1
|
Begum-Ali J, Mason L, Charman T, Johnson MH, Green J, Garg S, Jones EJH. Disrupted visual attention relates to cognitive development in infants with Neurofibromatosis Type 1. J Neurodev Disord 2025; 17:12. [PMID: 40087579 PMCID: PMC11907931 DOI: 10.1186/s11689-025-09599-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/05/2023] [Accepted: 02/13/2025] [Indexed: 03/17/2025] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Neurofibromatosis Type 1 is a genetic condition diagnosed in infancy that substantially increases the likelihood of a child experiencing cognitive and developmental difficulties, including Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Children with NF1 show clear differences in attention, but whether these differences emerge in early development and how they relate to broader difficulties with cognitive and learning skills is unclear. To address this question requires longitudinal prospective studies from infancy, where the relation between domains of visual attention (including exogenous and endogenous shifting) and cognitive development can be mapped over time. METHODS We report data from 28 infants with NF1 tested longitudinally at 5, 10 and 14 months compared to cohorts of 29 typical likelihood infants (with no history of NF1 or ASD and/or ADHD), and 123 infants with a family history of ASD and/or ADHD. We used an eyetracking battery to measure both exogenous and endogenous control of visual attention. RESULTS Infants with NF1 demonstrated intact social orienting, but slower development of endogenous visual foraging. This slower development presented as prolonged engagement with a salient stimulus in a static display relative to typically developing infants. In terms of exogenous attention shifting, NF1 infants showed faster saccadic reaction times than typical likelihood infants. However, the NF1 group demonstrated a slower developmental improvement from 5 to 14 months of age. Individual differences in foraging and saccade times were concurrently related to visual reception abilities within the full infant cohort (NF1, typical likelihood and those with a family history of ASD/ADHD). CONCLUSIONS Our results provide preliminary evidence that alterations in saccadic reaction time and visual foraging may contribute to learning difficulties in infants with NF1.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jannath Begum-Ali
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, School of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Science, Henry Wellcome Building, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London, UK.
| | - Luke Mason
- Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King'S College London, London, UK
| | - Tony Charman
- Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King'S College London, London, UK
| | - Mark H Johnson
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, School of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Science, Henry Wellcome Building, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London, UK
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Jonathan Green
- Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
| | - Shruti Garg
- Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
| | - Emily J H Jones
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, School of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Science, Henry Wellcome Building, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London, UK.
- Centre for Developmental Neurobiology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King'S College London, London, UK.
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King'S College London, London, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Moore SE. Sex differences in growth and neurocognitive development in infancy and early childhood. Proc Nutr Soc 2024; 83:221-228. [PMID: 38326969 DOI: 10.1017/s0029665124000144] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/09/2024]
Abstract
Undernutrition in early life remains a significant public health challenge affecting millions of infants and young children globally. Children who are wasted, stunted or underweight are at increased risk of morbidity and mortality. Undernutrition at critical periods also impacts on aspects of neurodevelopment, with longer-term consequences to educational performance and mental health outcomes. Despite consistent evidence highlighting an increased risk of neonatal and infant mortality among boys, a common assumption held across many disciplines is that girls are more vulnerable with respect to early-life exposures. In relation to undernutrition, however, recent evidence indicates the reverse, and in contexts of food insecurity, boys are at increased risk of undernutrition in early life compared to girls, with sex-specific risks for neurodevelopmental deficits. These effects appear independent of social factors that may favour boys, such as gender disparities in infant feeding practices and health-seeking behaviours. The observed vulnerability among boys may therefore be underpinned by biological processes such as differential energy requirements during periods of rapid growth. As boys have greater needs for growth and maintenance, then, in times of nutritional hardship, these needs may not be met resulting in risk of undernutrition and subsequent health consequences. In view of this emerging evidence, a greater understanding of the mechanisms behind this vulnerability among boys is needed and policy considerations to protect boys should be considered. This review will explore sex differences in risk of undernutrition and consider these in the context of existing programmes and policies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sophie E Moore
- Department of Women and Children's Health, King's College London, LondonSE1 7EH, UK
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Lloyd-Fox S, McCann S, Milosavljevic B, Katus L, Blasi A, Bulgarelli C, Crespo-Llado M, Ghillia G, Fadera T, Mbye E, Mason L, Njai F, Njie O, Perapoch-Amado M, Rozhko M, Sosseh F, Saidykhan M, Touray E, Moore SE, Elwell CE. The Brain Imaging for Global Health (BRIGHT) Project: Longitudinal cohort study protocol. Gates Open Res 2024; 7:126. [PMID: 39372355 PMCID: PMC11452580 DOI: 10.12688/gatesopenres.14795.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/30/2024] [Indexed: 10/08/2024] Open
Abstract
There is a scarcity of prospective longitudinal research targeted at early postnatal life which maps developmental pathways of early-stage processing and brain specialisation in the context of early adversity. Follow up from infancy into the one-five year age range is key, as it constitutes a critical gap between infant and early childhood studies. Availability of portable neuroimaging (functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) and electroencephalography (EEG)) has enabled access to rural settings increasing the diversity of our sampling and broadening developmental research to include previously underrepresented ethnic-racial and geographical groups in low- and middle- income countries (LMICs). The primary objective of the Brain Imaging for Global Health (BRIGHT) project was to establish brain function - using longitudinal data from mother - for-age reference curves infant dyads living in the UK and rural Gambia and investigate the association between context-associated moderators and developmental trajectories across the first two years of life in The Gambia. In total, 265 participating families were seen during pregnancy, at 7-14 days, 1-, 5-, 8-, 12-, 18- and 24-months post-partum. An additional visit is now underway at 3-5 years to assess pre-school outcomes. The majority of our Gambian cohort live in poverty, but while resource-poor in many factors they commonly experience a rich and beneficial family and caregiving context with multigenerational care and a close-knit supportive community. Understanding the impact of different factors at play in such an environment ( i.e., detrimental undernutrition versus beneficial multigenerational family support) will (i) improve the representativeness of models of general cognitive developmental pathways from birth, (ii) identify causal pathways of altered trajectories associated with early adversity at both individual and group level, and (iii) identify the context-associated moderators ( i.e. social context) that protect development despite the presence of poverty-associated challenges. This will in turn contribute to the development of targeted interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah Lloyd-Fox
- Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, UK
- Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, England, UK
| | - Sam McCann
- Women's and Children's Health, Kings College London, London, UK
| | - Bosiljka Milosavljevic
- Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, UK
- Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, Queen Mary University of London, London, England, UK
| | - Laura Katus
- School of Human Sciences, University of Greenwich, London, England, UK
| | - Anna Blasi
- Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London, England, UK
| | - Chiara Bulgarelli
- Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, England, UK
- Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London, England, UK
| | - Maria Crespo-Llado
- Institute of Lifecourse and Medical Sciences, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, England, UK
| | - Giulia Ghillia
- Women's and Children's Health, Kings College London, London, UK
| | - Tijan Fadera
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Ebrima Mbye
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Luke Mason
- Women's and Children's Health, Kings College London, London, UK
| | - Fabakary Njai
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Omar Njie
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | | | - Maria Rozhko
- Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, UK
| | - Fatima Sosseh
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Mariama Saidykhan
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Ebou Touray
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Sophie E. Moore
- Women's and Children's Health, Kings College London, London, UK
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Clare E. Elwell
- Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London, England, UK
| | - The BRIGHT Project team
- Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, UK
- Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, England, UK
- Women's and Children's Health, Kings College London, London, UK
- Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, Queen Mary University of London, London, England, UK
- School of Human Sciences, University of Greenwich, London, England, UK
- Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London, England, UK
- Institute of Lifecourse and Medical Sciences, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, England, UK
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
- Psychology, University of East London, London, England, UK
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Moore SE, McCann S, Jarjou O, Danjo MA, Sonko B, Sise E, Beaton S, Tod D, Fegan G, Prentice AM. Improving infant Neurocognitive Development and Growth Outcomes with micronutrients (INDiGO): A protocol for an efficacy trial in rural Gambia. Wellcome Open Res 2024; 9:377. [PMID: 39411463 PMCID: PMC11474154 DOI: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.21282.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 06/20/2024] [Indexed: 10/19/2024] Open
Abstract
Background Undernutrition during the early years of life has a harmful and irreversible impact on child growth and cognitive development. Many of the interventions tested to improve outcomes across infancy have had disappointing or inconsistent impact, a common feature being the absence of any attempt to provide nutritional supplements to infants during the first six months. With increasing evidence of micronutrient deficiencies in this age group, alongside strong evidence that growth and developmental deficits begin before six months, a renewed focus on the micronutrient status of infants is required. Methods This study is a five-arm, double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomised efficacy trial of micronutrient supplementation to mothers (during pregnancy or pregnancy and lactation) and infants (Day 8 to six months of age) in rural Gambia, where rates of micronutrient deficiencies are high. 600 pregnant women (<20 weeks gestation) will be enrolled into one of five trial arms and followed to 12 months post-partum. The primary outcome will be infant brain development at six months, with micronutrient status, growth and neurocognitive development to 12 months as secondary outcomes. Discussion This novel research will identify the most efficacious way of improving micronutrient status in infancy, and assess impact on infant developmental outcomes, providing an evidence base for future effectiveness trials and policy recommendations. Trial registration ISRCTN registry ( ISRCTN15063705, 09/07/2021); Pan African Clinical Trials Registry ( PACTR202201552774601, 21/01/2022).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sophie E. Moore
- Department of Women and Children’s Health, King's College London, London, England, UK
- MRC Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Fajara, The Gambia
| | - Samantha McCann
- Department of Women and Children’s Health, King's College London, London, England, UK
| | - Ousman Jarjou
- MRC Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Fajara, The Gambia
| | - Muhammed A. Danjo
- MRC Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Fajara, The Gambia
| | - Bakary Sonko
- MRC Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Fajara, The Gambia
| | - Ebrima Sise
- MRC Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Fajara, The Gambia
| | - Samuel Beaton
- Department of Women and Children’s Health, King's College London, London, England, UK
| | - Daniel Tod
- University of Swansea, Swansea Trials Unit, Swansea, UK
| | - Greg Fegan
- University of Swansea, Swansea Trials Unit, Swansea, UK
- Mahidol University, Mahidol Oxford Tropical Medicine Research Unit, Mahidol, Thailand
| | - Andrew M. Prentice
- MRC Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Fajara, The Gambia
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Del Bianco T, Haartsen R, Mason L, Leno VC, Springer C, Potter M, Mackay W, Smit P, Plessis CD, Brink L, Johnson MH, Murphy D, Loth E, Odendaal H, Jones EJH. The importance of decomposing periodic and aperiodic EEG signals for assessment of brain function in a global context. Dev Psychobiol 2024; 66:e22484. [PMID: 38528816 DOI: 10.1002/dev.22484] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/20/2022] [Revised: 01/31/2024] [Accepted: 02/26/2024] [Indexed: 03/27/2024]
Abstract
Measures of early neuro-cognitive development that are suitable for use in low-resource settings are needed to enable studies of the effects of early adversity on the developing brain in a global context. These measures should have high acquisition rates and good face and construct validity. Here, we investigated the feasibility of a naturalistic electroencephalography (EEG) paradigm in a low-resource context during childhood. Additionally, we examined the sensitivity of periodic and aperiodic EEG metrics to social and non-social stimuli. We recorded simultaneous 20-channel EEG and eye-tracking in 72 children aged 4-12 years (45 females) while they watched videos of women singing nursery rhymes and moving toys, selected to represent familiar childhood experiences. These measures were part of a feasibility study that assessed the feasibility and acceptability of a follow-up data collection of the South African Safe Passage Study, which tracks environmental adversity and brain and cognitive development from before birth up until childhood. We examined whether data quantity and quality varied with child characteristics and the sensitivity of varying EEG metrics (canonical band power in the theta and alpha band and periodic and aperiodic features of the power spectra). We found that children who completed the EEG and eye-tracking assessment were, in general, representative of the full cohort. Data quantity was higher in children with greater visual attention to the stimuli. Out of the tested EEG metrics, periodic measures in the theta frequency range were most sensitive to condition differences, compared to alpha range measures and canonical and aperiodic EEG measures. Our results show that measuring EEG during ecologically valid social and non-social stimuli is feasible in low-resource settings, is feasible for most children, and produces robust indices of social brain function. This work provides preliminary support for testing longitudinal links between social brain function, environmental factors, and emerging behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Teresa Del Bianco
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck University of London, London, UK
| | - Rianne Haartsen
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck University of London, London, UK
| | - Luke Mason
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck University of London, London, UK
- Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College, London, London, UK
| | - Virginia Carter Leno
- Department of Biostatistics and Health Informatics, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
| | - Cilla Springer
- Department of Paediatrics and Child Health, Stellenbosch University, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Mandy Potter
- Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Stellenbosch University, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Wendy Mackay
- Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Stellenbosch University, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Petrusa Smit
- Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Stellenbosch University, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Carlie Du Plessis
- Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Stellenbosch University, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Lucy Brink
- Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Stellenbosch University, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Mark H Johnson
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck University of London, London, UK
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Declan Murphy
- Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College, London, London, UK
| | - Eva Loth
- Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College, London, London, UK
| | - Hein Odendaal
- Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Stellenbosch University, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Emily J H Jones
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck University of London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
McCann S, Mason L, Milosavljevic B, Mbye E, Touray E, Colley A, Johnson W, Lloyd-Fox S, Elwell CE, Moore SE. Iron status in early infancy is associated with trajectories of cognitive development up to pre-school age in rural Gambia. PLOS GLOBAL PUBLIC HEALTH 2023; 3:e0002531. [PMID: 37910494 PMCID: PMC10619872 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pgph.0002531] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2023] [Accepted: 10/03/2023] [Indexed: 11/03/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Iron deficiency is among the leading risk factors for poor cognitive development. However, interventions targeting iron deficiency have had mixed results on cognitive outcomes. This may be due to previous interventions focusing on the correction of iron deficiency anaemia in late infancy and early childhood, at which point long lasting neural impacts may already be established. We hypothesise that the relationship between iron status and cognitive development will be observable in the first months of life and will not be recovered by 5 years of age. METHODS Using data from the Brain Imaging for Global Health (BRIGHT) Study in Gambia (n = 179), we conducted mixed effects modelling to assess the relationship between iron status at 5 months of age and trajectories of cognitive development from 5 months- 5 years using (i) a standardised measure of cognitive development (Mullen Scales of Early Learning) and (ii) an eye-tracking assessment of attention processing (visual disengagement time). RESULTS All infants were iron sufficient at 1 month of age. At 5 and 12 months of age 30% and 55% of infants were iron deficient respectively. In fully adjusted analyses, infants in the lowest tercile of soluble transferrin receptor (sTfR) (best iron status) achieved MSEL Cognitive Scores on average 1.9 points higher than infants in the highest sTfR tercile (p = 0.009, effect size = 0.48). There was no evidence that this group difference was recovered by 5 years of age. Infants in the lowest sTfR tercile had visual disengagement time 57ms faster than the highest tercile (p = 0.001, effect size = 0.59). However, this difference diminished by early childhood (p = 0.024). CONCLUSION Infants are at risk of iron deficiency in early infancy. A relationship between iron status and cognitive development is apparent from 5 months of age and remains observable at 5 years of age. One mechanism by which iron availability in early infancy impacts brain development may be through effects on early attentional processing, which is rapidly developing and has substantial nutritional requirements during this period. To support neurocognitive development, prevention of iron deficiency in pre- and early postnatal life may be more effective than correcting iron deficiency once already established.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samantha McCann
- Department of Women and Children’s Health, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Luke Mason
- Institute of Psychology, Psychiatry and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
| | | | - Ebrima Mbye
- Medical Research Council Unit, The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Fajara, The Gambia
| | - Ebou Touray
- Medical Research Council Unit, The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Fajara, The Gambia
| | - Alhassan Colley
- Medical Research Council Unit, The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Fajara, The Gambia
| | - William Johnson
- School of Sport, Exercise and Health Sciences, Loughborough University, Loughborough, United Kingdom
| | - Sarah Lloyd-Fox
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - Clare E. Elwell
- Department of Medical Physics, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Sophie E. Moore
- Department of Women and Children’s Health, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
- Medical Research Council Unit, The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Fajara, The Gambia
| | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Katus L, Blasi A, McCann S, Mason L, Mbye E, Touray E, Ceesay M, de Haan M, Moore SE, Elwell CE, Lloyd-Fox S. Longitudinal fNIRS and EEG metrics of habituation and novelty detection are correlated in 1-18-month-old infants. Neuroimage 2023; 274:120153. [PMID: 37146782 PMCID: PMC10199411 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120153] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/19/2023] [Revised: 04/06/2023] [Accepted: 05/01/2023] [Indexed: 05/07/2023] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Habituation and novelty detection are two fundamental and widely studied neurocognitive processes. Whilst neural responses to repetitive and novel sensory input have been well-documented across a range of neuroimaging modalities, it is not yet fully understood how well these different modalities are able to describe consistent neural response patterns. This is particularly true for infants and young children, as different assessment modalities might show differential sensitivity to underlying neural processes across age. Thus far, many neurodevelopmental studies are limited in either sample size, longitudinal scope or breadth of measures employed, impeding investigations of how well common developmental trends can be captured via different methods. METHOD This study assessed habituation and novelty detection in N = 204 infants using EEG and fNIRS measured in two separate paradigms, but within the same study visit, at 1, 5 and 18 months of age in an infant cohort in rural Gambia. EEG was acquired during an auditory oddball paradigm during which infants were presented with Frequent, Infrequent and Trial Unique sounds. In the fNIRS paradigm, infants were familiarised to a sentence of infant-directed speech, novelty detection was assessed via a change in speaker. Indices for habituation and novelty detection were extracted for both EEG and NIRS RESULTS: We found evidence for weak to medium positive correlations between responses on the fNIRS and the EEG paradigms for indices of both habituation and novelty detection at most age points. Habituation indices correlated across modalities at 1 month and 5 months but not 18 months of age, and novelty responses were significantly correlated at 5 months and 18 months, but not at 1 month. Infants who showed robust habituation responses also showed robust novelty responses across both assessment modalities. DISCUSSION This study is the first to examine concurrent correlations across two neuroimaging modalities across several longitudinal age points. Examining habituation and novelty detection, we show that despite the use of two different testing modalities, stimuli and timescale, it is possible to extract common neural metrics across a wide age range in infants. We suggest that these positive correlations might be strongest at times of greatest developmental change.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Katus
- Institute for Lifecourse Development, School of Human Sciences, University of Greenwich, London, United Kingdom; Centre for Family Research, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom.
| | - Anna Blasi
- Department of Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, United Kingdom
| | - Sam McCann
- Department of Women and Children's Health, Kings College London, United Kingdom
| | - Luke Mason
- Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, King's College London, United Kingdom
| | - Ebrima Mbye
- MRC Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, The Gambia
| | - Ebou Touray
- MRC Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, The Gambia
| | - Muhammed Ceesay
- MRC Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, The Gambia
| | - Michelle de Haan
- Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health, University College London, United Kingdom; Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children NHS Foundation Trust, London, United Kingdom
| | - Sophie E Moore
- Department of Women and Children's Health, Kings College London, United Kingdom; MRC Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, The Gambia
| | - Clare E Elwell
- Department of Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, United Kingdom
| | - Sarah Lloyd-Fox
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Steventon Roberts KJ, Smith C, Toska E, Cluver L, Wittesaele C, Langwenya N, Shenderovich Y, Saal W, Jochim J, Chen‐Charles J, Marlow M, Sherr L. Exploring the cognitive development of children born to adolescent mothers in South Africa. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2023; 32:e2408. [PMID: 38439906 PMCID: PMC10909423 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2408] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/12/2021] [Revised: 01/27/2023] [Accepted: 02/04/2023] [Indexed: 03/11/2023]
Abstract
This study explores the cognitive development of children born to adolescent mothers within South Africa compared to existing reference data, and explores development by child age bands to examine relative levels of development. Cross-sectional analyses present data from 954 adolescents (10-19 years) and their first-born children (0-68 months). All adolescents completed questionnaires relating to themselves and their children, and standardized child cognitive assessments (Mullen Scales of Early Learning) were undertaken. Cognitive development scores of the sample were lower than USA reference population scores and relative performance compared to the reference population was found to decline with increasing child age. When compared to children born to adult mothers in the sub-Saharan African region, children born to adolescent mothers (human immunodeficiency virus [HIV] unexposed; n = 724) were found to have lower cognitive development scores. Findings identify critical periods of development where intervention may be required to bolster outcomes for children born to adolescent mothers. Highlights An exploration of the cognitive development of children born to adolescent mothers within South Africa utilizing the Mullen Scales of Early Learning.Cognitive development scores of children born to adolescent mothers within South Africa were lower compared to USA norm reference data and declined with child age.Previous studies utilizing the Mullen Scales of Early Learning within sub-Saharan Africa were summarized, and comparisons were made with the current sample.Findings highlight a potential risk of developmental delay among children born to adolescent mothers compared to children of adult mothers in the sub-Saharan African region.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathryn J. Steventon Roberts
- Department of Social Policy and InterventionUniversity of OxfordUK
- Institute for Global HealthUniversity College LondonUK
| | - Colette Smith
- Institute for Global HealthUniversity College LondonUK
| | - Elona Toska
- Department of Social Policy and InterventionUniversity of OxfordUK
- Centre for Social Science ResearchUniversity of Cape TownSouth Africa
- Department of SociologyUniversity of Cape TownSouth Africa
| | - Lucie Cluver
- Department of Social Policy and InterventionUniversity of OxfordUK
- Department of Psychiatry and Mental HealthUniversity of Cape TownSouth Africa
| | - Camille Wittesaele
- Department of Social Policy and InterventionUniversity of OxfordUK
- London School of Hygiene and Tropical MedicineUK
| | | | - Yulia Shenderovich
- Department of Social Policy and InterventionUniversity of OxfordUK
- Wolfson Centre for Young People's Mental HealthCardiff UniversityUK
- Centre for the Development and Evaluation of Complex Interventions for Public Health Improvement (DECIPHer), School of Social SciencesCardiff UniversityUK
| | - Wylene Saal
- School of HumanitiesSol Plaatje UniversitySouth Africa
| | - Janina Jochim
- Department of Social Policy and InterventionUniversity of OxfordUK
| | | | - Marguerite Marlow
- Institute of Life Course Health ResearchStellenbosch UniversitySouth Africa
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Sherr L, Haag K, Steventon Roberts KJ, Cluver LD, Wittesaele C, Saliwe B, Tolmay J, Langwenya N, Jochim J, Saal W, Zhou S, Marlow M, Chen-Charles JJ, Toska E. The development of children born to young mothers with no, first- or second-generation HIV acquisition in the Eastern Cape province, South Africa: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 2022; 12:e058340. [PMID: 36229140 PMCID: PMC9562751 DOI: 10.1136/bmjopen-2021-058340] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2021] [Accepted: 10/03/2022] [Indexed: 11/04/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND The intergenerational effects of HIV require long-term investigation. We compared developmental outcomes of different generations impacted by HIV-children of mothers not living with HIV, the 'second generation' (ie, with recently infected mothers) and the 'third generation' (ie, children of perinatally infected mothers). METHODS A cross-sectional community sample of N=1015 young mothers (12-25 years) and their first children (2-68 months, 48.2% female), from South Africa's Eastern Cape Province. 71.3% (n=724) of children were born to mothers not living with HIV; 2.7% (n=27; 1 living with HIV) were third-generation and 26.0% (n=264; 11 living with HIV) second-generation children. Child scores on the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL), the WHO Ten Questions Screen for Disability and maternal demographics were compared between groups using χ2 tests and univariate approach, analysis of variance analysis. Hierarchical linear regressions investigated predictive effects of familial HIV infection patterns on child MSEL composite scores, controlling for demographic and family environment variables. RESULTS Second-generation children performed poorer on gross (M=47.0, SD=13.1) and fine motor functioning (M=41.4, SD=15.2) and the MSEL composite score (M=90.6, SD=23.0) than children with non-infected mothers (gross motor: M=50.4, SD=12.3; fine motor: M=44.4, SD=14.1; composite score: M=94.1, SD=20.7). The third generation performed at similar levels to non-exposed children (gross motor: M=52.4, SD=16.1; fine motor: M=44.3, SD=16.1, composite score: M=94.7, SD=22.2), though analyses were underpowered for definite conclusions. Hierarchical regression analyses suggest marginal predictive effects of being second-generation child compared with having a mother not living with HIV (B=-3.3, 95% CI=-6.8 to 0 .1) on MSEL total scores, and non-significant predictive effects of being a third-generation child (B=1.1, 5% CI=-7.5 to 9.7) when controlling for covariates. No group differences were found for disability rates (26.9% third generation, 27.7% second generation, 26.2% non-exposed; χ2=0.02, p=0.90). CONCLUSION Recently infected mothers and their children may struggle due to the disruptiveness of new HIV diagnoses and incomplete access to care/support during pregnancy and early motherhood. Long-standing familial HIV infection may facilitate care pathways and coping, explaining similar cognitive development among not exposed and third-generation children. Targeted intervention and fast-tracking into services may improve maternal mental health and socioeconomic support.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lorraine Sherr
- Institute for Global Health, University College London, London, UK
| | | | - Kathryn J Steventon Roberts
- Institute for Global Health, University College London, London, UK
- Department of Social Policy & Intervention, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Lucie Dale Cluver
- Department of Social Policy & Intervention, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
- Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, University of Cape Town, Rondebosch, Western Cape, South Africa
| | - Camille Wittesaele
- Department of Social Policy & Intervention, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
- Department of Infectious Disease Epidemiology, London School of Hygiene & Tropical Medicine, London, UK
| | - Bongiwe Saliwe
- Centre for Social Science Research, University of Cape Town, Rondebosch, Western Cape, South Africa
| | - Janke Tolmay
- Centre for Social Science Research, University of Cape Town, Rondebosch, Western Cape, South Africa
| | - Nontokozo Langwenya
- Department of Social Policy & Intervention, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Janina Jochim
- Department of Social Policy & Intervention, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Wylene Saal
- School of Humanities, Sol Plaatje University, Kimberly, South Africa
| | - Siyanai Zhou
- Centre for Social Science Research, University of Cape Town, Rondebosch, Western Cape, South Africa
| | - Marguerite Marlow
- Institute for Life Course Health Research, Department of Global Health, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of Stellenbosch, Stellenbosch, Western Cape, South Africa
| | | | - Elona Toska
- Department of Social Policy & Intervention, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
- Centre for Social Science Research, University of Cape Town, Rondebosch, Western Cape, South Africa
- Department of Sociology, University of Cape Town, Rondebosch, Western Cape, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Waldman-Levi A, Bundy A, Shai D. Cognition Mediates Playfulness Development in Early Childhood: A Longitudinal Study of Typically Developing Children. Am J Occup Ther 2022; 76:23884. [PMID: 35947034 DOI: 10.5014/ajot.2022.049120] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
IMPORTANCE Play, children's central occupation, and playfulness, its behavioral manifestation, are the foci of occupational therapy intervention. However, information about the development of playfulness behavior and the role of cognitive function is limited. OBJECTIVE To explore the development of playfulness and its relation to cognitive functioning from infancy to toddlerhood. DESIGN Longitudinal study with data collected at ages 6 mo, 18 mo, and 24 mo. SETTING Laboratory (age 6 mo) and home (ages 12 and 18 mo). PARTICIPANTS Eighty-six typically developing children drawn from a convenience sample of 109 low-risk families of middle to upper socioeconomic status. MEASURES The Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) to assess cognitive functioning and the Test of Playfulness (ToP) to assess children's playfulness. RESULTS ToP scores were significantly higher at age 24 mo than at age 6 mo, t(88) = -60.30, p < .001, 95% confidence interval (CI) [-1.47, -1.38]. Correlation analysis revealed that the more playful the infant was at age 6 mo, the higher their cognitive functioning was at age 18 mo and the more playful they were at age 24 mo. Toddlers with higher cognitive performance at age 18 mo demonstrated more playful behavior at age 24 mo (β = 0.120, SE = 0.05, 95% CI [0.0377, -0.2276]). CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE Children's playfulness is evident as early as age 6 mo and continues to develop through toddlerhood, depending on their cognitive growth. Occupational therapists play a key role in working with families with young children, promoting cognitive development to further the development of playfulness behaviors. What This Article Adds: Understanding the development of playfulness and exploring its relationship with cognitive functioning in typically developing children fills important gaps in occupational therapy knowledge and contributes to delivery of early intervention, especially when cognition or playfulness are at risk. Our findings confirmed that cognitive functioning contributes to the development of playfulness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amiya Waldman-Levi
- Amiya Waldman-Levi, PhD, OTR/L, is Clinical Associate Professor and Director of Scholarship and Research of Occupational Therapy, Katz School of Science and Health, Yeshiva University, New York, NY; . At the time this research was conducted, Waldman-Levi was Associate Professor, Department of Occupational Therapy, Long Island University, Brooklyn, NY
| | - Anita Bundy
- Anita Bundy, ScD, OT/L, FAOTA, FOTARA, is Professor and Department Head, Occupational Therapy Department, Colorado State University, Fort Collins
| | - Dana Shai
- Dana Shai, PhD, is Senior Lecturer and Founding Director, SEED Center, Academic College of Tel Aviv-Yaffo, Tel Aviv, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Katus L, Milosavljevic B, Rozhko M, McCann S, Mason L, Mbye E, Touray E, Moore SE, Elwell CE, Lloyd-Fox S, de Haan M. Neural Marker of Habituation at 5 Months of Age Associated with Deferred Imitation Performance at 12 Months: A Longitudinal Study in the UK and The Gambia. CHILDREN (BASEL, SWITZERLAND) 2022; 9:988. [PMID: 35883972 PMCID: PMC9320959 DOI: 10.3390/children9070988] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2022] [Revised: 06/28/2022] [Accepted: 06/28/2022] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
Abstract
Across cultures, imitation provides a crucial route to learning during infancy. However, neural predictors which would enable early identification of infants at risk of suboptimal developmental outcomes are still rare. In this paper, we examine associations between ERP markers of habituation and novelty detection measured at 1 and 5 months of infant age in the UK (n = 61) and rural Gambia (n = 214) and infants' responses on a deferred imitation task at 8 and 12 months. In both cohorts, habituation responses at 5 months significantly predicted deferred imitation responses at 12 months of age in both cohorts. Furthermore, ERP habituation responses explained a unique proportion of variance in deferred imitation scores which could not be accounted for by a neurobehavioural measure (Mullen Scales of Early Learning) conducted at 5 months of age. Our findings highlight the potential for ERP markers of habituation and novelty detection measured before 6 months of age to provide insight into later imitation abilities and memory development across diverse settings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Katus
- Centre for Family Research, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3RQ, UK
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK; (B.M.); (S.L.-F.)
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck College, London WC1E 7HX, UK;
| | - Bosiljka Milosavljevic
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK; (B.M.); (S.L.-F.)
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck College, London WC1E 7HX, UK;
| | - Maria Rozhko
- Psychology, School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University (NTU), Singapore 639798, Singapore;
| | - Samantha McCann
- Department of Women and Children’s Health, Kings College London, London SE1 7EH, UK; (S.M.); (S.E.M.)
- Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Keneba P.O. Box 273, The Gambia; (E.M.); (E.T.)
| | - Luke Mason
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck College, London WC1E 7HX, UK;
| | - Ebrima Mbye
- Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Keneba P.O. Box 273, The Gambia; (E.M.); (E.T.)
| | - Ebou Touray
- Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Keneba P.O. Box 273, The Gambia; (E.M.); (E.T.)
| | - Sophie E. Moore
- Department of Women and Children’s Health, Kings College London, London SE1 7EH, UK; (S.M.); (S.E.M.)
- Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Keneba P.O. Box 273, The Gambia; (E.M.); (E.T.)
| | - Clare E. Elwell
- Department of Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK;
| | - Sarah Lloyd-Fox
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK; (B.M.); (S.L.-F.)
| | - Michelle de Haan
- Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health, University College London, London WC1N 1EH, UK;
- Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children NHS Foundation Trust, London WC1N 1EH, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Cheong PL, Tsai JM, Wu YT, Lu L, Chiu YL, Shen YT, Li YJ, Tsao CH, Wang YC, Chang FM, Huang YH, Sun CW. Cultural Adaptation and Validation of Mullen Scales of Early Learning in Taiwanese children with Autism Spectrum Disorder, Global Developmental Delay, and Typically Developing Children. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2022; 122:104158. [PMID: 35032783 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2021.104158] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/21/2021] [Revised: 11/09/2021] [Accepted: 12/23/2021] [Indexed: 05/22/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) is a standardized comprehensive developmental assessment tool for children aged 0-68 months. However, few Asia-based studies have explored cultural and linguistic adaptations of the MSEL or investigated its psychometric properties in populations with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). AIMS This study evaluated the reliability and validity of the MSEL-Taiwan version (MSEL-T) for Taiwanese children with ASD, global developmental delay (GDD), and typical development (TD). METHODS AND PROCEDURES The MSEL items were translated and modified according to the language and culture in Taiwan. In total, 191 children (ASD, 69; GDD, 36; and TD, 86) aged 19-68 months were assessed using the MSEL-T and Peabody Developmental Motor Scales 2 (PDMS-2) at enrollment, followed by the assessments of Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale-Chinese version (VABS-C) at the age of 36 months or later. OUTCOMES AND RESULTS All subscales were verified to have good interrater reliability and internal consistency, and subscale scores indicated moderate to high correlations with PDMS-2 and VABS-C scores. Significant differences in MSEL-T scores were observed between same-aged pairs of children with TD and GDD and between pairs of children with TD and ASD. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS The findings provide evidence of validity and reliability of the MSEL-T. And it is suggested that the culturally and linguistically adapted MSEL-T is a good tool for the clinical assessment of children with and without ASD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pou-Leng Cheong
- Department of Pediatrics, National Taiwan University Hospital Hsin-Chu Hospital, No. 25, Lane 442, Sec. 1, Jingguo Rd., Hsinchu City, 300, Taiwan, ROC; Department of Photonics, College of Electrical and Computer Engineering, National Yang Ming Chiao Tung University, 1001 University Road, Hsinchu, 300, Taiwan, ROC; Department of Biological Science and Technology, National Yang Ming Chiao Tung University, Hsinchu, Taiwan, ROC
| | - Jung-Mei Tsai
- Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, No. 1, Sec. 4, Roosevelt Rd., Taipei, 10617, Taiwan, ROC
| | - Yen-Tzu Wu
- School and Graduate Institute of Physical Therapy, College of Medicine, National Taiwan University, No. 1, Sec. 4, Roosevelt Rd., Taipei, 10617, Taiwan, ROC; Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, National Taiwan University Hospital, No. 1, Changde St., Zhongzheng Dist., Taipei City, 100229, Taiwan, ROC.
| | - Lu Lu
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, National Taiwan University Hospital, No. 1, Changde St., Zhongzheng Dist., Taipei City, 100229, Taiwan, ROC
| | - Yi-Lun Chiu
- Department of Social Work, National Taiwan University, No. 1, Sec. 4, Roosevelt Rd., Taipei, 10617, Taiwan, ROC
| | - Yi-Ting Shen
- Department of Psychology, Kaohsiung Medical University, No. 100, Shih-Chuan 1st Road, Sanmin Dist., Kaohsiung City, 80708, Taiwan, ROC
| | - Yao-Jen Li
- Insititute of Epidemiology and Prevention Medicine, National Taiwan University, No. 1, Sec. 4, Roosevelt Rd., Taipei, 10617, Taiwan, ROC
| | - Chih-Hsuan Tsao
- Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, No. 1, Sec. 4, Roosevelt Rd., Taipei, 10617, Taiwan, ROC; Department of Foreign Languages and Literatures, National Taiwan University, No. 1, Sec. 4, Roosevelt Rd., Taipei, 10617, Taiwan, ROC
| | - Yi-Chung Wang
- School and Graduate Institute of Physical Therapy, College of Medicine, National Taiwan University, No. 1, Sec. 4, Roosevelt Rd., Taipei, 10617, Taiwan, ROC
| | - Fu-Mei Chang
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Songde Branch, Taipei City Hospital, No. 145, Zhengzhou Rd., Datong Dist., Taipei City, 10341, Taiwan, ROC
| | - Yen-Hsun Huang
- Being Alive Clinic, 1F., No. 6-1, Ln. 38, Sec. 2, Nangang Rd., Nangang Dist., Taipei City, 115028, Taiwan, ROC; Department of Clinical Psychology, Fu Jen Catholic University, No. 510, Zhongzheng Rd., Xinzhuang Dist., New Taipei City, 242062, Taiwan, ROC
| | - Chia-Wei Sun
- Department of Photonics, College of Electrical and Computer Engineering, National Yang Ming Chiao Tung University, 1001 University Road, Hsinchu, 300, Taiwan, ROC
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Bach AM, Xie W, Piazzoli L, Jensen SKG, Afreen S, Haque R, Petri WA, Nelson CA. Systemic inflammation during the first year of life is associated with brain functional connectivity and future cognitive outcomes. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2022; 53:101041. [PMID: 34973509 PMCID: PMC8728426 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2021] [Revised: 10/09/2021] [Accepted: 11/27/2021] [Indexed: 01/01/2023] Open
Abstract
The first years of life are a sensitive period of rapid neural and immune system development vulnerable to the impact of adverse experiences. Several studies support inflammation as a consequence of various adversities and an exposure negatively associated with developmental outcomes. The mechanism by which systemic inflammation may affect brain development and later cognitive outcomes remains unclear. In this longitudinal cohort study, we examine the associations between recurrent systemic inflammation, defined as C-reactive protein elevation on ≥ 2 of 4 measurements across the first year of life, electroencephalography (EEG) functional connectivity (FC) at 36 months, and composite cognitive outcomes at 3, 4, and 5 years among 122 children living in a limited-resource setting in Dhaka, Bangladesh. Recurrent systemic inflammation during the first year of life is significantly negatively associated with cognitive outcomes at 3, 4, and 5 years, after accounting for stunting and family care indicators (a measure of stimulation in the home environment). Recurrent systemic inflammation is significantly positively associated with parietal-occipital FC in the Beta band at 36 months, which in turn is significantly negatively associated with composite cognitive scores at 3 and 4 years. However, FC does not mediate the relationship between recurrent systemic inflammation and cognitive outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ashley M Bach
- Labs of Cognitive Neuroscience, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children's Hospital, USA; Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, USA
| | - Wanze Xie
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Peking University, China; PKU-IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking University, China; Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, China
| | - Laura Piazzoli
- Labs of Cognitive Neuroscience, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children's Hospital, USA; Harvard Medical School, USA
| | | | - Sajia Afreen
- International Centre for Diarrhoeal Disease Research, Bangladesh
| | - Rashidul Haque
- International Centre for Diarrhoeal Disease Research, Bangladesh
| | - William A Petri
- Division of Infectious Diseases, University of Virginia School of Medicine, USA
| | - Charles A Nelson
- Labs of Cognitive Neuroscience, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children's Hospital, USA; Harvard Medical School, USA; Harvard Graduate School of Education, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Motor skills, language development, and visual processing in preterm and full-term infants. CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1007/s12144-021-02658-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
|
15
|
Nimkar S, Joshi S, Kinikar A, Valvi C, Devaleenal DB, Thakur K, Bendre M, Khwaja S, Ithape M, Kattagoni K, Paradkar M, Gupte N, Gupta A, Suryavanshi N, Mave V, Dooley KE, Arenivas A. Mullen Scales of Early Learning Adaptation for Assessment of Indian Children and Application to Tuberculous Meningitis. J Trop Pediatr 2021; 67:fmaa034. [PMID: 32620972 PMCID: PMC8496186 DOI: 10.1093/tropej/fmaa034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Tuberculous meningitis (TBM) results in significant morbidity and mortality among children worldwide. Associated neurocognitive complications are common but not well characterized. The Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL), a well-established measure for assessment of neurodevelopment, has not yet been adapted for use in India. This study's goal was to adapt the MSEL for local language and culture to assess neurocognition among children in India, and apply the adapted measure for assessment of children with TBM. METHODS Administration of MSEL domains was culturally adapted. Robust translation procedures for instructions took place for three local languages: Marathi, Hindi and Tamil. Multilingual staff compared instructions against the original version for accuracy. The MSEL stimuli and instructions were reviewed by psychologists and pediatricians in India to identify items concerning for cultural bias. RESULTS MSEL stimuli unfamiliar to children in this setting were identified and modified within Visual Reception, Fine-Motor, Receptive Language and Expressive Language Scales. Item category was maintained for adaptations of items visually or linguistically different from those observed in daily life. Adjusted items were administered to six typically developing children to determine modification utility. Two children diagnosed with confirmed TBM (ages 11 and 29 months) were evaluated with the adapted MSEL before receiving study medications. Skills were below age-expectation across visual reception, fine motor and expressive language domains. CONCLUSIONS This is the first study to assess children with TBM using the MSEL adapted for use in India. Future studies in larger groups of Indian children are warranted to validate the adapted measure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Smita Nimkar
- Clinical Trial Unit, Byramjee Jeejeebhoy Government Medical College, Johns Hopkins University Clinical Research Site, Pune, India
- Department of Health and Biomedical Sciences, Symbiosis International (Deemed) University, Lavale, Pune, India
| | - Suvarna Joshi
- Department of Health and Biomedical Sciences, Symbiosis International (Deemed) University, Lavale, Pune, India
- Department of Pediatrics, Byramjee Jeejeebhoy Government Medical College, Pune, India
| | - Aarti Kinikar
- Department of Pediatrics, Byramjee Jeejeebhoy Government Medical College, Pune, India
| | - Chhaya Valvi
- Department of Pediatrics, Byramjee Jeejeebhoy Government Medical College, Pune, India
| | - D Bella Devaleenal
- Department of Clinical Research, ICMR - National Institute for Research in Tuberculosis, Chennai, India
| | - Kiran Thakur
- Department of Neurology, Columbia University Irving Medical Center, New York, NY, USA
| | - Manjushree Bendre
- Clinical Trial Unit, Byramjee Jeejeebhoy Government Medical College, Johns Hopkins University Clinical Research Site, Pune, India
| | - Saltanat Khwaja
- Clinical Trial Unit, Byramjee Jeejeebhoy Government Medical College, Johns Hopkins University Clinical Research Site, Pune, India
| | - Mahesh Ithape
- Clinical Trial Unit, Byramjee Jeejeebhoy Government Medical College, Johns Hopkins University Clinical Research Site, Pune, India
| | - Krishna Kattagoni
- Department of Clinical Research, ICMR - National Institute for Research in Tuberculosis, Chennai, India
| | - Mandar Paradkar
- Clinical Trial Unit, Byramjee Jeejeebhoy Government Medical College, Johns Hopkins University Clinical Research Site, Pune, India
| | - Nikhil Gupte
- Divisions of Clinical Pharmacology and Infectious Disease, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Amita Gupta
- Divisions of Clinical Pharmacology and Infectious Disease, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Nishi Suryavanshi
- Clinical Trial Unit, Byramjee Jeejeebhoy Government Medical College, Johns Hopkins University Clinical Research Site, Pune, India
| | - Vidya Mave
- Clinical Trial Unit, Byramjee Jeejeebhoy Government Medical College, Johns Hopkins University Clinical Research Site, Pune, India
- Divisions of Clinical Pharmacology and Infectious Disease, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Kelly E Dooley
- Divisions of Clinical Pharmacology and Infectious Disease, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Ana Arenivas
- Department of Rehabilitation Psychology and Neuropsychology, The Institute for Rehabilitation and Research (TIRR) Memorial Hermann, Houston, TX, USA
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Measelle JR, Baldwin DA, Gallant J, Chan K, Green TJ, Wieringa FT, Borath M, Prak S, Hampel D, Shahab-Ferdows S, Allen LH, Kroeun H, Whitfield KC. Thiamine supplementation holds neurocognitive benefits for breastfed infants during the first year of life. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2021; 1498:116-132. [PMID: 34101212 PMCID: PMC9291201 DOI: 10.1111/nyas.14610] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/02/2020] [Revised: 03/01/2021] [Accepted: 04/21/2021] [Indexed: 12/25/2022]
Abstract
Women reliant on mostly rice‐based diets can have inadequate thiamine intake, placing breastfed infants at risk of thiamine deficiency and, in turn, physical and cognitive impairments. We investigated the impact of maternal thiamine supplementation doses on infants’ cognitive, motor, and language development across the first year. In this double‐blind, four‐parallel‐arm, randomized controlled trial, healthy mothers of exclusively breastfed newborn infants were recruited in Kampong Thom, Cambodia. At 2 weeks postnatal, women (n = 335) were randomized to one of four treatment groups to consume one capsule/day with varying amounts of thiamine for 22 weeks: 0, 1.2, 2.4, and 10 mg. At 2, 12, 24, and 52 weeks of age, infants were assessed with the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) and the Caregiver Reported Early Development Instrument (CREDI). Multiple regression and mixed effects modeling suggest that by 6 months of age, the highest maternal thiamine dose (10 mg/day) held significant benefits for infants’ language development, but generally not for motor or visual reception development. Despite having achieved standardized scores on the MSEL that approximated U.S. norms by 6 months, infants showed a significant drop relative to these norms in both language domains following trial completion, indicating that nutritional interventions beyond 6 months may be necessary.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Dare A Baldwin
- Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon
| | - Jelisa Gallant
- Department of Applied Human Nutrition, Mount Saint Vincent University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada
| | - Kathleen Chan
- Department of Applied Human Nutrition, Mount Saint Vincent University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada
| | - Tim J Green
- South Australian Health and Medical Research Institute, Adelaide, South Australia, Australia
| | - Frank T Wieringa
- UMR-204, Institut de recherche pour le développement, Montpellier, France
| | - Mam Borath
- National Sub-Committee for Food Fortification, Cambodia Ministry of Planning, Phnom Penh, Cambodia
| | - Sophonneary Prak
- National Nutrition Programme, Maternal and Child Health Centre, Cambodia Ministry of Health, Phnom Penh, Cambodia
| | - Daniela Hampel
- USDA/ARS Western Human Nutrition Research Center, Davis, California.,Department of Nutrition, University of California, Davis, California
| | - Setareh Shahab-Ferdows
- USDA/ARS Western Human Nutrition Research Center, Davis, California.,Department of Nutrition, University of California, Davis, California
| | - Lindsay H Allen
- USDA/ARS Western Human Nutrition Research Center, Davis, California.,Department of Nutrition, University of California, Davis, California
| | - Hou Kroeun
- Helen Keller International Cambodia, Phnom Penh, Cambodia
| | - Kyly C Whitfield
- Department of Applied Human Nutrition, Mount Saint Vincent University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Stringer EM, Martinez E, Blette B, Toval Ruiz CE, Boivin M, Zepeda O, Stringer JSA, Morales M, Ortiz-Pujols S, Familiar I, Collins M, Chavarria M, Goldman B, Bowman N, de Silva A, Westreich D, Hudgens M, Becker-Dreps S, Bucardo F. Neurodevelopmental Outcomes of Children Following In Utero Exposure to Zika in Nicaragua. Clin Infect Dis 2021; 72:e146-e153. [PMID: 33515459 PMCID: PMC7935385 DOI: 10.1093/cid/ciaa1833] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2020] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Neurodevelopmental outcomes of asymptomatic children exposed to Zika virus (ZIKV) in utero are not well characterized. Methods We prospectively followed 129 newborns without evidence of congenital Zika syndrome (CZS) up to 24 months of age. Participants were classified as ZIKV exposed or ZIKV unexposed. The Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) was administered in the participants’ homes at 6, 12, 15, 18, 21, and 24 months of age by trained psychologists. Sociodemographic data, medical history, and infant anthropometry at birth were collected at each home visit. Our primary outcome was the Mullen Early Learning Composite Score (ECL) at 24 months of age between our 2 exposure groups. Secondary outcomes were differences in MSEL subscales over time and at 24 months. Results Of 129 infants in whom exposure status could be ascertained, 32 (24.8%) met criteria for in utero ZIKV exposure and 97 (75.2%) did not. There were no differences in maternal age, maternal educational attainment, birthweight, or gestational age at birth between the 2 exposure groups. The adjusted means and standard errors (SEs) for the ELC score between the ZIKV-exposed children compared to ZIKV-unexposed children were 91.4 (SE, 3.1) vs 96.8 (SE, 2.4) at 12 months and 93.3 (SE, 2.9) vs 95.9 (SE, 2.3) at 24 months. In a longitudinal mixed model, infants born to mothers with an incident ZIKV infection (P = .01) and low-birthweight infants (<2500 g) (P = .006) had lower composite ECL scores. Conclusions In this prospective cohort of children without CZS, children with in utero ZIKV exposure had lower neurocognitive scores at 24 months.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth M Stringer
- Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
| | - Evelin Martinez
- Department of Microbiology, Faculty of Medical Science, National Autonomous University of Nicaragua at León, Managua, Nicaragua
| | - Bryan Blette
- Department of Biostatistics, Gillings School of Global Public Health, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
| | - Christian Eduardo Toval Ruiz
- Department of Microbiology, Faculty of Medical Science, National Autonomous University of Nicaragua at León, Managua, Nicaragua
| | - Michael Boivin
- Department of Psychiatry, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA
| | - Omar Zepeda
- Department of Microbiology, Faculty of Medical Science, National Autonomous University of Nicaragua at León, Managua, Nicaragua
| | - Jeffrey S A Stringer
- Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
| | - Marlen Morales
- Department of Microbiology, Faculty of Medical Science, National Autonomous University of Nicaragua at León, Managua, Nicaragua
| | - Shiara Ortiz-Pujols
- Division of Endocrinology, New York-Presbyterian Hospital and Weill Cornell Medical Center, New York, New York, USA
| | - Itziar Familiar
- Department of Psychiatry, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA
| | - Matthew Collins
- Department of Medicine, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia, USA
| | - Meylin Chavarria
- Department of Microbiology, Faculty of Medical Science, National Autonomous University of Nicaragua at León, Managua, Nicaragua
| | - Barbara Goldman
- Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
| | - Natalie Bowman
- Department of Medicine, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
| | - Aravinda de Silva
- Department of Microbiology and Immunology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
| | - Daniel Westreich
- Department of Epidemiology, Gillings School of Global Public Health, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
| | - Michael Hudgens
- Department of Biostatistics, Gillings School of Global Public Health, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
| | - Sylvia Becker-Dreps
- Department of Family Medicine, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
| | - Filemon Bucardo
- Department of Microbiology, Faculty of Medical Science, National Autonomous University of Nicaragua at León, Managua, Nicaragua
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Valdes V, Pierce LJ, Lane CJ, Reilly EB, Jensen SKG, Gharib A, Levitt P, Nelson CA, Thompson BL. An exploratory study of predictors of cognition in two low-income samples of infants across the first year of life. PLoS One 2020; 15:e0238507. [PMID: 32881915 PMCID: PMC7470325 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238507] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2020] [Accepted: 08/18/2020] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE In this exploratory longitudinal study we assessed cognitive development in a community sample of infants born into predominantly low-income families from two different urban sites, to identify family and community factors that may associate with outcomes by 1 year of age. METHOD Infant-mother dyads (n = 109) were recruited in Boston and Los Angeles community pediatric practices. Infant cognition was measured using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning when the infant was aged 2, 6, 9, and 12 months. Longitudinal linear mixed effects modeling and linear regression models explored potential predictors of cognitive outcomes. RESULTS Cognitive scores were lower than the reference population mean at both 6 and 12 months. There were site differences in demographics and cognitive performance. Maternal education predicted expressive language in Boston, and speaking Spanish and lower rates of community poverty were associated with greater increases in overall cognition in Los Angeles. CONCLUSION This exploratory study identified a number of drivers of child development that are both shared across cohorts and unique to specific community samples. Factors influencing heterogeneity within and across populations both may be important contributors to prevention and intervention in supporting healthy development among children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Viviane Valdes
- Department of Pediatrics, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Lara J. Pierce
- Department of Pediatrics, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America
- Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Christianne Joy Lane
- Department of Preventive Medicine, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America
| | - Emily B. Reilly
- Department of Pediatrics, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Sarah K. G. Jensen
- Department of Pediatrics, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Alma Gharib
- Department of Pediatrics, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, United States of America
- Viterbi School of Engineering, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America
| | - Pat Levitt
- Department of Pediatrics, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, United States of America
- Program in Developmental Neuroscience and Neurogenetics, The Saban Research Institute, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America
| | - Charles A. Nelson
- Department of Pediatrics, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America
- Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America
- Harvard Graduate School of Education, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Barbara L. Thompson
- Department of Pediatrics and Human Development, College of Human Medicine, Michigan State University, Grand Rapids, Michigan, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Katus L, Mason L, Milosavljevic B, McCann S, Rozhko M, Moore SE, Elwell CE, Lloyd-Fox S, de Haan M. ERP markers are associated with neurodevelopmental outcomes in 1-5 month old infants in rural Africa and the UK. Neuroimage 2020; 210:116591. [PMID: 32007497 PMCID: PMC7068721 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116591] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/28/2019] [Revised: 11/22/2019] [Accepted: 01/27/2020] [Indexed: 02/02/2023] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Infants and children in low- and middle-income countries are frequently exposed to a range of poverty-related risk factors, increasing their likelihood of poor neurodevelopmental outcomes. There is a need for culturally objective markers, which can be used to study infants from birth, thereby enabling early identification and ultimately intervention during a critical time of neurodevelopment. METHOD In this paper, we investigate developmental changes in auditory event related potentials (ERP) associated with habituation and novelty detection in infants between 1 and 5 months living in the United Kingdom and The Gambia, West Africa. Previous research reports that whereas newborns' ERP responses are increased when presented with stimuli of higher intensity, this sensory driven response decreases over the first few months of life, giving rise to a cognitively driven, novelty-based response. Anthropometric measures were obtained concurrently with the ERP measures at 1 and 5 months of age. Neurodevelopmental outcome was measured using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) at 5 months of age. RESULTS The described developmental change was observed in the UK cohort, who exhibited an intensity-based response at 1 month and a novelty-based response at 5 months of age. This change was accompanied by greater habituation to stimulus intensity at 5 compared to 1 month. In the Gambian cohort we did not see a change from an intensity-to a novelty-based response, and no change in habituation to stimulus intensity across the two age points. The degree of change from an intensity towards a novelty-based response was further found to be associated with MSEL scores at 5 months of infant age, whereas infants' growth between 1 and 5 months was not. DISCUSSION Our study highlights the utility of ERP-based markers to study young infants in rural Africa. By implementing a well-established paradigm in a previously understudied population we have demonstrated its use as a culturally objective tool to better understand early learning in diverse settings world-wide. Results offer insight into the neurodevelopmental processes underpinning early neurocognitive development, which may in the future contribute to early identification of infants at heightened risk of adverse neurodevelopmental outcome.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Katus
- Centre for Family Research, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, UK; Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health, University College London, London, UK.
| | - Luke Mason
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck College, London, UK
| | | | - Samantha McCann
- Department of Women and Children's Health, Kings College London, UK
| | - Maria Rozhko
- Department of Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London, UK
| | - Sophie E Moore
- Department of Women and Children's Health, Kings College London, UK; Medical Research Council, The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London, UK
| | - Clare E Elwell
- Department of Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London, UK
| | - Sarah Lloyd-Fox
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck College, London, UK; Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, UK
| | - Michelle de Haan
- Centre for Family Research, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, UK; Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Blasi A, Lloyd-Fox S, Katus L, Elwell CE. fNIRS for Tracking Brain Development in the Context of Global Health Projects. PHOTONICS 2019; 6:89. [PMID: 33490142 PMCID: PMC7745110 DOI: 10.3390/photonics6030089] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/06/2019] [Accepted: 07/30/2019] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Over the past 25 years, functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) has emerged as a valuable tool to study brain function, and it is in younger participants where it has found, arguably, its most successful application. Thanks to its infant-friendly features, the technology has helped shape research in the neurocognitive development field by contributing to our understanding of the neural underpinnings of sensory perception and socio-cognitive skills. Furthermore, it has provided avenues of exploration for markers of compromised brain development. Advances in fNIRS instrumentation and methods have enabled the next step in the evolution of its applications including the investigation of the effects of complex and interacting socio-economic and environmental adversities on brain development. To do this, it is necessary to take fNIRS out of well-resourced research labs (the majority located in high-income countries) to study at-risk populations in resource-poor settings in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs). Here we review the use of this technology in global health studies, we discuss the implementation of fNIRS studies in LMICs with a particular emphasis on the Brain Imaging for Global Health (BRIGHT) project, and we consider its potential in this emerging field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna Blasi
- Department of Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK
| | - Sarah Lloyd-Fox
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, UK
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK
| | - Laura Katus
- Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health, University College London, London WC1N 1EH, UK
| | - Clare E. Elwell
- Department of Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Milosavljevic B, Vellekoop P, Maris H, Halliday D, Drammeh S, Sanyang L, Darboe MK, Elwell C, Moore SE, Lloyd-Fox S. Adaptation of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning for use among infants aged 5- to 24-months in rural Gambia. Dev Sci 2019; 22:e12808. [PMID: 30739382 PMCID: PMC6767903 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12808] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2018] [Revised: 01/21/2019] [Accepted: 02/01/2019] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
Infants in low‐resource settings are at heightened risk for compromised cognitive development due to a multitude of environmental insults in their surroundings. However, the onset of adverse outcomes and trajectory of cognitive development in these settings is not well understood. The aims of the present study were to adapt the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) for use with infants in a rural area of The Gambia, to examine cognitive development in the first 24‐months of life and to assess the association between cognitive performance and physical growth. In Phase 1 of this study, the adapted MSEL was tested on 52 infants aged 9‐ to 24‐months (some of whom were tested longitudinally at two time points). Further optimization and training were undertaken and Phase 2 of the study was conducted, where the original measures were administered to 119 newly recruited infants aged 5‐ to 24‐months. Infant length, weight and head circumference were measured concurrently in both phases. Participants from both phases were split into age categories of 5–9 m (N = 32), 10–14 m (N = 92), 15–19 m (N = 53) and 20–24 m (N = 43) and performance was compared across age groups. From the ages of 10–14 m, Gambian infants obtained lower MSEL scores than US norms. Performance decreased with age and was lowest in the 20–24 m old group. Differential onsets of reduced performance were observed in the individual MSEL domains, with declines in visual perception and motor performance detected as early as at 10–14 months, while reduced language scores became evident after 15–19 months of age. Performance on the MSEL was significantly associated with measures of growth.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Perijne Vellekoop
- The Global Health and Tropical Medicine Training Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Helen Maris
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
| | - Drew Halliday
- Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, Canada
| | - Saikou Drammeh
- Medical Research Council, The Gambia, at The London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London, UK
| | - Lamin Sanyang
- Medical Research Council, The Gambia, at The London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London, UK
| | - Momodou K Darboe
- Medical Research Council, The Gambia, at The London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London, UK
| | - Clare Elwell
- Department of Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London, UK
| | - Sophie E Moore
- Department of Women and Children's Health, King's College London, London, UK
| | - Sarah Lloyd-Fox
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK.,Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| |
Collapse
|