51
|
Han J, Dai Y, Xie L, Li F. Brain responses associated with different hierarchical effects on cues and targets during rule shifting. Biol Psychol 2018; 134:52-63. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.02.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/04/2017] [Revised: 02/14/2018] [Accepted: 02/15/2018] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
52
|
Wolff N, Gussek P, Stock AK, Beste C. Effects of high-dose ethanol intoxication and hangover on cognitive flexibility. Addict Biol 2018; 23:503-514. [PMID: 27790802 DOI: 10.1111/adb.12470] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2016] [Revised: 08/25/2016] [Accepted: 10/09/2016] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
The effects of high-dose ethanol intoxication on cognitive flexibility processes are not well understood, and processes related to hangover after intoxication have remained even more elusive. Similarly, it is unknown in how far the complexity of cognitive flexibility processes is affected by intoxication and hangover effects. We performed a neurophysiological study applying high density electroencephalography (EEG) recording to analyze event-related potentials (ERPs) and perform source localization in a task switching paradigm which varied the complexity of task switching by means of memory demands. The results show that high-dose ethanol intoxication only affects task switching (i.e. cognitive flexibility processes) when memory processes are required to control task switching mechanisms, suggesting that even high doses of ethanol compromise cognitive processes when they are highly demanding. The EEG and source localization data show that these effects unfold by modulating response selection processes in the anterior cingulate cortex. Perceptual and attentional selection processes as well as working memory processes were only unspecifically modulated. In all subprocesses examined, there were no differences between the sober and hangover states, thus suggesting a fast recovery of cognitive flexibility after high-dose ethanol intoxication. We assume that the gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABAergic) system accounts for the observed effects, while they can hardly be explained by the dopaminergic system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicole Wolff
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry; Carl Gustav Carus Faculty of Medicine; Germany
| | - Philipp Gussek
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry; Carl Gustav Carus Faculty of Medicine; Germany
| | - Ann-Kathrin Stock
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry; Carl Gustav Carus Faculty of Medicine; Germany
| | - Christian Beste
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry; Carl Gustav Carus Faculty of Medicine; Germany
- Experimental Neurobiology; National Institute of Mental Health; Czech Republic
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Olfers KJF, Band GPH. Game-based training of flexibility and attention improves task-switch performance: near and far transfer of cognitive training in an EEG study. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2018; 82:186-202. [PMID: 29260316 PMCID: PMC5816121 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-017-0933-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/23/2016] [Accepted: 10/19/2017] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
There is a demand for ways to enhance cognitive flexibility, as it can be a limiting factor for performance in daily life. Video game training has been linked to advantages in cognitive functioning, raising the question if training with video games can promote cognitive flexibility. In the current study, we investigated if game-based computerized cognitive training (GCCT) could enhance cognitive flexibility in a healthy young adult sample (N = 72), as measured by task-switch performance. Three GCCT schedules were contrasted, which targeted: (1) cognitive flexibility and task switching, (2) attention and working memory, or (3) an active control involving basic math games, in twenty 45-min sessions across 4-6 weeks. Performance on an alternating-runs task-switch paradigm during pretest and posttest sessions indicated greater overall reaction time improvements after both flexibility and attention training as compared to control, although not related to local switch cost. Flexibility training enhanced performance in the presence of distractor-related interference. In contrast, attention training was beneficial when low task difficulty undermined sustained selective attention. Furthermore, flexibility training improved response selection as indicated by a larger N2 amplitude after training as compared to control, and more efficient conflict monitoring as indicated by reduced Nc/CRN and larger Pe amplitude after training. These results provide tentative support for the efficacy of GCCT and suggest that an ideal training might include both task switching and attention components, with maximal task diversity both within and between training games.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kerwin J F Olfers
- Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition (LIBC), Leiden, The Netherlands.
- Cognitive Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands.
| | - Guido P H Band
- Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition (LIBC), Leiden, The Netherlands
- Cognitive Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Gema Díaz-Blancat, Juan García-Prieto, Fernando Maestú, Francisco Barceló. Fast Neural Dynamics of Proactive Cognitive Control in a Task-Switching Analogue of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Brain Topogr 2017; 31:407-418. [PMID: 29124546 DOI: 10.1007/s10548-017-0607-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/18/2017] [Accepted: 11/06/2017] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
One common assumption has been that prefrontal executive control is mostly required for target detection (Posner and Petersen in Ann Rev Neurosci 13:25-42, 1990). Alternatively, cognitive control has also been related to anticipatory updating of task-set (contextual) information, a view that highlights proactive control processes. Frontoparietal cortical networks contribute to both proactive control and reactive target detection, although their fast dynamics are still largely unexplored. To examine this, we analyzed rapid magnetoencephalographic (MEG) source activations elicited by task cues and target cards in a task-cueing analogue of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. A single-task (color sorting) condition with equivalent perceptual and motor demands was used as a control. Our results revealed fast, transient and largely switch-specific MEG activations across frontoparietal and cingulo-opercular regions in anticipation of target cards, including (1) early (100-200 ms) cue-locked MEG signals at visual, temporo-parietal and prefrontal cortices of the right hemisphere (i.e., calcarine sulcus, precuneus, inferior frontal gyrus, anterior insula and supramarginal gyrus); and (2) later cue-locked MEG signals at the right anterior and posterior insula (200-300 ms) and the left temporo-parietal junction (300-500 ms). In all cases larger MEG signal intensity was observed in switch relative to repeat cueing conditions. Finally, behavioral restart costs and test scores of working memory capacity (forward digit span) correlated with cue-locked MEG activations at key nodes of the frontoparietal network. Together, our findings suggest that proactive cognitive control of task rule updating can be fast and transiently implemented within less than a second and in anticipation of target detection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gema Díaz-Blancat
- Laboratory of Cognitive and Computational Neuroscience (UCM-UPM), Centre for Biomedical Technology, Universidad Politécnica de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | - Juan García-Prieto
- Laboratory of Cognitive and Computational Neuroscience (UCM-UPM), Centre for Biomedical Technology, Universidad Politécnica de Madrid, Madrid, Spain.,Laboratory of Electrical Engineering and Bioengineering, Department of Industrial Engineering, Universidad de La Laguna, Tenerife, Spain
| | - Fernando Maestú
- Laboratory of Cognitive and Computational Neuroscience (UCM-UPM), Centre for Biomedical Technology, Universidad Politécnica de Madrid, Madrid, Spain.,Department of Basic Psychology II, Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | - Francisco Barceló
- Laboratory of Neuropsychology, University of the Balearic Islands, 07122, Palma de Mallorca, Spain.
| |
Collapse
|
55
|
Schmitt H, Kray J, Ferdinand NK. Does the Effort of Processing Potential Incentives Influence the Adaption of Context Updating in Older Adults? Front Psychol 2017; 8:1969. [PMID: 29170649 PMCID: PMC5684189 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01969] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2017] [Accepted: 10/26/2017] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
A number of aging studies suggest that older adults process positive and negative information differently. For instance, the socioemotional selectivity theory postulates that older adults preferably process positive information in service of emotional well-being (Reed and Carstensen, 2012). Moreover, recent research has started to investigate whether incentives like gains or losses can influence cognitive control in an ongoing task. In an earlier study (Schmitt et al., 2015), we examined whether incentive cues, indicating potential monetary gains, losses, or neutral outcomes for good performance in the following trial, would influence older adults’ ability to exert cognitive control. Cognitive control was measured in an AX-Continuous-Performance-Task (AX-CPT) in which participants had to select their responses to probe stimuli depending on a preceding context cue. In this study, we did not find support for a positivity effect in older adults, but both gains and losses led to enhanced context processing. As the trial-wise presentation mode may be too demanding on cognitive resources for such a bias to occur, the main goal of the present study was to examine whether motivational mindsets, induced by block-wise presentation of incentives, would result in a positivity effect. For this reason, we examined 17 older participants (65–76 years) in the AX-CPT using a block-wise presentation of incentive cues and compared them to 18 older adults (69–78 years) with the trial-wise presentation mode from our earlier study (Schmitt et al., 2015). Event-related potentials were recorded to the onset of the motivational cue and during the AX-CPT. Our results show that (a) older adults initially process cues signaling potential losses more strongly, but later during the AX-CPT invest more cognitive resources in preparatory processes like context updating in conditions with potential gains, and (b) block-wise and trial-wise presentation of incentive cues differentially influenced cognitive control. When incentives were presented block-wise, the above described valence effects were consistently found. In contrast, when incentives were presented trial-wise, the effects were mixed and salience as well as valence effects can be obtained. Hence, how positive and negative incentive cues influence cognitive control in older adults is dependent on demands of cue processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hannah Schmitt
- Department of Psychology, Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany
| | - Jutta Kray
- Department of Psychology, Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
56
|
Wolff N, Zink N, Stock AK, Beste C. On the relevance of the alpha frequency oscillation's small-world network architecture for cognitive flexibility. Sci Rep 2017; 7:13910. [PMID: 29066804 PMCID: PMC5654836 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-14490-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/28/2017] [Accepted: 10/11/2017] [Indexed: 01/21/2023] Open
Abstract
Cognitive flexibility is a major requirement for successful behavior. nNeural oscillations in the alpha frequency band were repeatedly associated with cognitive flexibility in task-switching paradigms. Alpha frequencies are modulated by working memory load and are used to process information during task switching, however we do not know how this oscillatory network communication is modulated. In order to understand the mechanisms that drive cognitive flexibility, ERPs, oscillatory power and how the communication within these networks is organized are of importance. The EEG data show that during phases reflecting preparatory processes to pre-activate task sets, alpha oscillatory power but not the small world properties of the alpha network architecture was modulated. During the switching only the N2 ERP component showed clear modulations. After the response, alpha oscillatory power reinstates and therefore seems to be important to deactivate or maintain the previous task set. For these reactive control processes the network architecture in terms of small-world properties is modulated. Effects of memory load on small-world aspects were seen in repetition trials, where small-world properties were higher when memory processes were relevant. These results suggest that the alpha oscillatory network becomes more small-world-like when reactive control processes during task switching are less complex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicole Wolff
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, TU, Dresden, Germany
| | - Nicolas Zink
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, TU, Dresden, Germany
| | - Ann-Kathrin Stock
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, TU, Dresden, Germany
| | - Christian Beste
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, TU, Dresden, Germany. .,Experimental Neurobiology, National Institute of Mental Health, Klecany, Czech Republic.
| |
Collapse
|
57
|
Specific neurophysiological mechanisms underlie cognitive inflexibility in inflammatory bowel disease. Sci Rep 2017; 7:13943. [PMID: 29066846 PMCID: PMC5655331 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-14345-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/09/2017] [Accepted: 10/09/2017] [Indexed: 12/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Inflammatory bowel disease (IBD) is highly prevalent. While the pathophysiological mechanisms of IBD are increasingly understood, there is a lack of knowledge concerning cognitive dysfunctions in IBD. This is all the more the case concerning the underlying neurophysiological mechanisms. In the current study we focus on possible dysfunctions of cognitive flexibility (task switching) processes in IBD patients using a system neurophysiological approach combining event-related potential (ERP) recordings with source localization analyses. We show that there are task switching deficits (i.e. increased switch costs) in IBD patients. The neurophysiological data show that even though the pathophysiology of IBD is diverse and wide-spread, only specific cognitive subprocesses are altered: There was a selective dysfunction at the response selection level (N2 ERP) associated with functional alterations in the anterior cingulate cortex and the right inferior frontal gyrus. Attentional selection processes (N1 ERP), perceptual categorization processes (P1 ERP), or mechanisms related to the flexible implementation of task sets and related working memory processes (P3 ERP) do not contribute to cognitive inflexibility in IBD patients and were unchanged. It seems that pathophysiological processes in IBD strongly compromise cognitive-neurophysiological subprocesses related to fronto-striatal networks. These circuits may become overstrained in IBD when cognitive flexibility is required.
Collapse
|
58
|
Gratton G, Cooper P, Fabiani M, Carter CS, Karayanidis F. Dynamics of cognitive control: Theoretical bases, paradigms, and a view for the future. Psychophysiology 2017; 55. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.13016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 105] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2017] [Revised: 09/01/2017] [Accepted: 09/06/2017] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Gabriele Gratton
- Department of Psychology and Beckman InstituteUniversity of Illinois at Urbana‐ChampaignUrbana Illinois USA
| | - Patrick Cooper
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NewcastleNewcastle New South Wales Australia
| | - Monica Fabiani
- Department of Psychology and Beckman InstituteUniversity of Illinois at Urbana‐ChampaignUrbana Illinois USA
| | - Cameron S. Carter
- Departments of Psychiatry and PsychologyUniversity of California–DavisDavis California USA
| | - Frini Karayanidis
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NewcastleNewcastle New South Wales Australia
| |
Collapse
|
59
|
Wolff N, Mückschel M, Ziemssen T, Beste C. The role of phasic norepinephrine modulations during task switching: evidence for specific effects in parietal areas. Brain Struct Funct 2017; 223:925-940. [DOI: 10.1007/s00429-017-1531-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2017] [Accepted: 09/29/2017] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
|
60
|
Steinhauser M, Maier ME, Ernst B. Neural correlates of reconfiguration failure reveal the time course of task-set reconfiguration. Neuropsychologia 2017; 106:100-111. [PMID: 28939202 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.09.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/28/2017] [Revised: 08/01/2017] [Accepted: 09/18/2017] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
The ability to actively prepare for new tasks is crucial for achieving goal-directed behavior. The task-switching paradigm is frequently used to investigate this task-set reconfiguration. In the present study, we adopted a novel approach to identify a neural signature of reconfiguration in event-related potentials. Our method was to isolate neural correlates of reconfiguration failure and to use these correlates to reveal the time course of reconfiguration in task switches and task repetitions. We employed a task-switching paradigm in which two types of errors could be distinguished: task errors (the incorrect task was applied) and response errors (an incorrect response for the correct task was provided). Because differential activity between both error types distinguishes successful and failed reconfiguration, this activity could be used as a neural signature of the reconfiguration process. We found that, whereas reconfiguration takes place on task repetitions and task switches, it occurred earlier in the former than in the latter. Single-trial analysis revealed that the same activity predicted the amplitude of error-related brain activity, providing further support that this preparatory activity reflects reconfiguration. Our results implicate that reconfiguration is not switch-specific but that task switches and task repetitions differ with respect to the time course of reconfiguration. Furthermore, this study demonstrates that considering neural correlates of failure is a promising approach to link cognitive mechanisms to specific neural processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marco Steinhauser
- Department of Psychology, Catholic University of Eichstätt-Ingolstadt, Germany.
| | - Martin E Maier
- Department of Psychology, Catholic University of Eichstätt-Ingolstadt, Germany
| | - Benjamin Ernst
- Department of Psychology, Catholic University of Eichstätt-Ingolstadt, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
61
|
Provost A, Jamadar S, Heathcote A, Brown SD, Karayanidis F. Intertrial RT variability affects level of target‐related interference in cued task switching. Psychophysiology 2017; 55. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12971] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2016] [Revised: 06/27/2017] [Accepted: 06/29/2017] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Alexander Provost
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NewcastleCallaghan New South Wales Australia
- Priority Research Centre for Brain and Mental Health, University of NewcastleCallaghan New South Wales Australia
| | - Sharna Jamadar
- Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for Integrative Brain FunctionCanberra Australian Capital Territory Australia
- Monash Biomedical Imaging, Monash UniversityMelbourne Victoria Australia
- Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Psychological Science, Monash UniversityMelbourne Victoria Australia
| | - Andrew Heathcote
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NewcastleCallaghan New South Wales Australia
- Division of Psychology, School of MedicineUniversity of TasmaniaHobart Tasmania Australia
| | - Scott D. Brown
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NewcastleCallaghan New South Wales Australia
- Priority Research Centre for Brain and Mental Health, University of NewcastleCallaghan New South Wales Australia
| | - Frini Karayanidis
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NewcastleCallaghan New South Wales Australia
- Priority Research Centre for Brain and Mental Health, University of NewcastleCallaghan New South Wales Australia
- Priority Research Centre for Stroke and Brain Injury, University of NewcastleCallaghan New South Wales Australia
| |
Collapse
|
62
|
Wolff N, Buse J, Tost J, Roessner V, Beste C. Modulations of cognitive flexibility in obsessive compulsive disorder reflect dysfunctions of perceptual categorization. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2017; 58:939-949. [PMID: 28452405 DOI: 10.1111/jcpp.12733] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/23/2017] [Indexed: 01/06/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Despite cognitive inflexibility is trait like in obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) patients and underlies clinical symptomatology, it is elusive at what stage of information processing deficits, leading to cognitive inflexibility, emerges. We hypothesize that inhibitory control mechanisms during early stimulus categorization and integration into a knowledge system underlie these deficits. METHODS We examined N = 25 adolescent OCD patients and matched healthy controls (HC) in a paradigm manipulating the importance of the knowledge system to perform task switching. This was done using a paradigm in which task switches were signaled either by visual stimuli or by working memory processes. This was combined with event-related potential recordings and source localization. RESULTS Obsessive compulsive disorder patients showed increased switch costs in the memory as compared with the cue-based block, while HC showed similar switch costs in both blocks. At the neurophysiological level, these changes in OCD were not reflected by the N2 and P3 reflecting response-associated processes but by the P1 reflecting inhibitory control during sensory categorization processes. Activation differences in the right inferior frontal gyrus and superior temporal gyrus are associated with the P1 effect. CONCLUSIONS Cognitive flexibility in adolescent OCD patients is strongly modulated by working memory load. Contrary to common sense, not response-associated processes, but inhibitory control mechanisms during early stimulus categorization processes are likely to underlie cognitive inflexibility in OCD. These processes are associated with right inferior frontal and superior temporal gyrus mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicole Wolff
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine of the TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
| | - Judith Buse
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine of the TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
| | - Jadwiga Tost
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine of the TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
| | - Veit Roessner
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine of the TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
| | - Christian Beste
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine of the TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany.,Experimental Neurobiology, National Institute of Mental Health, Klecany, Czech Republic
| |
Collapse
|
63
|
Behavioral and electrophysiological correlates of cognitive control in ex-obese adults. Biol Psychol 2017; 127:198-208. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.05.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2016] [Revised: 05/18/2017] [Accepted: 05/23/2017] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
|
64
|
Wolff N, Mückschel M, Beste C. Neural mechanisms and functional neuroanatomical networks during memory and cue-based task switching as revealed by residue iteration decomposition (RIDE) based source localization. Brain Struct Funct 2017; 222:3819-3831. [DOI: 10.1007/s00429-017-1437-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2016] [Accepted: 04/28/2017] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
|
65
|
Baniqued PL, Low KA, Fletcher MA, Gratton G, Fabiani M. Shedding light on gray(ing) areas: Connectivity and task switching dynamics in aging. Psychophysiology 2017; 55. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12818] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2016] [Accepted: 11/02/2016] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Pauline L. Baniqued
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute; University of California; Berkeley, Berkeley California
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology; University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; Urbana Illinois
| | - Kathy A. Low
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology; University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; Urbana Illinois
| | - Mark A. Fletcher
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology; University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; Urbana Illinois
| | - Gabriele Gratton
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology; University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; Urbana Illinois
| | - Monica Fabiani
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology; University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; Urbana Illinois
| |
Collapse
|
66
|
Barceló F, Cooper PS. An information theory account of late frontoparietal ERP positivities in cognitive control. Psychophysiology 2017; 55. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12814] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2015] [Accepted: 11/23/2016] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Francisco Barceló
- Laboratory of Neuropsychology; University of the Balearic Islands; Mallorca Spain
| | - Patrick S. Cooper
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory, School of Psychology; University of Newcastle; Callaghan Australia
- Priority Research Centre for Brain and Mental Health; University of Newcastle; Callaghan Australia
| |
Collapse
|
67
|
Sokka L, Leinikka M, Korpela J, Henelius A, Lukander J, Pakarinen S, Alho K, Huotilainen M. Shifting of attentional set is inadequate in severe burnout: Evidence from an event-related potential study. Int J Psychophysiol 2016; 112:70-79. [PMID: 27988179 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.12.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2016] [Revised: 11/14/2016] [Accepted: 12/12/2016] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
Individuals with prolonged occupational stress often report difficulties in concentration. Work tasks often require the ability to switch back and forth between different contexts. Here, we studied the association between job burnout and task switching by recording event-related potentials (ERPs) time-locked to stimulus onset during a task with simultaneous cue-target presentation and unpredictable switches in the task. Participants were currently working people with severe, mild, or no burnout symptoms. In all groups, task performance was substantially slower immediately after task switch than during task repetition. However, the error rates were higher in the severe burnout group than in the mild burnout and control groups. Electrophysiological data revealed an increased parietal P3 response for the switch trials relative to repetition trials. Notably, the response was smaller in amplitude in the severe burnout group than in the other groups. The results suggest that severe burnout is associated with inadequate processing when rapid shifting of attention between tasks is required resulting in less accurate performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Sokka
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, P.O. Box 40, 00251 Helsinki, Finland.
| | - Marianne Leinikka
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, P.O. Box 40, 00251 Helsinki, Finland
| | - Jussi Korpela
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, P.O. Box 40, 00251 Helsinki, Finland
| | - Andreas Henelius
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, P.O. Box 40, 00251 Helsinki, Finland
| | - Jani Lukander
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, P.O. Box 40, 00251 Helsinki, Finland
| | - Satu Pakarinen
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, P.O. Box 40, 00251 Helsinki, Finland
| | - Kimmo Alho
- Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Siltavuorenpenger 1-5, P.O. Box 9, FI-00014 University of Helsinki, Finland
| | - Minna Huotilainen
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, P.O. Box 40, 00251 Helsinki, Finland; CICERO Learning Network, Faculty of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Siltavuorenpenger 1-5, P.O. Box 9, FI-00014 University of Helsinki, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
68
|
Richter FR, Yeung N. ERP Correlates of Encoding Success and Encoding Selectivity in Attention Switching. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0167396. [PMID: 27907075 PMCID: PMC5131936 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0167396] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2016] [Accepted: 11/14/2016] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Long-term memory encoding depends critically on effective processing of incoming information. The degree to which participants engage in effective encoding can be indexed in electroencephalographic (EEG) data by studying event-related potential (ERP) subsequent memory effects. The current study investigated ERP correlates of memory success operationalised with two different measures—memory selectivity and global memory—to assess whether previously observed ERP subsequent memory effects reflect focused encoding of task-relevant information (memory selectivity), general encoding success (global memory), or both. Building on previous work, the present study combined an attention switching paradigm—in which participants were presented with compound object-word stimuli and switched between attending to the object or the word across trials—with a later recognition memory test for those stimuli, while recording their EEG. Our results provided clear evidence that subsequent memory effects resulted from selective attentional focusing and effective top-down control (memory selectivity) in contrast to more general encoding success effects (global memory). Further analyses addressed the question of whether successful encoding depended on similar control mechanisms to those involved in attention switching. Interestingly, differences in the ERP correlates of attention switching and successful encoding, particularly during the poststimulus period, indicated that variability in encoding success occurred independently of prestimulus demands for top-down cognitive control. These results suggest that while effects of selective attention and selective encoding co-occur behaviourally their ERP correlates are at least partly dissociable.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Franziska R. Richter
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
- * E-mail:
| | - Nick Yeung
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
69
|
Elchlepp H, Verbruggen F. How to withhold or replace a prepotent response: An analysis of the underlying control processes and their temporal dynamics. Biol Psychol 2016; 123:250-268. [PMID: 27756580 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.10.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2016] [Revised: 09/27/2016] [Accepted: 10/10/2016] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
The present study isolated and compared ERP components associated with flexible behavior in two action-control tasks. The 'withhold' groups had to withhold all responses when a signal appeared. The 'change' groups had to replace a prepotent go response with a different response on signal trials. We proposed that the same chain of processes determined the effectiveness of action control in both tasks. Consistent with this idea, lateral (Experiment 1) and central (Experiment 2) signal presentation elicited the same perceptual and response-related components in both tasks with similar latencies. Thus, completely withholding a response and replacing a response required a similar chain of processes. Furthermore, latency analyses revealed intra-individual differences: When the signal occurred in the periphery, differences between fast and slow change trials arose at early perceptual stages; by contrast, differences arose at later processing stages when signal detection was easy but stimulus discrimination and response selection were harder.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H Elchlepp
- University of Exeter, School of Psychology, Exeter EX4 4QG, UK.
| | - F Verbruggen
- University of Exeter, School of Psychology, Exeter EX4 4QG, UK; Ghent University, Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
70
|
Electrophysiological correlates of the cognitive control processes underpinning mixing and switching costs. Brain Res 2016; 1646:160-173. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.05.048] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2015] [Revised: 05/17/2016] [Accepted: 05/25/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
71
|
Behavioral and neurophysiological evidence for increased cognitive flexibility in late childhood. Sci Rep 2016; 6:28954. [PMID: 27349808 PMCID: PMC4923946 DOI: 10.1038/srep28954] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2015] [Accepted: 06/07/2016] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Executive functions, like the capacity to control and organize thoughts and behavior, develop from childhood to young adulthood. Although task switching and working memory processes are known to undergo strong developmental changes from childhood to adulthood, it is currently unknown how task switching processes are modulated between childhood and adulthood given that working memory processes are central to task switching. The aim of the current study is therefore to examine this question using a combined cue- and memory-based task switching paradigm in children (N = 25) and young adults (N = 25) in combination with neurophysiological (EEG) methods. We obtained an unexpected paradoxical effect suggesting that memory-based task switching is better in late childhood than in young adulthood. No group differences were observed in cue-based task switching. The neurophysiological data suggest that this effect is not due to altered attentional selection (P1, N1) or processes related to the updating, organization, and implementation of the new task-set (P3). Instead, alterations were found in the resolution of task-set conflict and the selection of an appropriate response (N2) when a task has to be switched. Our observation contrasts findings showing that cognitive control mechanisms reach their optimal functioning in early adulthood.
Collapse
|
72
|
Cooper PS, Darriba Á, Karayanidis F, Barceló F. Contextually sensitive power changes across multiple frequency bands underpin cognitive control. Neuroimage 2016; 132:499-511. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/18/2015] [Revised: 02/02/2016] [Accepted: 03/04/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022] Open
|
73
|
Elchlepp H, Lavric A, Chambers CD, Verbruggen F. Proactive inhibitory control: A general biasing account. Cogn Psychol 2016; 86:27-61. [PMID: 26859519 PMCID: PMC4825542 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.01.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/02/2014] [Revised: 01/21/2016] [Accepted: 01/21/2016] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
Flexible behavior requires a control system that can inhibit actions in response to changes in the environment. Recent studies suggest that people proactively adjust response parameters in anticipation of a stop signal. In three experiments, we tested the hypothesis that proactive inhibitory control involves adjusting both attentional and response settings, and we explored the relationship with other forms of proactive and anticipatory control. Subjects responded to the color of a stimulus. On some trials, an extra signal occurred. The response to this signal depended on the task context subjects were in: in the 'ignore' context, they ignored it; in the 'stop' context, they had to withhold their response; and in the 'double-response' context, they had to execute a secondary response. An analysis of event-related brain potentials for no-signal trials in the stop context revealed that proactive inhibitory control works by biasing the settings of lower-level systems that are involved in stimulus detection, action selection, and action execution. Furthermore, subjects made similar adjustments in the double-response and stop-signal contexts, indicating an overlap between various forms of proactive action control. The results of Experiment 1 also suggest an overlap between proactive inhibitory control and preparatory control in task-switching studies: both require reconfiguration of task-set parameters to bias or alter subordinate processes. We conclude that much of the top-down control in response inhibition tasks takes place before the inhibition signal is presented.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Heike Elchlepp
- Psychology, College for Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, UK.
| | - Aureliu Lavric
- Psychology, College for Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, UK
| | - Christopher D Chambers
- School of Psychology and Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Centre, Cardiff University, UK
| | - Frederick Verbruggen
- Psychology, College for Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
74
|
Sikora K, Roelofs A, Hermans D. Electrophysiology of executive control in spoken noun-phrase production: Dynamics of updating, inhibiting, and shifting. Neuropsychologia 2016; 84:44-53. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2015] [Revised: 12/22/2015] [Accepted: 01/29/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
75
|
Wang M, Yang P, Zhao QJ, Wang M, Jin Z, Li L. Differential Preparation Intervals Modulate Repetition Processes in Task Switching: An ERP Study. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:57. [PMID: 26924974 PMCID: PMC4757667 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00057] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/07/2015] [Accepted: 02/05/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In task-switching paradigms, reaction times (RTs) switch cost (SC) and the neural correlates underlying the SC are affected by different preparation intervals. However, little is known about the effect of the preparation interval on the repetition processes in task-switching. To examine this effect we utilized a cued task-switching paradigm with long sequences of repeated trials. Response-stimulus intervals (RSI) and cue-stimulus intervals (CSI) were manipulated in short and long conditions. Electroencephalography (EEG) and behavioral data were recorded. We found that with increasing repetitions, RTs were faster in the short CSI conditions, while P3 amplitudes decreased in the LS (long RSI and short CSI) conditions. Positive correlations between RT benefit and P3 activation decrease (repeat 1 − repeat 5), and between the slope of the RT and P3 regression lines were observed only in the LS condition. Our findings suggest that differential preparation intervals modulate repetition processes in task switching.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Min Wang
- Key Laboratory for NeuroInformation of Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China Chengdu, China
| | - Ping Yang
- Key Laboratory for NeuroInformation of Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China Chengdu, China
| | - Qian-Jing Zhao
- Key Laboratory for NeuroInformation of Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China Chengdu, China
| | - Meng Wang
- Key Laboratory for NeuroInformation of Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China Chengdu, China
| | - Zhenlan Jin
- Key Laboratory for NeuroInformation of Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China Chengdu, China
| | - Ling Li
- Key Laboratory for NeuroInformation of Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China Chengdu, China
| |
Collapse
|
76
|
The system neurophysiological basis of backward inhibition. Brain Struct Funct 2016; 221:4575-4587. [DOI: 10.1007/s00429-016-1186-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2015] [Accepted: 01/11/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
77
|
Blanco-Elorrieta E, Pylkkänen L. Bilingual Language Control in Perception versus Action: MEG Reveals Comprehension Control Mechanisms in Anterior Cingulate Cortex and Domain-General Control of Production in Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex. J Neurosci 2016; 36:290-301. [PMID: 26758823 PMCID: PMC6602022 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2597-15.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 83] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2015] [Revised: 11/05/2015] [Accepted: 11/13/2015] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
For multilingual individuals, adaptive goal-directed behavior as enabled by cognitive control includes the management of two or more languages. This work used magnetoencephalography (MEG) to investigate the degree of neural overlap between language control and domain-general cognitive control both in action and perception. Highly proficient Arabic-English bilingual individuals participated in maximally parallel language-switching tasks in production and comprehension as well as in analogous tasks in which, instead of the used language, the semantic category of the comprehended/produced word changed. Our results indicated a clear dissociation of language control mechanisms in production versus comprehension. Language-switching in production recruited dorsolateral prefrontal regions bilaterally and, importantly, these regions were similarly recruited by category-switching. Conversely, effects of language-switching in comprehension were observed in the anterior cingulate cortex and were not shared by category-switching. These results suggest that bilingual individuals rely on adaptive language control strategies and that the neural involvement during language-switching could be extensively influenced by whether the switch is active (e.g., in production) or passive (e.g., in comprehension). In addition, these results support that humans require high-level cognitive control to switch languages in production, but the comprehension of language switches recruits a distinct neural circuitry. The use of MEG enabled us to obtain the first characterization of the spatiotemporal profile of these effects, establishing that switching processes begin ∼ 400 ms after stimulus presentation. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT This research addresses the neural mechanisms underlying multilingual individuals' ability to successfully manage two or more languages, critically targeting whether language control is uniform across linguistic domains (production and comprehension) and whether it is a subdomain of general cognitive control. The results showed that language production and comprehension rely on different networks: whereas language control in production recruited domain-general networks, the brain bases of switching during comprehension seemed language specific. Therefore, the crucial assumption of the bilingual advantage hypothesis, that there is a close relationship between language control and general cognitive control, seems to only hold during production.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Esti Blanco-Elorrieta
- New York University Abu Dhabi Institute, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates, and Department of Psychology and
| | - Liina Pylkkänen
- New York University Abu Dhabi Institute, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates, and Department of Psychology and Department of Linguistics, New York University, New York, New York 10003
| |
Collapse
|
78
|
Chiew KS, Braver TS. Reward favors the prepared: Incentive and task-informative cues interact to enhance attentional control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2015; 42:52-66. [PMID: 26322689 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000129] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The dual mechanisms of control account suggests that cognitive control may be implemented through relatively proactive mechanisms in anticipation of stimulus onset, or through reactive mechanisms, triggered in response to changing stimulus demands. Reward incentives and task-informative cues (signaling the presence/absence of upcoming cognitive demand) have both been found to influence cognitive control in a proactive or preparatory fashion; yet, it is currently unclear whether and how such cue effects interact. We investigated this in 2 experiments using an adapted flanker paradigm, where task-informative and reward incentive cues were orthogonally manipulated on a trial-by-trial basis. In Experiment 1, results indicated that incentives not only speed reaction times, but specifically reduce both interference and facilitation effects when combined with task-informative cues, suggesting enhanced proactive attentional control. Experiment 2 manipulated the timing of incentive cue information, demonstrating that such proactive control effects were only replicated with sufficient time to process the incentive cue (early incentive); when incentive signals were presented close to target onset (late incentive) the primary effect was a speed-accuracy trade-off. Together, results suggest that advance cueing may trigger differing control strategies, and that these strategies may critically depend on both the timing-and the motivational incentive-to use such cues.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Todd S Braver
- Department of Psychology, Washington University in St. Louis
| |
Collapse
|
79
|
Chevalier N, Martis SB, Curran T, Munakata Y. Metacognitive processes in executive control development: the case of reactive and proactive control. J Cogn Neurosci 2015; 27:1125-36. [PMID: 25603026 PMCID: PMC4510990 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00782] [Citation(s) in RCA: 114] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Young children engage cognitive control reactively in response to events, rather than proactively preparing for events. Such limitations in executive control have been explained in terms of fundamental constraints on children's cognitive capacities. Alternatively, young children might be capable of proactive control but differ from older children in their metacognitive decisions regarding when to engage proactive control. We examined these possibilities in three conditions of a task-switching paradigm, varying in whether task cues were available before or after target onset. RTs, ERPs, and pupil dilation showed that 5-year-olds did engage in advance preparation, a critical aspect of proactive control, but only when reactive control was made more difficult, whereas 10-year-olds engaged in proactive control whenever possible. These findings highlight metacognitive processes in children's cognitive control, an understudied aspect of executive control development.
Collapse
|
80
|
The neural dynamic mechanisms of asymmetric switch costs in a combined Stroop-task-switching paradigm. Sci Rep 2015; 5:10240. [PMID: 25989933 PMCID: PMC4650803 DOI: 10.1038/srep10240] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/12/2014] [Accepted: 04/07/2015] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Switch costs have been constantly found asymmetrical when switching between two tasks of unequal dominance. We used a combined Stroop-task-switching paradigm and recorded electroencephalographic (EEG) signals to explore the neural mechanism underlying the phenomenon of asymmetrical switch costs. The results revealed that a fronto-central N2 component demonstrated greater negativity in word switch (cW) trials relative to word repeat (wW) trials, and both First P3 and P3b components over the parieto-central region exhibited greater positivity in color switch (wC) trials relative to color repeat (cC) trials, whereas a contrasting switch-related fronto-central SP effect was found to have an opposite pattern for each task. Moreover, the time-frequency analysis showed a right-frontal lower alpha band (9-11 Hz) modulation in the word task, whereas a fronto-central upper alpha band (11-13 Hz) modulation was exclusively found in the color task. These results provide evidence for dissociable neural processes, which are related to inhibitory control and endogenous control, contributing to the generation of asymmetrical switch costs.
Collapse
|
81
|
Gaál ZA, Czigler I. Age-related processing strategies and go-nogo effects in task-switching: an ERP study. Front Hum Neurosci 2015; 9:177. [PMID: 26029072 PMCID: PMC4428357 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00177] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2014] [Accepted: 03/16/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
We studied cognitive and age-related changes in three task-switching (TS) paradigms: (1) informatively cued TS with go stimuli, (2) informatively cued TS with go and nogo stimuli, (3) non-informatively cued TS with go and nogo stimuli. This design allowed a direct comparison, how informative and non-informative cues influenced preparatory processes, and how nogo stimuli changed the context of the paradigm and cognitive processing in different aging groups. Beside the behavioral measures [reaction time (RT), error rate], event-related potentials (ERPs) were registered to the cue and target stimuli in young (N = 39, mean age = 21.6 ± 1.6 years) and older (N = 40, mean age = 65.7 ± 3.2 years) adults. The results provide evidence for declining performance in the older group: they had slower RT, less hits, more erroneous responses, higher mixing costs and decreased amplitude of ERP components than the participants of the younger group. In the task without the nogo stimuli young adults kept the previous task-set active that could be seen in shorter RT and larger amplitude of cue-locked late positivity (P3b) in task repeat (TR) trials compared to task switch trials. If both go and nogo stimuli were presented, similar RTs and P3b amplitudes appeared in the TR and TS trials. In the complex task situations older adults did not evolve an appropriate task representation and task preparation, as indicated by the lack of cue-locked P3b, CNV, and target-locked P3b. We conclude that young participants developed explicit representation of task structures, but the presence of nogo stimuli had marked effects on such representation. On the other hand, older people used only implicit control strategy to solve the task, hence the basic difference between the age groups was their strategy of task execution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zsófia A Gaál
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Hungarian Academy of Sciences Budapest, Hungary
| | - István Czigler
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Hungarian Academy of Sciences Budapest, Hungary
| |
Collapse
|
82
|
Tsai CL, Wang WL. Exercise-mode-related changes in task-switching performance in the elderly. Front Behav Neurosci 2015; 9:56. [PMID: 25798097 PMCID: PMC4351633 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00056] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/25/2014] [Accepted: 02/17/2015] [Indexed: 12/24/2022] Open
Abstract
The objective of the current study was to explore the relationships between exercise modes and executive functions in the elderly. Twenty-one elderly individuals in the open-skill group, 22 in the closed-skill group, and 21 in the sedentary-behavior (control) group were recruited in the current study, and performed a task-switching paradigm during which the switches occurred unpredictably and infrequently, while the behavioral and electrophysiological performances were assessed simultaneously. The results indicated that although there were no group differences in accuracy rates, the two exercise groups exhibited shorter reaction times (RTs), and larger P2 and P3 amplitudes across all conditions compared to the control group. In addition, the exercise-mode differences revealed a relatively smaller specific cost, and faster motor RTs and larger P3 amplitudes, in the switch condition for the open-skill group in comparison with the closed-skill and control groups. These findings suggest that regularly participating in physical exercise can enhance behavioral and electrophysiological performance with regard to executive control in the elderly, and provide further evidence for the beneficial effects of open-skill exercise on the task-switching paradigm.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chia-Liang Tsai
- Lab of Cognitive Neurophysiology, Institute of Physical Education, Health and Leisure Studies, National Cheng Kung University Tainan, Taiwan
| | - Wen-Liang Wang
- Lab of Cognitive Neurophysiology, Institute of Physical Education, Health and Leisure Studies, National Cheng Kung University Tainan, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
83
|
Hilgard J, Bartholow BD, Dickter CL, Blanton H. Characterizing switching and congruency effects in the Implicit Association Test as reactive and proactive cognitive control. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2015; 10:381-8. [PMID: 24812074 PMCID: PMC4350479 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsu060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2013] [Revised: 03/07/2014] [Accepted: 04/28/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent research has identified an important role for task switching, a cognitive control process often associated with executive functioning, in the Implicit Association Test (IAT). However, switching does not fully account for IAT effects, particularly when performance is scored using more recent d-score formulations. The current study sought to characterize multiple control processes involved in IAT performance through the use of event-related brain potentials (ERPs). Participants performed a race-evaluative IAT while ERPs were recorded. Behaviorally, participants experienced superadditive reaction time costs of incongruency and task switching, consistent with previous studies. The ERP showed a marked medial frontal negativity (MFN) 250-450 ms post-stimulus at midline fronto-central locations that were more negative for incongruent than congruent trials but more positive for switch than for no-switch trials, suggesting separable control processes are engaged by these two factors. Greater behavioral IAT bias was associated with both greater switch-related and congruency-related ERP activity. Findings are discussed in terms of the Dual Mechanisms of Control model of reactive and proactive cognitive control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joseph Hilgard
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211-2500, Department of Psychology, College of William and Mary, Williamsburg, VA 23187-8795, and Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269-1020, USA
| | - Bruce D Bartholow
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211-2500, Department of Psychology, College of William and Mary, Williamsburg, VA 23187-8795, and Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269-1020, USA
| | - Cheryl L Dickter
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211-2500, Department of Psychology, College of William and Mary, Williamsburg, VA 23187-8795, and Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269-1020, USA
| | - Hart Blanton
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211-2500, Department of Psychology, College of William and Mary, Williamsburg, VA 23187-8795, and Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269-1020, USA
| |
Collapse
|
84
|
Cooper PS, Wong AS, Fulham W, Thienel R, Mansfield E, Michie PT, Karayanidis F. Theta frontoparietal connectivity associated with proactive and reactive cognitive control processes. Neuroimage 2015; 108:354-63. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 99] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2014] [Revised: 11/07/2014] [Accepted: 12/10/2014] [Indexed: 12/01/2022] Open
|
85
|
The influence of monetary incentives on context processing in younger and older adults: an event-related potential study. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2015; 15:416-34. [DOI: 10.3758/s13415-015-0335-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
|
86
|
Weiler J, Hassall CD, Krigolson OE, Heath M. The unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost: Electroencephalographic evidence of task-set inertia in oculomotor control. Behav Brain Res 2015; 278:323-9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.10.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/12/2014] [Revised: 10/05/2014] [Accepted: 10/11/2014] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
87
|
At will or not at will: Electrophysiological correlates of preparation for voluntary and instructed task-switching paradigms. Psychon Bull Rev 2014; 22:1389-402. [DOI: 10.3758/s13423-014-0789-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
88
|
Hsieh S, Wu M, Lin F. Neural correlates of response-effector switching using event-related potentials. Biol Psychol 2014; 103:332-48. [PMID: 25448134 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.10.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2014] [Revised: 10/20/2014] [Accepted: 10/21/2014] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
The primary aim of the present study was to explore whether response-effector shifts can be considered as a cognitive component in models of task switching. The secondary aim was to provide some information regarding the issue of whether the two types of task shifts, stimulus-dimension shift and response-effector shift, share common and/or distinct switch-related ERP modulations. The tertiary aim was to illuminate the organization of task-set components by comparing the performance of a concurrent shift of both stimulus dimensions and response effectors to that of a single shift. Two experiments with two different types of judgment tasks (Experiment 1: a same-match-to-sample task; Experiment 2: a categorical-judgment task) were conducted. Intermittently cued task switching was employed. Each trial was composed of a series of stimulus displays following a transition-cue display, which indicated whether the current trial was identical to (repeat) or different from the previous trial (switch). There were stimulus-dimension (color and shape) and response-effector (hand and foot) variables that could be repeated or switched independently with an equal probability from the previous trial. Regarding the primary issue, the results of the two experiments reported in this study consistently showed significant RT switch costs as well as switch-related ERP modulations for a shift of response effectors. Yet, one of the switch-related ERPs, i.e., the cue-locked P3b, observed in this study was found to be reduced rather than increased in amplitudes. As to the secondary issue, the two experiments consistently showed that the two single shifts share some common switch-related ERPs. Finally, this study also provides ERP evidence for the integrated model of task-set organization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shulan Hsieh
- Cognitive Electrophysiology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, National Cheng Kung University, 1 University Road, Tainan 701, Taiwan.
| | - Mengyao Wu
- Cognitive Electrophysiology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, National Cheng Kung University, 1 University Road, Tainan 701, Taiwan
| | - Fan Lin
- Cognitive Electrophysiology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, National Cheng Kung University, 1 University Road, Tainan 701, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
89
|
Schmitt H, Wolff MC, Ferdinand NK, Kray J. Age Differences in the Processing of Context Information. J PSYCHOPHYSIOL 2014. [DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000126] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
The dual mechanisms of control theory (DMC; Braver & Barch, 2002 ) assumes that age-related changes in the temporal structure of context processing underlie age differences in numerous cognitive control tasks. Younger adults usually exhibit a proactive control mode, characterized by cue-related context updating, while older adults show a reactive control mode, updating information when interference is detected. This study aimed at determining whether age differences in electrophysiological correlates of context updating in a pro-and reactive manner are independent of individual differences in task performance. To this end, younger and older adults were split into four groups according to their updating efficiency in behavioral data. Nineteen younger and 18 older adults completed a modified AX-Continuous-Performance Task ( Lenartowicz, Escobedo-Quiroz, & Cohen, 2010 ) in which correct responses to probes were either dependent (c-dep) or independent (c-indep) on a preceding contextual cue. Analysis of the behavioral data showed no differences in context updating when performance was matched, that is, between low performing younger and high performing older adults. However, low performing younger adults showed larger cue-locked parietal P3b amplitudes on c-dep than c-indep trials, indicating c-dep trials to require context updating to a larger extent, while high performing older adults exhibited a specific control strategy and continuously updated context information, as reflected in comparable P3b amplitudes on c-dep and c-indep trials. The persistent age effect in the P3b when controlling for performance differences suggests context updating to be fundamental to cognitive aging. High performing in contrast to low performing older adults also showed a larger negative N450 to ambiguous probes on c-dep trials associated with conflict detection. According to the DMC, this finding suggests late conflict detection at the time interference is detected, indicative of a reactive control style particular in high performing elderly.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Jutta Kray
- Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
90
|
De Baene W, Brass M. Dissociating strategy-dependent and independent components in task preparation. Neuropsychologia 2014; 62:331-40. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.04.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2013] [Revised: 04/01/2014] [Accepted: 04/14/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
91
|
Weiler J, Heath M. Oculomotor task switching: alternating from a nonstandard to a standard response yields the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost. J Neurophysiol 2014; 112:2176-84. [PMID: 25122700 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00352.2014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The completion of an antisaccade (i.e., a nonstandard task) lengthens the reaction time (RT) of a subsequent prosaccade: a behavioral phenomenon termed the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost. One explanation for the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost is suppressing a stimulus-driven prosaccade during the preceding antisaccade trial engenders a residual inhibition of the oculomotor networks that support prosaccade planning (i.e., the oculomotor inhibition hypothesis). Alternatively, the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost may reflect the persistent activation of the antisaccade's nonstandard task rules (i.e., task set), which delays the planning of the next prosaccade (i.e., task-set inertia hypothesis). To determine which hypothesis provides the most parsimonious account for the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost, participants alternated between pro- and antisaccades wherein task instructions (i.e., pro- and antisaccade) were provided before (i.e., classic cuing) or concurrent (i.e., delayed cuing) with response cuing. Importantly, pro- and antisaccades elicited via the delayed cuing condition required the suppression of a stimulus-driven prosaccade at response cuing (i.e., response suppression) to discern the appropriate to-be-performed task. Results showed that classic and delayed antisaccades, but not delayed prosaccades, lengthened the RT of subsequent prosaccades. That delayed prosaccades, which require response suppression for their successful execution, did not lengthen the RT of subsequent prosaccades indicates that the oculomotor inhibition hypothesis does not account for the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost. Instead, the current findings are in line with the assertion that the task set associated with a nonstandard antisaccade persists inertially and delays the planning of a subsequent prosaccade (i.e., task-set inertia hypothesis).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey Weiler
- School of Kinesiology, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada; and
| | - Matthew Heath
- School of Kinesiology, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada; and Graduate Program in Neuroscience, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
92
|
Whitson LR, Karayanidis F, Fulham R, Provost A, Michie PT, Heathcote A, Hsieh S. Reactive control processes contributing to residual switch cost and mixing cost across the adult lifespan. Front Psychol 2014; 5:383. [PMID: 24817859 PMCID: PMC4012181 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00383] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/14/2013] [Accepted: 04/11/2014] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
In task-switching paradigms, performance is better when repeating the same task than when alternating between tasks (switch cost) and when repeating a task alone rather than intermixed with another task (mixing cost). These costs remain even after extensive practice and when task cues enable advanced preparation (residual costs). Moreover, residual reaction time mixing cost has been consistently shown to increase with age. Residual switch and mixing costs modulate the amplitude of the stimulus-locked P3b. This mixing effect is disproportionately larger in older adults who also prepare more for and respond more cautiously on these “mixed” repeat trials (Karayanidis et al., 2011). In this paper, we analyze stimulus-locked and response-locked P3 and lateralized readiness potentials to identify whether residual switch and mixing cost arise from the need to control interference at the level of stimulus processing or response processing. Residual mixing cost was associated with control of stimulus-level interference, whereas residual switch cost was also associated with a delay in response selection. In older adults, the disproportionate increase in mixing cost was associated with greater interference at the level of decision-response mapping and response programming for repeat trials in mixed-task blocks. These findings suggest that older adults strategically recruit greater proactive and reactive control to overcome increased susceptibility to post-stimulus interference. This interpretation is consistent with recruitment of compensatory strategies to compensate for reduced repetition benefit rather than an overall decline on cognitive flexibility.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa R Whitson
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory, School of Psychology, University of Newcastle Callaghan, NSW, Australia ; Centre for Translational Neuroscience and Mental Health Research, The University of Newcastle Callaghan, NSW, Australia ; Hunter Medical Research Institute, New Lambton Heights NSW, Australia
| | - Frini Karayanidis
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory, School of Psychology, University of Newcastle Callaghan, NSW, Australia ; Centre for Translational Neuroscience and Mental Health Research, The University of Newcastle Callaghan, NSW, Australia ; Hunter Medical Research Institute, New Lambton Heights NSW, Australia
| | - Ross Fulham
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory, School of Psychology, University of Newcastle Callaghan, NSW, Australia ; Centre for Translational Neuroscience and Mental Health Research, The University of Newcastle Callaghan, NSW, Australia ; Hunter Medical Research Institute, New Lambton Heights NSW, Australia
| | - Alexander Provost
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory, School of Psychology, University of Newcastle Callaghan, NSW, Australia ; Centre for Translational Neuroscience and Mental Health Research, The University of Newcastle Callaghan, NSW, Australia ; Hunter Medical Research Institute, New Lambton Heights NSW, Australia
| | - Patricia T Michie
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory, School of Psychology, University of Newcastle Callaghan, NSW, Australia ; Centre for Translational Neuroscience and Mental Health Research, The University of Newcastle Callaghan, NSW, Australia ; Hunter Medical Research Institute, New Lambton Heights NSW, Australia
| | - Andrew Heathcote
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory, School of Psychology, University of Newcastle Callaghan, NSW, Australia ; Centre for Translational Neuroscience and Mental Health Research, The University of Newcastle Callaghan, NSW, Australia ; Hunter Medical Research Institute, New Lambton Heights NSW, Australia
| | - Shulan Hsieh
- Department of Psychology and Institute of Allied Health Sciences, National Cheng Kung University Tainan City, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
93
|
Yildiz A, Wolf OT, Beste C. Stress intensifies demands on response selection during action cascading processes. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2014; 42:178-87. [PMID: 24636514 DOI: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.01.022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/06/2013] [Revised: 01/26/2014] [Accepted: 01/27/2014] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
Stress has been shown to modulate a number of cognitive processes including action control. These functions are important in daily life and are mediated by various cognitive subprocesses. However, it is unknown if stress affects the whole processing cascade, or exerts specific effects on a restricted subset of processes involved in the chaining of actions. We examine the effects of stress on action selection processes in a stop-change paradigm and apply event-related potentials (ERPs) combined with source localization analysis to examine potentially restricted effects of stress on subprocesses mediating action cascading. The results show that attentional selection processes, as well as processes related to allocation of processing resources were not affected by stress. Stress only seems to affect response selection functions during action cascading and leads to slowing of responses when two actions are executed in succession. These changes are related to the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Changes in response selection were predictable on the basis of individual salivary cortisol levels. The results show that stress does not affect the whole processing cascade involved in the cascading of different actions, but seems to exert circumscribed effects on response selection processes which have previously been shown to depend on dopaminergic neural transmission.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ali Yildiz
- Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, Biopsychology, Ruhr Universität Bochum, Germany; Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, TU Dresden, Germany
| | - Oliver T Wolf
- Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, Cognitive Psychology, Ruhr Universität Bochum, Germany
| | - Christian Beste
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, TU Dresden, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
94
|
Age-differential effects on updating cue information: Evidence from event-related potentials. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2014; 14:1115-31. [DOI: 10.3758/s13415-014-0268-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
95
|
A novel cognitive-neurophysiological state biomarker in premanifest Huntington's disease validated on longitudinal data. Sci Rep 2014; 3:1797. [PMID: 23652721 PMCID: PMC3647202 DOI: 10.1038/srep01797] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2013] [Accepted: 04/19/2013] [Indexed: 01/25/2023] Open
Abstract
In several neurodegenerative diseases, like Huntington's disease (HD), treatments are still lacking. To determine whether a treatment is effective, sensitive disease progression biomarkers are especially needed for the premanifest phase, since this allows the evaluation of neuroprotective treatments preventing, or delaying disease manifestation. On the basis of a longitudinal study we present a biomarker that was derived by integrating behavioural and neurophysiological data reflecting cognitive processes of action control. The measure identified is sensitive enough to track disease progression over a period of only 6 month. Changes tracked were predictive for a number of clinically relevant parameters and the sensitivity of the measure was higher than that of currently used parameters to track prodromal disease progression. The study provides a biomarker, which could change practice of progression diagnostics in a major basal ganglia disease and which may help to evaluate potential neuroprotective treatments in future clinical trials.
Collapse
|
96
|
Karayanidis F, Jamadar S, Sanday D. Stimulus-level interference disrupts repetition benefit during task switching in middle childhood. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:841. [PMID: 24367317 PMCID: PMC3851739 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00841] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/08/2013] [Accepted: 11/19/2013] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The task-switching paradigm provides a powerful tool to measure the development of core cognitive control processes. In this study, we use the alternating runs task-switching paradigm to assess preparatory control processes involved in flexibly preparing for a predictable change in task and stimulus-driven control processes involved in controlling stimulus-level interference. We present three experiments that examine behavioral and event-related potential (ERP) measures of task-switching performance in middle childhood and young adulthood under low and high stimulus interference conditions. Experiment 1 confirms that our new child-friendly tasks produce similar behavioral and electrophysiological findings in young adults as those previously reported. Experiment 2 examines task switching with univalent stimuli across a range of preparation intervals in middle childhood. Experiment 3 compares task switching with bivalent stimuli across the same preparation intervals in children and young adults. Children produced a larger RT switch cost than adults with univalent stimuli and a short preparation interval. Both children and adults showed significant reduction in switch cost with increasing preparation interval, but in children this was caused by greater increase in RT for repeat than switch trials. Response-locked ERPs showed intact preparation for univalent, but less efficient preparation for bivalent stimulus conditions. Stimulus-locked ERPs confirmed that children showed greater stimulus-level interference for repeat trials, especially with bivalent stimuli. We conclude that children show greater stimulus-level interference especially for repeat trials under high interference conditions, suggesting weaker mental representation of the current task set.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Frini Karayanidis
- Functional Neuroimaging Lab, School of Psychology, University of NewcastleCallaghan, NSW, Australia
- Centre for Translational Neuroscience and Mental Health, University of Newcastle and Hunter Medical Research InstituteCallaghan, NSW, Australia
| | - Sharna Jamadar
- Functional Neuroimaging Lab, School of Psychology, University of NewcastleCallaghan, NSW, Australia
- Centre for Translational Neuroscience and Mental Health, University of Newcastle and Hunter Medical Research InstituteCallaghan, NSW, Australia
- Monash Biomedical Imaging and School of Psychology and Psychiatry, Monash UniversityMelbourne, VIC, Australia
| | - Dearne Sanday
- Functional Neuroimaging Lab, School of Psychology, University of NewcastleCallaghan, NSW, Australia
- Centre for Translational Neuroscience and Mental Health, University of Newcastle and Hunter Medical Research InstituteCallaghan, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
97
|
Hawkes TD, Manselle W, Woollacott MH. Cross-sectional comparison of executive attention function in normally aging long-term T'ai chi, meditation, and aerobic fitness practitioners versus sedentary adults. J Altern Complement Med 2013; 20:178-84. [PMID: 24286339 DOI: 10.1089/acm.2013.0266] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
This cross-sectional field study documented the effect of long-term t'ai chi, meditation, or aerobic exercise training versus a sedentary lifestyle on executive function. It was predicted that long-term training in t'ai chi and meditation plus exercise would produce greater benefits to executive function than aerobic exercise. T'ai chi and meditation plus exercise include mental and physical training. Fifty-four volunteers were tested: t'ai chi (n=10); meditation+exercise (n=16); aerobic exercisers (n=16); and sedentary controls (n=12). A one-factor (group), one-covariate (age) multivariate analysis of covariance was performed. Significant main effects of group and age were found (group, 67.9%, p<0.001; age, 76.3%, p=0.001). T'ai chi and meditation practitioners but not aerobic exercisers outperformed sedentary controls on percent switch costs (p=0.001 and p=0.006, respectively), suggesting that there may be differential effects of training type on executive function.
Collapse
|
98
|
Rangelov D, Töllner T, Müller HJ, Zehetleitner M. What are task-sets: a single, integrated representation or a collection of multiple control representations? Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:524. [PMID: 24027513 PMCID: PMC3759751 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00524] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2013] [Accepted: 08/13/2013] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Performing two randomly alternating tasks typically results in higher reaction times (RTs) following a task switch, relative to a task repetition. These task switch costs (TSC) reflect processes of switching between control settings for different tasks. The present study investigated whether task sets operate as a single, integrated representation or as an agglomeration of relatively independent components. In a cued task switch paradigm, target detection (present/absent) and discrimination (blue/green/right-/left-tilted) tasks alternated randomly across trials. The target was either a color or an orientation singleton among homogeneous distractors. Across two trials, the task and target-defining dimension repeated or changed randomly. For task switch trials, agglomerated task sets predict a difference between dimension changes and repetitions: joint task and dimension switches require full task set reconfiguration, while dimension repetitions permit re-using some control settings from the previous trial. By contrast, integrated task sets always require full switches, predicting dimension repetition effects (DREs) to be absent across task switches. RT analyses showed significant DREs across task switches as well as repetitions supporting the notion of agglomerated task sets. Additionally, two event-related potentials (ERP) were analyzed: the Posterior-Contralateral-Negativity (PCN) indexing spatial selection dynamics, and the Sustained-Posterior-Contralateral-Negativity (SPCN) indexing post-selective perceptual/semantic analysis. Significant DREs across task switches were observed for both the PCN and SPCN components. Together, DREs across task switches for RTs and two functionally distinct ERP components suggest that re-using control settings across different tasks is possible. The results thus support the “agglomerated-task-set” hypothesis, and are inconsistent with “integrated task sets.”
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dragan Rangelov
- Department Psychologie, Allgemeine und Experimentelle Psychologie I, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München München, Germany
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
99
|
Kopp B, Lange F. Electrophysiological indicators of surprise and entropy in dynamic task-switching environments. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:300. [PMID: 23840183 PMCID: PMC3699721 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00300] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/06/2012] [Accepted: 06/05/2013] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
This event-related brain potential (ERP) study aimed at bridging two hitherto widely separated domains of cognitive neuroscience. Specifically, we combined the analysis of cognitive control in a cued task-switching paradigm with the fundamental question of how uncertainty is encoded in the brain. Two functional models of P3 amplitude variation in cued task-switching paradigms were put to an empirical test: (1) According to the P3b surprise hypothesis, parietal P3b waveforms are related to surprise over switch cues. (2) According to the P3a entropy hypothesis, frontal P3a waveforms are associated with entropy over switch outcomes. In order to examine these hypotheses, we measured the EEG while sixteen healthy young participants performed cued task-switching paradigms closely modeled to the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). We applied a factorial design, with number of tasks (two vs. three viable tasks), cue explicitness (task cuing vs. transition cuing), and cue contingency (prospectively-signaled cuing vs. feedback-based cuing) as independent variables. The ERP results replicated the commonly reported P3b effect associated with task switches, and further showed that P3a amplitudes were related to the entropy of switch outcomes, thereby supporting both hypotheses. Based on these ERP data, we suggest that surprise over task switches, and entropy over switch outcomes, constitute dissociable functional correlates of P3b and P3a ERP components in task-switching paradigms, respectively. Finally, a theoretical integration of the findings is proposed within the framework of Sokolov's (1966) entropy model of the orienting response (OR).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bruno Kopp
- Department of Neurology, Hannover Medical School Hannover, Germany ; Cognitive Neurology, Technische Universität Braunschweig Braunschweig, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
100
|
Elchlepp H, Rumball F, Lavric A. A brain-potential correlate of task-set conflict. Psychophysiology 2013; 50:314-23. [PMID: 23317090 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/09/2011] [Accepted: 11/10/2012] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Brain-potential correlates of response conflict are well documented, but those of task conflict are not. Task-switching studies have suggested a plausible correlate of task conflict--a poststimulus posterior negativity--however, in such paradigms the negativity may also reflect poststimulus task-set reconfiguration postulated in some models. Here, participants alternated between single-task blocks of classifying letters and digits; hence, no within-block task-set reconfiguration was required. Presenting letters alongside digits slowed responses to the digits and elicited an ERP negativity from ≈ 350 ms, relative to task-neutral symbols presented alongside digits, consistent with task conflict. The negativity was also present for congruent digit-letter stimuli; this and the lack of behavioral response congruency effects indicate conflict at the level of task-set rather than response selection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Heike Elchlepp
- Centre for Cognitive Control and Associative Learning, University of Exeter, Exeter, UK.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|