCHOOL HEALTH 2007; 77:694-700. [PMID: 18076415 DOI: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2007.00253.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/25/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Schoolteachers can affect students' eating habits in several ways: through nutrition knowledge, positive role modeling, and avoidance of unhealthy classroom food practices. In this study, the knowledge, attitudes, and eating behaviors of prospective teachers as determinants of intended classroom food practices and the school environment and its potential impact on classroom food practices were examined and explored. METHODS One hundred and three students (response rate 79%) enrolled in the final year of a bachelor of education program with at least 22 weeks of practice teaching completed a self-administered questionnaire adapted from the Teens Eating for Energy and Nutrition at School teaching staff survey. Indexes related to classroom food practices, school food environment, personal health, fat intake, and nutrition knowledge were constructed and explored quantitatively using linear modeling techniques and contingency table analysis. RESULTS The majority of respondents reported a high fat intake (65%) and had mid-to-low nutrition knowledge (72%). While most respondents (93%) believed that a healthy school food environment was important, two thirds reported unhealthy classroom food practices. Unhealthy classroom food practices were more likely to be used by those intending to teach at the secondary level, those who held a high personal health belief, and those who demonstrated less support for a healthy school environment. CONCLUSIONS These findings suggest that knowledge, attitudes, and food behaviors of prospective teachers may be barriers to promoting healthy food habits to their future students. Further, prospective teachers would benefit from policies and programs that support healthy classroom practices and from compulsory nutrition education in the teacher training curriculum.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melissa Rossiter
- Department of Family Relations and Applied Nutrition, University of Guelph, 50 Stone Rd E, Guelph, Ontario, Canada N1G 2W1.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Brug J, Wammes B, Kremers S, Giskes K, Oenema A. Underestimation and overestimation of personal weight status: associations with socio-demographic characteristics and weight maintenance intentions. J Hum Nutr Diet 2006; 19:253-62. [PMID: 16911237 DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-277x.2006.00707.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Unwarranted underestimation and overestimation of personal weight status may prevent weight maintenance behaviour. The present study reports on correlates of under- and overestimation of personal weight status and the association with weight maintenance intentions and self-reported action. DESIGN Comparison of three cross-sectional surveys, representing different population groups. SUBJECTS Survey 1: 1694 adolescents 13-19 years of age; survey 2: 979 nonobese adults 25-35 years of age; survey 3: 617 adults 21-62 years of age. MEASUREMENTS Self-administered written questionnaires (surveys 1 and 3) and telephone-administered questionnaires (survey 2); self-reported BMI, self-rated weight status, intentions and self-reported actions to avoid weight gain or to lose weight, sex, age, education and ethnic background. Respondents were classified as people who are realistic about personal body weight status or people who under- and overestimate their body weight status, based on BMI and self-rated weight status. RESULTS Most respondents in the three survey populations were realistic about their weight status. Overestimation of weight status was consistently more likely among women, whereas underestimation was more likely among men, older respondents and respondents from ethnic minorities. Self-rated weight status was a stronger correlate of intentions and self-report actions to avoid weight gain than weight status based on Body Mass Index. CONCLUSIONS Relevant proportions of the study populations underestimated or overestimated their bodyweight status. Overestimation of personal weight status may lead to unwarranted weight maintenance actions, whereas underestimation may result in lack of motivation to avoid further weight gain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Brug
- Department of Public Health, Erasmus University Medical Center, Rotterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Abstract
INTRODUCTION This study examined what methods people use to determine and interpret their body weight, and what factors are associated with either an underestimation of overweight or an overestimation of a healthy body weight. METHOD The study used self-reported data on weight and height. Data were collected by means of questionnaires (n = 722). RESULTS In comparison with unaware overweight individuals, aware overweight respondents had a significantly lower score on comparing their body to that of others and on listening to remarks from others as methods to determine and interpret their body weight. The same was true for respondents with a correct perception of their healthy body weight compared with respondents who overestimated their healthy body weight. Respondents with a correct perception also had a significantly lower score on using the need to change to a different clothing size to determine body weight. Underestimation of overweight was significantly associated with body mass index (BMI), intense physical activity, knowledge of a healthy weight range and body comparison; overestimation of healthy body weight was significantly associated with gender, BMI, weight loss history and media influences. DISCUSSION The study had a cross-sectional design, and therefore no causal relations could be determined. Despite this, the study provided more insight into the way people estimate and judge their body weight.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- I H M Steenhuis
- Institute for Health Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|