1
|
Bolcato S, Aplin L. The effect of habitat health and environmental change on cultural diversity and richness in animals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2025; 380:20240141. [PMID: 40308145 PMCID: PMC12044386 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2024.0141] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/08/2024] [Revised: 01/09/2025] [Accepted: 01/14/2025] [Indexed: 05/02/2025] Open
Abstract
There is increasing evidence that habitat decline via fragmentation or species loss can lead to loss of cultural diversity, complexity or richness in non-human animals. For example, a reduction in local bird species richness leads lyrebirds to sing fewer complex songs, while great apes living in fragmented landscapes have smaller cultural repertoires. However, the link between animal culture and local ecology remains understudied, and the potentially complex interactions between ongoing ecological change and animal culture are poorly understood. Here, we review the current state of knowledge on how ecology influences animal culture, focusing on vocal communication and foraging behaviour. We identify key factors affecting cultural patterning, including direct effects (e.g. environmental variability) and indirect effects (e.g. connectivity). We then review the emerging evidence for the effects of environmental change on culture, identifying three major threatening processes: habitat fragmentation, habitat degradation and urbanization. Finally, we develop a predictive framework for the effect of these threatening processes on animal culture and highlight how the loss of cultural diversity and complexity can lead to fitness costs with conservation implications.This article is part of the theme issue 'Animal culture: conservation in a changing world'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sofia Bolcato
- Division of Ecology and Evolution, Research School of Biology, Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory 2601, Australia
| | - Lucy Aplin
- Division of Ecology and Evolution, Research School of Biology, Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory 2601, Australia
- Institute of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, 8057 Zurich, Switzerland
- Cognitive and Cultural Ecology Research Group, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, 78315 Radolfzell, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Brakes P, Aplin L, Carroll EL, Greggor AL, Whiten A, Garland EC. Animal culture: conservation in a changing world. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2025; 380:20240127. [PMID: 40308140 PMCID: PMC12044377 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2024.0127] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/26/2025] [Revised: 04/09/2025] [Accepted: 04/09/2025] [Indexed: 05/02/2025] Open
Abstract
Social learning and animal culture can influence conservation outcomes in significant ways. Culture is a dynamic phenomenon; socially learned behaviours can be transmitted within and/or between generations and among populations, which can facilitate resilience, or in other circumstances generate vulnerability. Culture can be a driver of evolutionary diversification, population structure and demography, shaping sociality and influencing underlying biological processes such as reproduction and survival, affecting fitness. This theme issue synthesizes the current state of knowledge on cultural variation within major vertebrate taxa, offering practical insights on how social learning can interface directly with conservation interventions. It ranges over topics that include translocations, human-wildlife interactions and adaptation to anthropogenic change. Culture is complex; integrating cultural processes into conservation is challenging. No one-size-fits-all policy can be recommended. Instead, we aim to balance current understanding of underlying processes with a diversity of practical implementations in this nascent field, exploring and supporting developing pathways towards conservation efficiencies. Key themes that emerge include conserving cultural capacity, benefits of data sharing, along with the intrinsic value of animal cultures and the role of Indigenous Peoples and local communities.This article is part of the theme issue 'Animal culture: conservation in a changing world'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Philippa Brakes
- Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter Faculty of Environment Science and Economy, Penryn, Cornwall, UK
- Cetacean Ecology Research Group, School of Natural Sciences, Massey University, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Lucy Aplin
- Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Zürich8050, Switzerland
- Evolution and Ecology, Australian National University Research School of Biology, Acton, ACT, Australia
| | - Emma L. Carroll
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Auckland - Waipapa Taumata Rau, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Alison L. Greggor
- Conservation Science Wildlife Health, San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance, Escondido, CA, USA
| | - Andrew Whiten
- Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, UK
| | - Ellen C. Garland
- Sea Mammal Research Unit, School of Biology, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, UK
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Izar P, van de Waal E, Robbins MM. Integrating culture into primate conservation. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2025; 380:20240135. [PMID: 40308144 PMCID: PMC12044375 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2024.0135] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2024] [Revised: 12/11/2024] [Accepted: 12/18/2024] [Indexed: 05/02/2025] Open
Abstract
Primates exhibit the richest cultural repertoire among animal taxa, spanning foraging, communication, sociality and tool use. Understanding the cultural behaviours of primates has strongly influenced the study of animal behaviour and challenged traditional views that culture is exclusive to humans. With nearly 60% of primate species endangered owing to human-driven habitat changes, recent calls have emerged to integrate cultural diversity into conservation strategies. However, the integration of culture into primate conservation requires careful planning to avoid misallocation of resources or skewed conservation priorities. Our review reveals that studies on primate culture are limited to less than 3% of extant species, largely owing to taxonomic and methodological biases favouring long-term observations in protected habitats. We propose that including culture in conservation policies can broaden the scope of research, fostering more inclusive conservation agendas that address taxa with diverse habitats and underexplored cultural traits. Furthermore, anthropogenic habitat changes can both erode and foster cultural behaviours, emphasizing the need for context-specific conservation strategies. We suggest that recognizing cultural traits in conservation frameworks may enhance the resilience of primate populations in changing environments. This approach promises a more comprehensive and equitable allocation of conservation efforts, preserving both the biological and cultural diversity of primates.This article is part of the theme issue 'Animal culture: conservation in a changing world'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patricia Izar
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Universidade de Sao Paulo, Sao Paulo05508-030, Brazil
| | - Erica van de Waal
- Department of Ecology and Evolution, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Vaud, Switzerland
- Mawana Game Reserve, Inkawu Vervet Project, Kwazulu Natal, South Africa
| | - Martha M. Robbins
- Department of Primate Behavior and Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Whiten A, Rutz C. The growing methodological toolkit for identifying and studying social learning and culture in non-human animals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2025; 380:20240140. [PMID: 40308147 PMCID: PMC12044376 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2024.0140] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2024] [Revised: 11/28/2024] [Accepted: 12/16/2024] [Indexed: 05/02/2025] Open
Abstract
There is a growing consensus that animals' socially transmitted knowledge should be recognized when planning conservation management, but demonstrating social learning or culture can present considerable challenges, especially in the wild. Fortunately, decades of research have spawned a rich methodological toolkit for exactly this purpose. Here, we review principal approaches, including: social learning experiments; analyses of natural or experimentally seeded diffusions of novel behaviours, sometimes using specialist statistical techniques; mapping of behavioural variation across neighbouring, sympatric or captive groups, or at larger scales; and assessment of aspects of cross-generational transmission, including teaching, learning during ontogenetic development and cumulative change. Some methods reviewed were developed for captive studies, but have subsequently been adapted for application in the wild, or are useful for exploring a species' general propensity to learn and transmit information socially. We highlight several emerging 'rapid assessment' approaches-including camera trapping, passive acoustic monitoring, animal-borne tags, AI-assisted data mining and computer simulations-that should prove useful in addressing particularly urgent conservation needs. We conclude by considering how best to use this growing methodological toolkit in practice, to guide further research on animal social learning and cultures, and maximize conservation and policy impact.This article is part of the theme issue 'Animal culture: conservation in a changing world'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew Whiten
- Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, UK
| | - Christian Rutz
- Centre for Biological Diversity, School of Biology, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, UK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Hersh TA, Marcondes DS, Fonseca GF, Valle-Pereira JVS, Kratofil MA, Machado AMS, Atkins S, Bankhead KR, McGarvey K, Rahman MM, de Moura SPG, Fecci F, Cantor M. Ecology and conservation of socially learned foraging tactics in odontocetes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2025; 380:20240134. [PMID: 40308133 PMCID: PMC12044383 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2024.0134] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/05/2024] [Revised: 10/16/2024] [Accepted: 11/10/2024] [Indexed: 05/02/2025] Open
Abstract
Culture-group-typical behaviour shared by community members that rely on socially learned and transmitted information-can drive animal adaptations to local environments and thus has the potential of generating specialized behavioural tactics to solve fundamental life challenges, including capturing prey. However, as human activities rapidly change the world in unprecedented ways, animal foraging cultures may no longer represent optimal solutions to local environments. Odontocetes (toothed whales, dolphins and porpoises) are of particular concern because they rely on learned, specialized foraging tactics in habitats highly affected by human activities. We present a global inventory of odontocete foraging tactics to evaluate their cultural underpinnings, vulnerability to human-induced threats and how this knowledge can inform safeguards. Our synthesis reveals a diverse repertoire-190 cases of 36 foraging tactics in 21 species-but highlights that linkages between culture and anthropogenic impacts are generally obscured by a dearth of data on individual identity, social associations and behavioural diffusion. By identifying global patterns, knowledge gaps and common threats to specialized foraging, our review can guide long-term research towards understanding their ecological and evolutionary drivers. This crucial first step towards designing policies that mitigate human impacts on marine habitats may ultimately protect the diverse odontocete behavioural repertoires that contribute to their survival.This article is part of the theme issue 'Animal culture: conservation in a changing world'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taylor A. Hersh
- Marine Mammal Institute, Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, & Conservation Sciences, Oregon State University, Newport, OR97365, USA
| | - Daiane S. Marcondes
- Marine Mammal Institute, Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, & Conservation Sciences, Oregon State University, Newport, OR97365, USA
- Center for Marine Studies, Universidade Federal do Paraná, Pontal do Paraná, Paraná83255-976, Brazil
| | - Gabriel F. Fonseca
- Marine Mammal Institute, Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, & Conservation Sciences, Oregon State University, Newport, OR97365, USA
- Center for Marine Studies, Universidade Federal do Paraná, Pontal do Paraná, Paraná83255-976, Brazil
| | - João V. S. Valle-Pereira
- Marine Mammal Institute, Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, & Conservation Sciences, Oregon State University, Newport, OR97365, USA
| | - Michaela A. Kratofil
- Marine Mammal Institute, Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, & Conservation Sciences, Oregon State University, Newport, OR97365, USA
| | - Alexandre M. S. Machado
- Department of Ecology and Zoology, Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Florianópolis, Santa Catarina88040-900, Brazil
| | - Shanan Atkins
- School of Animal, Plant and Environmental Sciences, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, Gauteng2000, South Africa
| | - Kyra R. Bankhead
- Marine Mammal Institute, Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, & Conservation Sciences, Oregon State University, Newport, OR97365, USA
| | - Kiera McGarvey
- Marine Mammal Institute, Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, & Conservation Sciences, Oregon State University, Newport, OR97365, USA
| | - Muhammad Mahmudur Rahman
- Marine Mammal Institute, Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, & Conservation Sciences, Oregon State University, Newport, OR97365, USA
| | - Stephane P. G. de Moura
- Marine Mammal Institute, Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, & Conservation Sciences, Oregon State University, Newport, OR97365, USA
- Center for Marine Studies, Universidade Federal do Paraná, Pontal do Paraná, Paraná83255-976, Brazil
| | - Fernanda Fecci
- Center for Marine Studies, Universidade Federal do Paraná, Pontal do Paraná, Paraná83255-976, Brazil
| | - Mauricio Cantor
- Marine Mammal Institute, Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, & Conservation Sciences, Oregon State University, Newport, OR97365, USA
- Center for Marine Studies, Universidade Federal do Paraná, Pontal do Paraná, Paraná83255-976, Brazil
- Department of Ecology and Zoology, Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Florianópolis, Santa Catarina88040-900, Brazil
- School of Animal, Plant and Environmental Sciences, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, Gauteng2000, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Wessling EG, Whiten A, Soiret SK, Scholfield K, Samuni L, Rutz C, Redmond I, Pintea L, Lanjouw A, Koops K, Kamgang SA, Kalan AK, Ikemeh RA, Humle T, Hobaiter C, Frisch-Nwakanma H, Freymann E, Doumbe O, Brakes P, Abwe E, Sanz C. Concerted conservation actions to support chimpanzee cultures. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2025; 380:20240143. [PMID: 40308136 PMCID: PMC12044366 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2024.0143] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2024] [Revised: 11/23/2024] [Accepted: 12/02/2024] [Indexed: 05/02/2025] Open
Abstract
Chimpanzees were among the first animals recognized to have culture, and our understanding of the breadth of their cultural repertoire has grown significantly since the 1960s. Throughout their range, chimpanzee populations have come under increasing pressure, with their endangered status necessitating immediate and long-term conservation interventions. Recognizing the importance of diverse behavioural repertoires for chimpanzees' survival, there has been a recent focus of conservation efforts on preserving their culturally transmitted behaviours and the environments in which they are exhibited. This article evaluates the practicality of developing conservation measures focused on chimpanzee culture. We highlight innovative conservation strategies aimed at integrating chimpanzee cultural behaviours into conservation policies. We review synergistic conservation initiatives led by the International Union for Conservation of Nature, the UN Convention on the Conservation of Migratory Species of Wild Animals and other international and local groups that share the goal of preserving chimpanzee populations and their cultural diversity. We underline how successful conservation implementation requires engagement and collaboration with a diverse group of interested or affected people. Finally, we provide recommendations aimed at guiding future efforts to incorporate animal cultures into conservation strategies.This article is part of the theme issue 'Animal culture: conservation in a changing world'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erin G. Wessling
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St AndrewsKY16 9JP, UK
- Cognitive Ethology Lab, Deutsches Primatenzentrum GmbH - Leibniz-Institut für Primatenforschung, Göttingen, Niedersachsen37077, Germany
- Senckenberg Museum für Naturkunde Görlitz, 02826 Görlitz, Germany
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA02138, USA
- Cooperative Evolution Lab, Deutsches Primatenzentrum GmbH - Leibniz-Institut für Primatenforschung, Gottingen, Niedersachsen37077, Germany
| | - Andrew Whiten
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St AndrewsKY16 9JP, UK
| | - Serge K. Soiret
- Centre de Recherche en Ecologie (CRE) / Nangui Abrogoua University, 02 B.P. 801 Abidjan 02, Côte d'Ivoire
| | | | - Liran Samuni
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA02138, USA
- Cooperative Evolution Lab, Deutsches Primatenzentrum GmbH - Leibniz-Institut für Primatenforschung, Gottingen, Niedersachsen37077, Germany
| | - Christian Rutz
- Centre for Biological Diversity, School of Biology, University of St Andrews, St AndrewsKY16 9JP, UK
| | - Ian Redmond
- Ecoflix Foundation, Los Angeles, CA 91302, USA
- Ape Alliance, Stroud, UK
| | - Lilian Pintea
- Conservation Science Department, The Jane Goodall Institute, Washington, DC 20036, USA
| | | | - Kathelijne Koops
- Ape Behaviour & Ecology Group, Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Zurich, 8057 Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Serge A. Kamgang
- BSB Yamoussa, Deutsche Gesellschaft für Internationale Zusammenarbeit, Garoua-Plateau (Ecole de Faune), Cameroon
- Biodiversity-Environment & Sustainable Development, Garoua, Cameroon
| | - Ammie K. Kalan
- GAB Lab, Department of Anthropolgy, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada V8W 2Y2
| | | | | | - Catherine Hobaiter
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St AndrewsKY16 9JP, UK
| | - Heidrun Frisch-Nwakanma
- Secretariat Convention on the Conservation of Migratory Species of Wild Animals, 53113 Bonn, Germany
| | - Elodie Freymann
- School of Anthropology and Museum Ethnography, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6PE, UK
| | | | - Philippa Brakes
- Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, Penryn, Cornwall TR10 9FE, UK
- Cetacean Ecology Research Group, Massey University School of Natural Sciences, Albany, Auckland 0745, New Zealand
- Whale and Dolphin Conservation, Chippenham, Wiltshire SN15 1LJ, UK
| | - Ekwoge Abwe
- San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance, San Diego, CA 92027, USA
- Cameroon Biodiversity Association, Douala, Cameroon
| | - Crickette Sanz
- Department of Anthropology, Washington University in Saint Louis, St Louis, MO 63130, USA
- Congo Program, Wildlife Conservation Society, Brazzaville, Congo
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Andres‐Bray TC, Nichols I, Wilke T, Hafner M, Jordan A, Eysseric A, Borzym V, Abwe EE, Morgan B, Gonder MK. Patterns of Technical Variation in Chimpanzee Termite Fishing Behavior in Mbam and Djerem National Park, Cameroon. Am J Primatol 2025; 87:e70014. [PMID: 40023876 PMCID: PMC11872189 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.70014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2024] [Revised: 02/05/2025] [Accepted: 02/08/2025] [Indexed: 03/04/2025]
Abstract
Chimpanzees exhibit considerable inter- and intra-community variation in cognitively complex tool use behaviors, often attributed to social, genetic, and environmental factors. Termite fishing is a well-documented chimpanzee tool-using behavior that has been the subject of comparative research exploring behavioral variation between chimpanzee communities. However, termite fishing in the Nigeria-Cameroon chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes ellioti) has been historically underrepresented due to a lack of habituated populations. In this study, we used remote-activated camera traps at several termite mounds for 3 years to study termite fishing near Ganga Research Station in central Cameroon. We aimed to (1) identify elemental variation in chimpanzee termite fishing techniques at Ganga, an understudied community of P. t. ellioti, and (2) compare termite fishing behaviors in the Ganga community among more well-studied chimpanzee communities. We found 46 different combinations of behavioral elements representing termite fishing techniques used by Ganga chimpanzees (n = 9) across five termite mounds. The average technique was between three and four elements long (x̄ = 3.673), and many chimpanzees had unique personal repertoires. Chimpanzees at Ganga shared the most behavioral similarities with two communities of savanna chimpanzees, Dindefelo and Kayan, and the nearby rainforest community of La Belgique in southern Cameroon. This behavioral similarity between Ganga chimpanzees, who inhabit a complex forest/savanna matrix, and two distant savanna-dwelling communities suggests similar environmental contexts contribute to termite fishing similarity. These results add to comparative studies of termite fishing behavior and demonstrate the utility of quantitative ethnographic methodology in exploring chimpanzee behavioral variation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tyler C. Andres‐Bray
- Center for the Advancement of STEM Teaching and Learning ExcellenceDrexel UniversityPhiladelphiaPennsylvaniaUSA
- Cameroon Biodiversity Protection ProgramYaoundéCameroon
| | - Ian Nichols
- Department of Forest Resources and Environmental ConservationVirginia TechBlacksburgVirginiaUSA
| | - Tabitha Wilke
- Department of Ecology and Conservation BiologyTexas A&M UniversityCollege StationTexasUSA
| | - Macy Hafner
- Department of Biological SciencesDrexel UniversityPhiladelphiaPennsylvaniaUSA
| | - Abigail Jordan
- Department of Biological SciencesDrexel UniversityPhiladelphiaPennsylvaniaUSA
| | - Andrea Eysseric
- Department of Ecology and Conservation BiologyTexas A&M UniversityCollege StationTexasUSA
| | - Vivianna Borzym
- Department of Ecology and Conservation BiologyTexas A&M UniversityCollege StationTexasUSA
| | - Ekwoge E. Abwe
- Cameroon Biodiversity AssociationDoualaCameroon
- San Diego Zoo Wildlife AllianceSan DiegoCaliforniaUSA
| | - Bethan Morgan
- San Diego Zoo Wildlife AllianceSan DiegoCaliforniaUSA
| | - Mary Katherine Gonder
- Cameroon Biodiversity Protection ProgramYaoundéCameroon
- Department of Ecology and Conservation BiologyTexas A&M UniversityCollege StationTexasUSA
- Department of Biological SciencesDrexel UniversityPhiladelphiaPennsylvaniaUSA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Gilbert MA, Kalan AK. A review of great ape behavioural responses and their outcomes to anthropogenic landscapes. Primates 2025; 66:163-181. [PMID: 39903403 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-025-01180-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2024] [Accepted: 01/17/2025] [Indexed: 02/06/2025]
Abstract
Industrial expansion has brought humans and wildlife into closer contact, and added novel, complex dimensions to human-wildlife relationships. The seven great apes (chimpanzee, Bornean orangutan, Sumatran orangutan, Tapanuli orangutan, Eastern gorilla, Western gorilla, bonobo), the closest extant relatives to humans, have experienced substantial population declines resulting from anthropogenic activities. The effect of human activity on great ape behavioural ecology is therefore an emerging field of inquiry in primatology which has historically been minimally considered. This review explores how wild great apes respond behaviourally to human activities and environmental changes, synthesizing current knowledge and addressing potential outcomes and risks. Using precise search criteria, we found 96 studies documenting changes in great ape behaviour in response to human activity, and despite their broad geographic distribution, we found common patterns and responses across species to increasing human influence. Literature documented shifts in existing behaviour (57), the generation of novel behaviours (53) or reported both (15). Forty-three studies (45%) included direct (23) or indirect (20) assessment of the consequences of these behaviours. Only one study modelled a widespread loss of existing behaviours. The majority of studies included chimpanzees (67), followed by orangutans (19) and gorillas (19), and only 2 included bonobos. We found that the most frequently documented drivers of behavioural responses to anthropogenic activity were wide-scale land-use conversions in ape habitats. In response, apes have adopted crop foraging, and altered nesting behaviour, range use, and social strategies. While these responses appear to allow survival in the immediate sense, they may expose individuals to more risks in the long term. Analysis revealed that under many contexts changing great ape behaviour is putting strain on the human-ape relationship, resulting in injury, harassment, and even the killing of apes. We found examples of tolerant relationships between humans and apes shifting towards conflict, potentially worsening the conservation crisis and inviting inquiry into tolerance thresholds among human communities. We emphasize the importance of community-engaged strategies for reducing competition over resources and conclude that great ape behavioural responses to human activity must be interpreted through a locally specific lens.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miranda A Gilbert
- GAB Lab, Department of Anthropology, University of Victoria, 3800 Finnerty Rd, Victoria, BC, V8P 5C2, Canada.
| | - Ammie K Kalan
- GAB Lab, Department of Anthropology, University of Victoria, 3800 Finnerty Rd, Victoria, BC, V8P 5C2, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Kazaba PK, Kulik L, Beukou Choumbou GB, Douhin Tiémoko CB, Oni FL, Kamgang SA, Heinicke S, Koné I, Mucyo SJP, Sop T, Boesch C, Stephens C, Agbor A, Angedakin S, Bailey E, Bessone M, Coupland C, Deschner T, Dieguez P, Granjon A, Harder B, Head J, Hicks TC, Jones S, Kadam P, Kalan AK, Langergraber KE, Lapuente J, Lee KC, Lynn LK, Maldonado N, McCarthy MS, Meier AC, Ormsby LJ, Piel A, Robbins MM, Sciaky L, Sommer V, Stewart FA, Widness J, Wittig RM, Wessling EG, Arandjelovic M, Kühl H, van der Hoek Y. Chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes) Indicate Mammalian Abundance Across Broad Spatial Scales. Ecol Evol 2025; 15:e71000. [PMID: 40092902 PMCID: PMC11909632 DOI: 10.1002/ece3.71000] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/06/2024] [Revised: 02/02/2025] [Accepted: 02/04/2025] [Indexed: 03/19/2025] Open
Abstract
Ongoing ecosystem change and biodiversity decline across the Afrotropics call for tools to monitor the state of biodiversity or ecosystem elements across extensive spatial and temporal scales. We assessed relationships in the co-occurrence patterns between great apes and other medium to large-bodied mammals to evaluate whether ape abundance serves as a proxy for mammal diversity across broad spatial scales. We used camera trap footage recorded at 22 research sites, each known to harbor a population of chimpanzees, and some additionally a population of gorillas, across 12 sub-Saharan African countries. From ~350,000 1-min camera trap videos recorded between 2010 and 2016, we estimated mammalian community metrics, including species richness, Shannon diversity, and mean animal mass. We then fitted Bayesian Regression Models to assess potential relationships between ape detection rates (as proxy for ape abundance) and these metrics. We included site-level protection status, human footprint, and precipitation variance as control variables. We found that relationships between detection rates of great apes and other mammal species, as well as animal mass were largely positive. In contrast, relationships between ape detection rate and mammal species richness were less clear and differed according to site protection and human impact context. We found no clear association between ape detection rate and mammal diversity. Our findings suggest that chimpanzees hold potential as indicators of specific elements of mammalian communities, especially population-level and composition-related characteristics. Declines in chimpanzee populations may indicate associated declines of sympatric medium to large-bodied mammal species and highlight the need for improved conservation interventions.Changes in chimpanzee abundance likely precede extirpation of sympatric mammals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paul K. Kazaba
- Dian Fossey Gorilla Fund, Grauer's Gorilla Research and Conservation ProgramsGomaDemocratic Republic of the Congo
- Ecology, Restoration Ecology and Landscape (EREP) Research Unit, Département d'Aménagement des Ecosystèmes et Biodiversité, Faculté des Sciences AgronomiquesUniversité de LubumbashiLubumbashiDemocratic Republic of the Congo
| | - Lars Kulik
- Senckenberg Museum for Natural History GörlitzSenckenberg – Member of the Leibniz AssociationGörlitzGermany
| | | | | | - Funmilayo L. Oni
- Department of Wildlife and Ecotourism Management, Faculty of Renewable Natural ResourcesLadoke Akintola University of TechnologyOgbomosoNigeria
| | - Serge A. Kamgang
- Biodiversité‐Environnement et Développement DurableGarouaCameroon
- ERAIFT‐UNESCOKinshasaDemocratic Republic of the Congo
| | - Stefanie Heinicke
- Potsdam Institute for Climate Impact Research (PIK), Member of the Leibniz AssociationPotsdamGermany
| | - Inza Koné
- Université Félix Houphouët‐BoignyAbidjanCôte d'Ivoire
- Centre Suisse de Recherches Scientifiques en Côte d'IvoireAbidjanCôte d'Ivoire
| | | | - Tenekwetche Sop
- Senckenberg Museum for Natural History GörlitzSenckenberg – Member of the Leibniz AssociationGörlitzGermany
- Re:WildAustinTexasUSA
| | | | - Colleen Stephens
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
| | - Anthony Agbor
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
| | - Samuel Angedakin
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
- Department of Environmental ManagementMakerere UniversityKampalaUganda
| | - Emma Bailey
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
| | - Mattia Bessone
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
- Department of Biology, Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective BehaviourUniversity of KonstanzKonstanzGermany
- Department of Animal SocietiesMax Planck Institute of Animal BehaviourKonstanzGermany
| | | | - Tobias Deschner
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
- Comparative BioCognitionInstitute of Cognitive Science, University of OsnabrückOsnabrückGermany
| | - Paula Dieguez
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv)LeipzigGermany
| | | | - Briana Harder
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
| | | | | | - Sorrel Jones
- The David Attenborough Building, RSPB Centre for Conservation ScienceCambridgeUK
| | - Parag Kadam
- Warnell School of Forestry and Natural ResourcesUniversity of GeorgiaAthensGeorgiaUSA
| | - Ammie K. Kalan
- Department of AnthropologyUniversity of VictoriaVictoriaBritish ColumbiaCanada
| | - Kevin E. Langergraber
- School of Human Evolution and Social ChangeArizona State UniversityTempeArizonaUSA
- Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State UniversityTempeArizonaUSA
| | - Juan Lapuente
- Animal Ecology and Tropical Biology, Biozentrum, (Zoologie III)WürzburgGermany
| | - Kevin C. Lee
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
- School of Human Evolution and Social ChangeArizona State UniversityTempeArizonaUSA
- Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State UniversityTempeArizonaUSA
| | - Laura K. Lynn
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
| | - Nuria Maldonado
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
| | | | - Amelia C. Meier
- Hawai'i Institute of Marine BiologyUniversity of Hawai'i at MānoaHonoluluHawaiiUSA
| | | | - Alex Piel
- Department of AnthropologyUniversity College LondonLondonUK
| | - Martha M. Robbins
- Department of Primate Behavior and EvolutionMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeizpigGermany
| | - Lilah Sciaky
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
| | - Volker Sommer
- Department of AnthropologyUniversity College LondonLondonUK
- Gashaka Primate ProjectSertiTarabaNigeria
| | - Fiona A. Stewart
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
- Department of AnthropologyUniversity College LondonLondonUK
- School of Biological and Environmental SciencesLiverpool John Moores UniversityLiverpoolUK
| | | | - Roman M. Wittig
- Ape Social Mind LabInstitute of Cognitive Science, CNRS UMR5229BronFrance
- Taï Chimpanzee ProjectCentre Suisse de Recherche Scientifique en Côte d'IvoireAbidjanCôte d'Ivoire
| | - Erin G. Wessling
- Cognitive Ethology LaboratoryGerman Primate Center—Leibniz Institute for Primate ResearchGöttingenGermany
| | - Mimi Arandjelovic
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv)LeipzigGermany
- Department of Primate Behavior and EvolutionMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeizpigGermany
| | - Hjalmar Kühl
- Senckenberg Museum for Natural History GörlitzSenckenberg – Member of the Leibniz AssociationGörlitzGermany
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv)LeipzigGermany
- International Institute Zittau, Technische Universität DresdenZittauGermany
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Funkhouser JA, Boostrom H, Hellmuth H, Lacy L, Bekins S, Joshi P, Mayoukou W, Ndassoba S, Singono C, Abedine C, Ayina CE, Claisse E, Eslinger E, McElmurray P, Musgrave S, Morgan D, Sanz C. Chimpanzee Activity and Behavioral Diversity Extends Across 24 Hours in Both Captive and Wild Settings. Am J Primatol 2025; 87:e23729. [PMID: 39815785 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23729] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2024] [Revised: 12/03/2024] [Accepted: 01/01/2025] [Indexed: 01/18/2025]
Abstract
Studying nocturnal behavior is crucial for understanding the full scope of a species' behavioral flexibility so as to inform the conservation of wild populations and the care of captive individuals. However, this aspect of primate behavior is understudied, especially in great apes, which exhibit some of the widest documented behavioral diversity and flexibility. Our investigation is among the first to systematically compare the 24 h activity patterns and behavioral activities of captive chimpanzees (Saint Louis Zoo, USA) with those of wild chimpanzees (three sites across the Nouabalé-Ndoki National Park in the Republic of Congo) and a published data set of the nocturnal behavior of all chimpanzee subspecies. Furthermore, we examined the influence of human activity and changes to the group's composition on the activity patterns and nocturnal behaviors of the zoo-living chimpanzees. Our results reveal that the zoo-living chimpanzees exhibit significantly different activity patterns compared to their wild counterparts, with increased nocturnal activity (particularly in the early morning) and more observations of feeding and social behaviors at night. Additionally, the absence of human visitors and a change in the group's composition were found to influence these activity patterns. These findings underscore the importance of integrating more holistic approaches to captive primate care and wild primate conservation. This study also highlights the immense potential of implementing remote monitoring technology, such as video camera traps, across contexts. Such data that extend across contexts benefit not only the captive and wild great apes but also provide opportunities for caregivers, conservation managers, and students who are involved in these collaborative initiatives.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jake A Funkhouser
- Department of Anthropology, Washington University in Saint Louis, Saint Louis, Missouri, USA
- Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | | | | | - Logan Lacy
- Department of Anthropology, Washington University in Saint Louis, Saint Louis, Missouri, USA
| | - Sophie Bekins
- Department of Anthropology, Washington University in Saint Louis, Saint Louis, Missouri, USA
| | - Priyanka Joshi
- Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes, Lincoln Park Zoo, Chicago, Illinois, USA
| | - Wen Mayoukou
- Wildlife Conservation Society, Congo Program, Brazzaville, Republic of Congo
| | - Severin Ndassoba
- Wildlife Conservation Society, Congo Program, Brazzaville, Republic of Congo
| | - Chigue Singono
- Wildlife Conservation Society, Congo Program, Brazzaville, Republic of Congo
| | - Claude Abedine
- Wildlife Conservation Society, Congo Program, Brazzaville, Republic of Congo
| | - Crepin Eyana Ayina
- Wildlife Conservation Society, Congo Program, Brazzaville, Republic of Congo
| | - Emma Claisse
- Department of Anthropology, Washington University in Saint Louis, Saint Louis, Missouri, USA
| | - Emmalee Eslinger
- Department of Anthropology, Washington University in Saint Louis, Saint Louis, Missouri, USA
| | - Philip McElmurray
- Department of Anthropology, Washington University in Saint Louis, Saint Louis, Missouri, USA
| | - Stephanie Musgrave
- Department of Anthropology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, Florida, USA
| | - David Morgan
- Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes, Lincoln Park Zoo, Chicago, Illinois, USA
| | - Crickette Sanz
- Department of Anthropology, Washington University in Saint Louis, Saint Louis, Missouri, USA
- Wildlife Conservation Society, Congo Program, Brazzaville, Republic of Congo
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Kalan AK, Nakano R, Warshawski L. What we know and don't know about great ape cultural communication in the wild. Am J Primatol 2025; 87:e23560. [PMID: 37828822 PMCID: PMC11650962 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23560] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2023] [Revised: 09/15/2023] [Accepted: 10/02/2023] [Indexed: 10/14/2023]
Abstract
Following the first descriptions of culture in primates, widespread agreement has developed that the term can be applied to nonhumans as group-specific, socially learned behaviors. While behaviors such as those involving extractive tool use have been researched intensively, we propose that behaviors that are more subtle, less likely to be ecologically constrained, and more likely to be socially shaped, such as cultural forms of communication, provide compelling evidence of culture in nonhuman primates. Additionally, cultural forms of communication can provide novel insights into animal cognition such as the capacity for conformity, conventionalized meanings, arbitrariness in signal forms, and even symbolism. In this paper we focus on evidence from studies conducted on wild great apes. First, we provide a thorough review of what exactly we do know, and by extension don't know, about great ape cultural communication. We argue that detailed research on both vocal and gestural communication in wild great apes shows a more nuanced and variable repertoire than once assumed, with increasing support for group-specific variation. Second, we discuss the relevance of great ape cultural communication and its potential for illustrating evolutionary continuity for human-like cultural attributes, namely cumulative culture and symbolism. In sum, a concerted effort to examine cultural forms of communication in great apes could reveal novel evidence for cultural capacities that have thus far been heavily debated in the literature and can simultaneously contribute to an improved understanding of the complex minds of our closest living relatives.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ammie K. Kalan
- Department of AnthropologyUniversity of VictoriaVictoriaBritish ColumbiaCanada
| | - Robyn Nakano
- Department of AnthropologyUniversity of VictoriaVictoriaBritish ColumbiaCanada
| | - Lindsey Warshawski
- Department of AnthropologyUniversity of VictoriaVictoriaBritish ColumbiaCanada
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Wang X, Bocksberger G, Arandjelovic M, Agbor A, Angedakin S, Aubert F, Ayimisin EA, Bailey E, Barubiyo D, Bessone M, Bobe R, Bonnet M, Boucher R, Brazzola G, Brewer S, Lee KC, Carvalho S, Chancellor R, Cipoletta C, Cohen H, Copeland SR, Corogenes K, Costa AM, Coupland C, Curran B, de Ruiter DJ, Deschner T, Dieguez P, Dierks K, Dilambaka E, Dowd D, Dunn A, Egbe VE, Finckh M, Fruth B, Gijanto L, Yuh YG, Goedmakers A, Gokee C, Gomes Coelho R, Goodman AH, Granjon AC, Grimes V, Grueter CC, Haour A, Hedwig D, Hermans V, Hernandez-Aguilar RA, Hohmann G, Imong I, Jeffery KJ, Jones S, Junker J, Kadam P, Kambere M, Kambi M, Kienast I, Knudson KJ, Langergraber KE, Lapeyre V, Lapuente J, Larson B, Lautenschläger T, le Roux P, Leinert V, Llana M, Logan A, Lowry B, Lüdecke T, Maretti G, Marrocoli S, Fernandez R, McNeill PJ, Meier AC, Meller P, Monroe JC, Morgan D, Mulindahabi F, Murai M, Neil E, Nicholl S, Niyigaba P, Normand E, Ormsby LJ, Diotoh O, Pacheco L, Piel A, Preece J, Regnaut S, Richard FG, Richards MP, Rundus A, Sanz C, Sommer V, Sponheimer M, Steele TE, Stewart FA, Tagg N, Tédonzong LR, Tickle A, et alWang X, Bocksberger G, Arandjelovic M, Agbor A, Angedakin S, Aubert F, Ayimisin EA, Bailey E, Barubiyo D, Bessone M, Bobe R, Bonnet M, Boucher R, Brazzola G, Brewer S, Lee KC, Carvalho S, Chancellor R, Cipoletta C, Cohen H, Copeland SR, Corogenes K, Costa AM, Coupland C, Curran B, de Ruiter DJ, Deschner T, Dieguez P, Dierks K, Dilambaka E, Dowd D, Dunn A, Egbe VE, Finckh M, Fruth B, Gijanto L, Yuh YG, Goedmakers A, Gokee C, Gomes Coelho R, Goodman AH, Granjon AC, Grimes V, Grueter CC, Haour A, Hedwig D, Hermans V, Hernandez-Aguilar RA, Hohmann G, Imong I, Jeffery KJ, Jones S, Junker J, Kadam P, Kambere M, Kambi M, Kienast I, Knudson KJ, Langergraber KE, Lapeyre V, Lapuente J, Larson B, Lautenschläger T, le Roux P, Leinert V, Llana M, Logan A, Lowry B, Lüdecke T, Maretti G, Marrocoli S, Fernandez R, McNeill PJ, Meier AC, Meller P, Monroe JC, Morgan D, Mulindahabi F, Murai M, Neil E, Nicholl S, Niyigaba P, Normand E, Ormsby LJ, Diotoh O, Pacheco L, Piel A, Preece J, Regnaut S, Richard FG, Richards MP, Rundus A, Sanz C, Sommer V, Sponheimer M, Steele TE, Stewart FA, Tagg N, Tédonzong LR, Tickle A, Toubga L, van Schijndel J, Vergnes V, Njomen NW, Wessling EG, Willie J, Wittig RM, Yurkiw K, Zipkin AM, Zuberbühler K, Kühl HS, Boesch C, Oelze VM. Strontium isoscape of sub-Saharan Africa allows tracing origins of victims of the transatlantic slave trade. Nat Commun 2024; 15:10891. [PMID: 39738027 PMCID: PMC11685951 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-55256-0] [Show More Authors] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/07/2024] [Accepted: 12/04/2024] [Indexed: 01/01/2025] Open
Abstract
Strontium isotope (87Sr/86Sr) analysis with reference to strontium isotope landscapes (Sr isoscapes) allows reconstructing mobility and migration in archaeology, ecology, and forensics. However, despite the vast potential of research involving 87Sr/86Sr analysis particularly in Africa, Sr isoscapes remain unavailable for the largest parts of the continent. Here, we measure the 87Sr/86Sr ratios in 778 environmental samples from 24 African countries and combine this data with published data to model a bioavailable Sr isoscape for sub-Saharan Africa using random forest regression. We demonstrate the efficacy of this Sr isoscape, in combination with other lines of evidence, to trace the African roots of individuals from historic slavery contexts, particularly those with highly radiogenic 87Sr/86Sr ratios uncommon in the African Diaspora. Our study provides an extensive African 87Sr/86Sr dataset which includes scientifically marginalized regions of Africa, with significant implications for the archaeology of the transatlantic slave trade, wildlife ecology, conservation, and forensics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xueye Wang
- Center for Archaeological Science, Sichuan University, Chengdu, China
- Anthropology Department, University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA, USA
| | - Gaëlle Bocksberger
- Anthropology Department, University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA, USA
- Senckenberg Biodiversity and Climate Research Centre (SBiK-F), Frankfurt, Germany
| | - Mimi Arandjelovic
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Anthony Agbor
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Samuel Angedakin
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | | | - Emma Bailey
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | - Mattia Bessone
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - René Bobe
- Institute of Human Sciences, School of Anthropology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
- Gorongosa National Park, Sofala, Mozambique
| | - Matthieu Bonnet
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Renée Boucher
- Anthropology Department, University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA, USA
| | - Gregory Brazzola
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Simon Brewer
- Department of Geography, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
| | - Kevin C Lee
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Susana Carvalho
- Institute of Human Sciences, School of Anthropology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
- Gorongosa National Park, Sofala, Mozambique
| | - Rebecca Chancellor
- Departments of Anthropology and Sociology, West Chester University, West Chester, PA, USA
| | | | - Heather Cohen
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Sandi R Copeland
- Environmental Stewardship Group, Los Alamos National Laboratory, Los Alamos, NM, USA
| | | | - Ana Maria Costa
- Laboratório de Arqueociências (LARC)-DGPC and CIBIO | BIOPOLIS, 1300-418, Lisbon/ IDL - Instituto Dom Luiz, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal
| | | | - Bryan Curran
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Darryl J de Ruiter
- Department of Anthropology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA
| | - Tobias Deschner
- Comparative BioCognition, Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Osnabrück, Osnabrück, Germany
| | - Paula Dieguez
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Karsten Dierks
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | - Dervla Dowd
- Wild Chimpanzee Foundation, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Andrew Dunn
- Wildlife Conservation Society, Bronx, NY, USA
| | - Villard Ebot Egbe
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Manfred Finckh
- Ecological Modeling, Institute of Plant Science and Microbiology, University of Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
| | - Barbara Fruth
- Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Konstanz, Germany
| | - Liza Gijanto
- Department of Anthropology, St. Mary's College of Maryland, St. Mary's City, MD, USA
| | - Yisa Ginath Yuh
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | - Cameron Gokee
- Department of Anthropology, Appalachian State University, Boone, NC, USA
| | - Rui Gomes Coelho
- Department of Archaeology, Durham University, Durham, UK
- Centre for Archaeology, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Alan H Goodman
- School of Natural Science, Hampshire College, Amherst, MA, USA
| | | | - Vaughan Grimes
- Department of Archaeology, Memorial University, St. John's, Newfoundland, Canada
| | - Cyril C Grueter
- School of Human Sciences, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia, Australia
| | - Anne Haour
- Sainsbury Research Unit for the Arts of Africa, Oceania and the Americas, University of East Anglia, Norwich, United Kingdom
| | - Daniela Hedwig
- K. Lisa Yang Center for Conservation Bioacoustics, Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA
| | - Veerle Hermans
- Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - R Adriana Hernandez-Aguilar
- Department of Social Psychology and Quantitative Psychology, University of Barcelona, Serra Hunter Programme, Barcelona, Spain
- Jane Goodall Institute Spain and Senegal, Dindefelo Biological Station, Dindefelo, Kedougou, Senegal
| | - Gottfried Hohmann
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | - Kathryn J Jeffery
- School of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling, Scotland, UK
| | - Sorrel Jones
- RSPB Centre for Conservation Science, Royal Society for the Protection of Birds, The David Attenborough Building, Cambridge, UK
| | - Jessica Junker
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Parag Kadam
- Warnell School of Forestry and Natural Resources, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
| | - Mbangi Kambere
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Mohamed Kambi
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Ivonne Kienast
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Kelly J Knudson
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
| | - Kevin E Langergraber
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
| | | | - Juan Lapuente
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Bradley Larson
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Thea Lautenschläger
- Department of Biology, Institute of Botany, Faculty of Science, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
- Botanical Garden, Universität Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
| | - Petrus le Roux
- Department of Geological Sciences, University of Cape Town, Rondebosch, South Africa
| | | | - Manuel Llana
- Jane Goodall Institute Spain and Senegal, Dindefelo Biological Station, Dindefelo, Kedougou, Senegal
| | - Amanda Logan
- Department of Anthropology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
| | - Brynn Lowry
- Anthropology Department, University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA, USA
| | - Tina Lüdecke
- Emmy Noether Group for Hominin Meat Consumption, Max Planck Institute for Chemistry, Mainz, Germany
| | - Giovanna Maretti
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Sergio Marrocoli
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Rumen Fernandez
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Patricia J McNeill
- Department of Anthropology, University of California at Davis, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Amelia C Meier
- Hawai'i Institute of Marine Biology, University of Hawai'i at Manoa, Kane'ohe, HI, USA
| | - Paulina Meller
- Ecological Modeling, Institute of Plant Science and Microbiology, University of Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
| | - J Cameron Monroe
- Anthropology Department, University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA, USA
| | - David Morgan
- Lester E. Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes, Lincoln Park Zoo, Chicago, IL, USA
| | | | - Mizuki Murai
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Emily Neil
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
- School of Geography and the Environment, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Sonia Nicholl
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | | | - Lucy Jayne Ormsby
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Orume Diotoh
- Korup Rainforest Conservation Society, Korup National Park, Mundemba, SW Region, Cameroon
| | - Liliana Pacheco
- Save the Dogs and Other Animals, Cernavoda, Cernavoda, CT, Romania
| | - Alex Piel
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
- Department of Anthropology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Jodie Preece
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | | | | | - Aaron Rundus
- Departments of Anthropology and Sociology, West Chester University, West Chester, PA, USA
| | - Crickette Sanz
- Department of Anthropology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA
- Wildlife Conservation Society, Congo Program, Brazzaville, Republic of the Congo
| | - Volker Sommer
- Department of Anthropology, University College London, London, UK
- Gashaka Primate Project, Serti, Taraba, Nigeria
| | - Matt Sponheimer
- Department of Anthropology, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, USA
| | - Teresa E Steele
- Department of Anthropology, University of California at Davis, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Fiona A Stewart
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
- Department of Anthropology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Nikki Tagg
- Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Luc Roscelin Tédonzong
- Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Alexander Tickle
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | | | | | | | - Erin G Wessling
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
- University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, Scotland, UK
| | - Jacob Willie
- Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Roman M Wittig
- Institut des Sciences Cognitives, University of Lyon 1, Bron, France
- Tai Chimpanzee Project, Centre Suisse de Recherches Scientifiques, Abidjan, Côte d'Ivoire
| | - Kyle Yurkiw
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Andrew M Zipkin
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
| | - Klaus Zuberbühler
- Université de Neuchâtel, Institut de Biologie, Neuchâtel, Switzerland
| | - Hjalmar S Kühl
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv), Leipzig, Germany
- Senckenberg Museum for Natural History Görlitz, Senckenberg-Member of the Leibniz Association, Görlitz, Germany
- International Institute Zittau, Technische Universität Dresden, Zittau, Germany
| | - Christophe Boesch
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Vicky M Oelze
- Anthropology Department, University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Sandel AA, Scott JE, Kamilar JM. Primate Behavior and the Importance of Comparative Studies in Biological Anthropology. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF BIOLOGICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 2024; 186 Suppl 78:e70009. [PMID: 40071872 DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.70009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2024] [Revised: 11/06/2024] [Accepted: 12/21/2024] [Indexed: 04/02/2025]
Abstract
Biological anthropology seeks to understand humans from an evolutionary perspective. Namely, what makes humans different from other animals, and how did we get this way? Many relevant traits are physical, but many others are behavioral. For example, when and why did our species develop complex cognition, enduring bonds, and intense cooperation? Given the importance of behavior, biological anthropologists have a long history of turning to our primate relatives to generate hypotheses about the evolutionary processes shaping humans. Indeed, primate behavior is foundational to our field. But not all biological anthropologists appreciate the value of primate behavior for understanding human evolution. Beyond lip service in introductory paragraphs and grant proposals, many primatologists do not make explicit how their work is relevant to human evolution. In this review, we have three main goals: (1) emphasize how comparative studies of primate behavior are crucial to biological anthropology; (2) outline how primatologists and biological anthropologists can improve their work by avoiding common problems that arise when making such comparisons; and (3) provide a primer on the concepts and methods underlying comparative analyses of traits. We provide examples to highlight these points related to cognition, sociality, and diet. We conclude with several recommendations including (1) detailed, high-quality studies of behavior that allow for appropriate comparisons within and across species; (2) using primates as a "gateway clade" and expanding our research to any relevant taxa; and (3) careful attention to the ethical implications of making comparisons to other primates given racist tropes and a history of eugenics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron A Sandel
- Department of Anthropology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas, USA
| | - Jeremiah E Scott
- Department of Medical Anatomical Sciences, College of Osteopathic Medicine of the Pacific, Western University of Health Sciences, Pomona, California, USA
| | - Jason M Kamilar
- Department of Anthropology and Graduate Program in Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, University of Massachusetts Amherst, Amherst, Massachusetts, USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Gunasekaram C, Battiston F, Sadekar O, Padilla-Iglesias C, van Noordwijk MA, Furrer R, Manica A, Bertranpetit J, Whiten A, van Schaik CP, Vinicius L, Migliano AB. Population connectivity shapes the distribution and complexity of chimpanzee cumulative culture. Science 2024; 386:920-925. [PMID: 39571020 DOI: 10.1126/science.adk3381] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/28/2023] [Accepted: 09/23/2024] [Indexed: 04/18/2025]
Abstract
Although cumulative culture is a hallmark of hominin evolution, its origins can be traced back to our common ancestor with chimpanzees. Here, we investigated the evolutionary origins of chimpanzee cumulative culture and why it remained incipient. To trace cultural transmission among the four chimpanzee subspecies, we compared population networks based on genetic markers of recent migration and shared cultural traits. We show that limited levels of group connectivity favored the emergence of a few instances of cumulative culture in chimpanzees. As in humans, cultural complexification likely happened in steps, with transmission between populations, incremental changes, and repurposing of technologies. We propose that divergence in social patterns led to increased mobility between groups in the genus Homo, resulting in irreversible dependence on cultural exchange and complexification.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cassandra Gunasekaram
- Human Evolutionary Ecology Group, Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Federico Battiston
- Department of Network and Data Science, Central European University, Vienna, Austria
| | - Onkar Sadekar
- Department of Network and Data Science, Central European University, Vienna, Austria
| | - Cecilia Padilla-Iglesias
- Human Evolutionary Ecology Group, Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
- Emmanuel College, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Maria A van Noordwijk
- Human Evolutionary Ecology Group, Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
- Comparative Socioecology Group, Department for the Ecology of Animal Societies, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Konstanz, Germany
| | - Reinhard Furrer
- Department of Mathematical Modeling and Machine Learning, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Andrea Manica
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Jaume Bertranpetit
- Institut de Biologia Evolutiva, CSIC - Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Andrew Whiten
- Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, UK
| | - Carel P van Schaik
- Human Evolutionary Ecology Group, Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
- Comparative Socioecology Group, Department for the Ecology of Animal Societies, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Konstanz, Germany
- Department of Evolutionary Biology & Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Lucio Vinicius
- Human Evolutionary Ecology Group, Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Andrea Bamberg Migliano
- Human Evolutionary Ecology Group, Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Kunze J, Harvati K, Hotz G, Karakostis FA. Humanlike manual activities in Australopithecus. J Hum Evol 2024; 196:103591. [PMID: 39366305 DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2024.103591] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/15/2023] [Revised: 08/31/2024] [Accepted: 08/31/2024] [Indexed: 10/06/2024]
Abstract
The evolution of the human hand is a topic of great interest in paleoanthropology. As the hand can be involved in a vast array of activities, knowledge regarding how it was used by early hominins can yield crucial information on the factors driving biocultural evolution. Previous research on early hominin hands focused on the overall bone shape. However, while such approaches can inform on mechanical abilities and the evolved efficiency of manipulation, they cannot be used as a definite proxy for individual habitual activity. Accordingly, it is crucial to examine bone structures more responsive to lifetime biomechanical loading, such as muscle attachment sites or internal bone architecture. In this study, we investigate the manual entheseal patterns of Australopithecus afarensis, Australopithecus africanus, and Australopithecus sediba through the application of the validated entheses-based reconstruction of activity method. Using a comparative sample of later Homo and three great ape genera, we analyze the muscle attachment site proportions on the thumb, fifth ray, and third intermediate phalanx to gain insight into the habitual hand use of Australopithecus. We use a novel statistical procedure to account for the effects of interspecies variation in overall size and ray proportions. Our results highlight the importance of certain muscles of the first and fifth digits for humanlike hand use. In humans, these muscles are required for variable in-hand manipulation and are activated during stone-tool production. The entheses of A. sediba suggest muscle activation patterns consistent with a similar suite of habitual manual activities as in later Homo. In contrast, A. africanus and A. afarensis display a mosaic entheseal pattern that combines indications of both humanlike and apelike manipulation. Overall, these findings provide new evidence that some australopith species were already habitually engaging in humanlike manipulation, even if their manual dexterity was likely not as high as in later Homo.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jana Kunze
- Paleoanthropology, Senckenberg Centre for Human Evolution and Palaeoenvironment, Institute for Archaeological Sciences, Eberhard Karls University of Tübingen, Rümelinstraße 23, Tübingen D-72070, Germany.
| | - Katerina Harvati
- Paleoanthropology, Senckenberg Centre for Human Evolution and Palaeoenvironment, Institute for Archaeological Sciences, Eberhard Karls University of Tübingen, Rümelinstraße 23, Tübingen D-72070, Germany; DFG Centre of Advanced Studies 'Words, Bones, Genes, Tools', Eberhard Karls University of Tübingen, Rümelinstraße 23, Tübingen D-72070, Germany
| | - Gerhard Hotz
- Anthropological Collection, Natural History Museum Basel, Augustinergasse 2, Basel S-4051, Switzerland; Integrative Prehistory and Archaeological Science, University of Basel, Spalenring 145, Basel S-4055, Switzerland
| | - Fotios Alexandros Karakostis
- Paleoanthropology, Senckenberg Centre for Human Evolution and Palaeoenvironment, Institute for Archaeological Sciences, Eberhard Karls University of Tübingen, Rümelinstraße 23, Tübingen D-72070, Germany; DFG Centre of Advanced Studies 'Words, Bones, Genes, Tools', Eberhard Karls University of Tübingen, Rümelinstraße 23, Tübingen D-72070, Germany; Anthropological Collection, Natural History Museum Basel, Augustinergasse 2, Basel S-4051, Switzerland; Integrative Prehistory and Archaeological Science, University of Basel, Spalenring 145, Basel S-4055, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Ostridge HJ, Fontsere C, Lizano E, Soto DC, Schmidt JM, Saxena V, Alvarez-Estape M, Barratt CD, Gratton P, Bocksberger G, Lester JD, Dieguez P, Agbor A, Angedakin S, Assumang AK, Bailey E, Barubiyo D, Bessone M, Brazzola G, Chancellor R, Cohen H, Coupland C, Danquah E, Deschner T, Dotras L, Dupain J, Egbe VE, Granjon AC, Head J, Hedwig D, Hermans V, Hernandez-Aguilar RA, Jeffery KJ, Jones S, Junker J, Kadam P, Kaiser M, Kalan AK, Kambere M, Kienast I, Kujirakwinja D, Langergraber KE, Lapuente J, Larson B, Laudisoit A, Lee KC, Llana M, Maretti G, Martín R, Meier A, Morgan D, Neil E, Nicholl S, Nixon S, Normand E, Orbell C, Ormsby LJ, Orume R, Pacheco L, Preece J, Regnaut S, Robbins MM, Rundus A, Sanz C, Sciaky L, Sommer V, Stewart FA, Tagg N, Tédonzong LR, van Schijndel J, Vendras E, Wessling EG, Willie J, Wittig RM, Yuh YG, Yurkiw K, Vigilant L, Piel A, Boesch C, Kühl HS, Dennis MY, Marques-Bonet T, Arandjelovic M, Andrés AM. Local genetic adaptation to habitat in wild chimpanzees. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2024:2024.07.09.601734. [PMID: 39026872 PMCID: PMC11257515 DOI: 10.1101/2024.07.09.601734] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/20/2024]
Abstract
How populations adapt to their environment is a fundamental question in biology. Yet we know surprisingly little about this process, especially for endangered species such as non-human great apes. Chimpanzees, our closest living relatives, are particularly interesting because they inhabit diverse habitats, from rainforest to woodland-savannah. Whether genetic adaptation facilitates such habitat diversity remains unknown, despite having wide implications for evolutionary biology and conservation. Using 828 newly generated exomes from wild chimpanzees, we find evidence of fine-scale genetic adaptation to habitat. Notably, adaptation to malaria in forest chimpanzees is mediated by the same genes underlying adaptation to malaria in humans. This work demonstrates the power of non-invasive samples to reveal genetic adaptations in endangered populations and highlights the importance of adaptive genetic diversity for chimpanzees.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Harrison J Ostridge
- UCL Genetics Institute, Department of Genetics, Evolution and Environment, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Claudia Fontsere
- Center for Evolutionary Hologenomics, The Globe Institute, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
| | - Esther Lizano
- Institute of Evolutionary Biology (UPF-CSIC), PRBB, Dr. Aiguader 88, 08003 Barcelona, Spain
| | - Daniela C Soto
- University of California, Davis, Genome Center, MIND Institute, Department of Biochemistry & Molecular Medicine, One Shields Drive, Davis, CA, 95616, USA
| | - Joshua M Schmidt
- Flinders Health and Medical Research Institute (FHMRI), Department of Ophthalmology, Flinders University Sturt Rd, Bedford Park South Australia 5042 Australia
| | - Vrishti Saxena
- UCL Genetics Institute, Department of Genetics, Evolution and Environment, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Marina Alvarez-Estape
- University of California, Davis, Genome Center, MIND Institute, Department of Biochemistry & Molecular Medicine, One Shields Drive, Davis, CA, 95616, USA
| | - Christopher D Barratt
- Naturalis Biodiversity Center, Darwinweg 2, 2333 CR Leiden, the Netherlands
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv) Halle-Leipzig-Jena, Puschstrasse 4, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Paolo Gratton
- University of Rome "Tor Vergata" Department of Biology Via Cracovia, 1, Roma, Italia
| | - Gaëlle Bocksberger
- Senckenberg Biodiversity and Climate Research Centre (SBiK-F), Senckenberganlage, 60325 Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| | - Jack D Lester
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Paula Dieguez
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv) Halle-Leipzig-Jena, Puschstrasse 4, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Anthony Agbor
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Samuel Angedakin
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Alfred Kwabena Assumang
- Department of Wildlife and Range Management, Faculty of Renewable Natural Resources, Kwame Nkrumah University of Science and Technology, Kumasi, Ghana
| | - Emma Bailey
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Donatienne Barubiyo
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Mattia Bessone
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
- University of Konstanz, Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, Universitätsstraße 10, 78464, Konstanz, Germany
| | - Gregory Brazzola
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Rebecca Chancellor
- West Chester University, Depts of Anthropology & Sociology and Psychology, West Chester, PA, 19382 USA
| | - Heather Cohen
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv) Halle-Leipzig-Jena, Puschstrasse 4, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Charlotte Coupland
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Emmanuel Danquah
- Department of Wildlife and Range Management, Faculty of Renewable Natural Resources, Kwame Nkrumah University of Science and Technology, Kumasi, Ghana
| | - Tobias Deschner
- Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Osnabrück, Artilleriestrasse 34, 49076 Osnabrück, Germany
| | - Laia Dotras
- Jane Goodall Institute Spain and Senegal, Dindefelo Biological Station, Dindefelo, Kedougou, Senegal
- Department of Social Psychology and Quantitative Psychology, Serra Hunter Programme, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Jef Dupain
- Antwerp Zoo Foundation, RZSA, Kon.Astridplein 26, 2018 Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Villard Ebot Egbe
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Anne-Céline Granjon
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Josephine Head
- The Biodiversity Consultancy, 3E Kings Parade, Cambridge, CB2 1SJ, UK
| | - Daniela Hedwig
- Elephant Listening Project, K. Lisa Yang Center for Conservation Bioacoustics, Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Cornell University, 159 Sapsucker Woods Road, Ithaca, NY 14850, USA
| | - Veerle Hermans
- KMDA, Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Koningin Astridplein 20-26, B-2018 Antwerp, Belgium
| | - R Adriana Hernandez-Aguilar
- Jane Goodall Institute Spain and Senegal, Dindefelo Biological Station, Dindefelo, Kedougou, Senegal
- Department of Social Psychology and Quantitative Psychology, Serra Hunter Programme, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Kathryn J Jeffery
- School of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, UK
- Agence National des Parcs Nationaux (ANPN) Batterie 4, BP20379, Libreville, Gabon
| | - Sorrel Jones
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Jessica Junker
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv) Halle-Leipzig-Jena, Puschstrasse 4, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Parag Kadam
- Greater Mahale Ecosystem Research and Conservation Project
| | - Michael Kaiser
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Ammie K Kalan
- Department of Anthropology, University of Victoria, 3800 Finnerty Rd, Victoria, BC V8P 5C2, Canada
| | - Mbangi Kambere
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Ivonne Kienast
- Department of Natural Resources and the Environment, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14850, USA
- K. Lisa Yang Center for Conservation Bioacoustics, Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14850, USA
| | - Deo Kujirakwinja
- Wildlife Conservation Society (WCS), 2300 Southern Boulevard. Bronx, New York 10460, USA
| | - Kevin E Langergraber
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, 777 East University Drive, Tempe, AZ 85287 Arizona State University, PO Box 872402, Tempe, AZ 85287-2402 USA
- Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, 900 Cady Mall, Tempe, AZ 85287 Arizona State University, PO Box 872402, Tempe, AZ 85287-2402 USA
| | - Juan Lapuente
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | | | | | - Kevin C Lee
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
- K. Lisa Yang Center for Conservation Bioacoustics, Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14850, USA
| | - Manuel Llana
- Jane Goodall Institute Spain and Senegal, Dindefelo Biological Station, Dindefelo, Kedougou, Senegal
| | - Giovanna Maretti
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Rumen Martín
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Amelia Meier
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
- Hawai'i Insititute of Marine Biology, University of Hawai'i at Manoa, 46-007 Lilipuna Place, Kaneohe, HI, 96744, USA
| | - David Morgan
- Lester E. Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes, Lincoln Park Zoo, 2001 North Clark Street, Chicago, Illinois 60614 USA
| | - Emily Neil
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Sonia Nicholl
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Stuart Nixon
- North of England Zoological Society, Chester Zoo, Upton by Chester, CH2 1LH, United Kingdom
| | | | - Christopher Orbell
- Panthera, 8 W 40TH ST, New York, NY 10018, USA
- School of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, UK
| | - Lucy Jayne Ormsby
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Robinson Orume
- Korup Rainforest Conservation Society, c/o Korup National Park, P.O. Box 36 Mundemba, South West Region, Cameroon
| | - Liliana Pacheco
- Save the Dogs and Other Animals, DJ 223 Km 3, 905200 Cernavoda CT, Romania
| | - Jodie Preece
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | | | - Martha M Robbins
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Department of Primate Behavior and Evolution, Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Aaron Rundus
- West Chester University, Depts of Anthropology & Sociology and Psychology, West Chester, PA, 19382 USA
| | - Crickette Sanz
- Washington University in Saint Louis, Department of Anthropology, One Brookings Drive, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA
- Congo Program, Wildlife Conservation Society, 151 Avenue Charles de Gaulle, Brazzaville, Republic of Congo
| | - Lilah Sciaky
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Volker Sommer
- University College London, Department of Anthropology, 14 Taviton Street, London WC1H 0BW, UK
| | - Fiona A Stewart
- University College London, Department of Anthropology, 14 Taviton Street, London WC1H 0BW, UK
- Department of Human Origins, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Nikki Tagg
- KMDA, Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Koningin Astridplein 20-26, B-2018 Antwerp, Belgium
- Born Free Foundation, Floor 2 Frazer House, 14 Carfax, Horsham, RH12 1ER, UK
| | - Luc Roscelin Tédonzong
- KMDA, Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Koningin Astridplein 20-26, B-2018 Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Joost van Schijndel
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Elleni Vendras
- Frankfurt Zoological Society, Bernhard-Grzimek-Allee 1, 60316 Frankfurt, Germany
| | - Erin G Wessling
- Johann-Friedrich-Blumenbach Institute for Zoology and Anthropology, Georg-August-University Göttingen,Göttingen, Germany
- German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Göttingen, Germany
| | - Jacob Willie
- KMDA, Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Koningin Astridplein 20-26, B-2018 Antwerp, Belgium
- Terrestrial Ecology Unit (TEREC), Department of Biology, Ghent University (UGent), K.L. Ledeganckstraat 35, 9000 Ghent, Belgium
| | - Roman M Wittig
- Ape Social Mind Lab, Institute for Cognitive Sciences Marc Jeannerod, CNRS UMR 5229 CNRS, 67 bd Pinel, 69675 Bron CEDEX, France
- Taï Chimpanzee Project, Centre Suisse de Recherches Scientifiques, BP 1301, Abidjan 01, CI
| | - Yisa Ginath Yuh
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Kyle Yurkiw
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Linda Vigilant
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
| | - Alex Piel
- University College London, Department of Anthropology, 14 Taviton Street, London WC1H 0BW, UK
| | | | - Hjalmar S Kühl
- Senckenberg Museum for Natural History Görlitz, Senckenberg - Member of the Leibniz Association Am Museum 1, 02826 Görlitz, Germany
- International Institute Zittau, Technische Universität Dresden, Markt 23, 02763 Zittau, Germany
| | - Megan Y Dennis
- University of California, Davis, Genome Center, MIND Institute, Department of Biochemistry & Molecular Medicine, One Shields Drive, Davis, CA, 95616, USA
| | - Tomas Marques-Bonet
- Institute of Evolutionary Biology (UPF-CSIC), PRBB, Dr. Aiguader 88, 08003 Barcelona, Spain
- Catalan Institution of Research and Advanced Studies (ICREA), Passeig de Lluís Companys, 23, 08010, Barcelona, Spain
- CNAG-CRG, Centre for Genomic Regulation (CRG), Barcelona Institute of Science and Technology (BIST), Baldiri i Reixac 4, 08028 Barcelona, Spain
- Institut Català de Paleontologia Miquel Crusafont, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Edifici ICTA-ICP, c/ Columnes s/n, 08193 Cerdanyola del Vallès, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Mimi Arandjelovic
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Department of Primate Behavior and Evolution, Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv) Halle-Jena-Leipzig, Puschstrasse 4, 04103
| | - Aida M Andrés
- UCL Genetics Institute, Department of Genetics, Evolution and Environment, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Pashchevskaya S, Fruth B, Hohmann G. Water scooping: tool use by a wild bonobo (Pan paniscus) at LuiKotale, a case report. Primates 2024; 65:145-150. [PMID: 38488904 PMCID: PMC11018684 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-024-01121-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2023] [Accepted: 02/12/2024] [Indexed: 03/17/2024]
Abstract
Tool use diversity is often considered to differentiate our two closest living relatives: the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and the bonobo (P. paniscus). Chimpanzees appear to have the largest repertoire of tools amongst nonhuman primates, and in this species, many forms of tool use enhance food and water acquisition. In captivity, bonobos seem as adept as chimpanzees in tool use complexity, including in the foraging context. However, in the wild, bonobos have only been observed engaging in habitual tool use in the contexts of comfort, play, self-directed behaviour and communication, whilst no tool-assisted food acquisition has been reported. Whereas captive bonobos use tools for drinking, so far, the only report from the wild populations comes down to four observations of moss sponges used at Lomako. Here, we present the first report of tool use in the form of water scooping by a wild bonobo at LuiKotale. An adult female was observed and videotaped whilst using an emptied Cola chlamydantha pod to scoop and drink water from a stream. We discuss the conditions for such observations and the importance of looking out for rare behaviours and attempt to put the observation into the context of the opportunity versus necessity hypotheses. By adding novel information on tool use, our report contributes to the ongoing efforts to differentiate population-specific traits in the behavioural ecology of the bonobo.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sonya Pashchevskaya
- Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Constance, DE, Germany.
- Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium.
| | - Barbara Fruth
- Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Constance, DE, Germany
- Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Sakurai Y, Ikeda Y. Effect of visual lateralization on the spatial position of individuals within a school of oval squid (Sepioteuthis lessoniana). J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2024; 210:381-398. [PMID: 37515730 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01654-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/10/2023] [Revised: 06/23/2023] [Accepted: 06/29/2023] [Indexed: 07/31/2023]
Abstract
The spatial position of individuals within a social group, which provides the group members with benefits and costs, is determined by several physical and physiological factors. Lateralization (left and right asymmetry of morphology and behavior) could also be factors determining the individual's positions within a group. However, this possibility has been documented in some fish species, but never in an invertebrate species. This study investigates the association between spatial positions and lateralization in oval squid, Sepioteuthis lessoniana, which displays social behavior, such as schooling and lateralization for eye use (visual lateralization). The direction and strength of visual lateralization were determined for single squid by observing which eye was used to detect the prey, predators, and conspecifics. The spatial positions of individuals were determined by identifying whether the squids were in the left or right side from the center of the school. When the prey was presented to schooling squids, strongly lateralized squids against prey positioned themselves on the right side, whereas weakly lateralized squids positioned themselves on the left side. When the predator was presented to squids, the strongly lateralized squids against the conspecifics positioned themselves on the right side, and the weakly lateralized squids positioned themselves on the left side. When no targets were presented, the strongly lateralized squids against the predator positioned themselves on the right side, whereas the weakly lateralized squids positioned themselves on the left side. The strength of visual lateralization of oval squid could offer the defensive and offensive functions of schools with specific individual positions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuma Sakurai
- Department of Marine and Environmental Sciences, Graduate School of Engineering and Science, University of the Ryukyus, Senbaru, Nishihara, Okinawa, 903-0213, Japan
- Department of Biological Sciences, Faculty of Science, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, 060-0810, Japan
| | - Yuzuru Ikeda
- Department of Chemistry, Biology and Marine Science, Faculty of Science, University of the Ryukyus, Senbaru, Nishihara, Okinawa, 903-0213, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
van Leeuwen EJC, DeTroy SE, Haun DBM, Call J. Chimpanzees use social information to acquire a skill they fail to innovate. Nat Hum Behav 2024; 8:891-902. [PMID: 38448718 PMCID: PMC11132989 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-024-01836-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/12/2023] [Accepted: 01/22/2024] [Indexed: 03/08/2024]
Abstract
Cumulative cultural evolution has been claimed to be a uniquely human phenomenon pivotal to the biological success of our species. One plausible condition for cumulative cultural evolution to emerge is individuals' ability to use social learning to acquire know-how that they cannot easily innovate by themselves. It has been suggested that chimpanzees may be capable of such know-how social learning, but this assertion remains largely untested. Here we show that chimpanzees use social learning to acquire a skill that they failed to independently innovate. By teaching chimpanzees how to solve a sequential task (one chimpanzee in each of the two tested groups, n = 66) and using network-based diffusion analysis, we found that 14 naive chimpanzees learned to operate a puzzle box that they failed to operate during the preceding three months of exposure to all necessary materials. In conjunction, we present evidence for the hypothesis that social learning in chimpanzees is necessary and sufficient to acquire a new, complex skill after the initial innovation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Edwin J C van Leeuwen
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Department of Biology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands.
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
- Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium.
| | - Sarah E DeTroy
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Daniel B M Haun
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Josep Call
- School of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, UK
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Pascual-Garrido A, Carvalho S, Almeida-Warren K. Primate archaeology 3.0. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF BIOLOGICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 2024; 183:e24835. [PMID: 37671610 DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.24835] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2023] [Revised: 07/25/2023] [Accepted: 08/03/2023] [Indexed: 09/07/2023]
Abstract
The new field of primate archaeology investigates the technological behavior and material record of nonhuman primates, providing valuable comparative data on our understanding of human technological evolution. Yet, paralleling hominin archaeology, the field is largely biased toward the analysis of lithic artifacts. While valuable comparative data have been gained through an examination of extant nonhuman primate tool use and its archaeological record, focusing on this one single aspect provides limited insights. It is therefore necessary to explore to what extent other non-technological activities, such as non-tool aided feeding, traveling, social behaviors or ritual displays, leave traces that could be detected in the archaeological record. Here we propose four new areas of investigation which we believe have been largely overlooked by primate archaeology and that are crucial to uncovering the full archaeological potential of the primate behavioral repertoire, including that of our own: (1) Plant technology; (2) Archaeology beyond technology; (3) Landscape archaeology; and (4) Primate cultural heritage. We discuss each theme in the context of the latest developments and challenges, as well as propose future directions. Developing a more "inclusive" primate archaeology will not only benefit the study of primate evolution in its own right but will aid conservation efforts by increasing our understanding of changes in primate-environment interactions over time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alejandra Pascual-Garrido
- Primate Models for Behavioural Evolution Lab, Institute of Human Sciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Susana Carvalho
- Primate Models for Behavioural Evolution Lab, Institute of Human Sciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
- Interdisciplinary Centre for Archaeology and the Evolution of Human Behaviour, University of Algarve, Faro, Portugal
- Gorongosa National Park, Sofala, Mozambique
| | - Katarina Almeida-Warren
- Primate Models for Behavioural Evolution Lab, Institute of Human Sciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
- Interdisciplinary Centre for Archaeology and the Evolution of Human Behaviour, University of Algarve, Faro, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Hobaiter C, Klein H, Gruber T. Habitual ground nesting in the Bugoma Forest chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii), Uganda. Am J Primatol 2024; 86:e23583. [PMID: 38037523 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23583] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2023] [Revised: 11/05/2023] [Accepted: 11/24/2023] [Indexed: 12/02/2023]
Abstract
We report the presence of habitual ground nesting in a newly studied East African chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) population in the Bugoma Central Forest Reserve, Uganda. Across a 2-year period, we encountered 891 night nests, 189 of which were classified as ground nests, a rate of ~21%. We find no preliminary evidence of socio-ecological factors that would promote its use and highlight local factors, such as high incidence of forest disturbance due to poaching and logging, which appear to make its use disadvantageous. While further study is required to establish whether this behavior meets the strict criteria for nonhuman animal culture, we support the argument that the wider use of population and group-specific behavioral repertoires in flagship species, such as chimpanzees, offers a tool to promote the urgent conservation action needed to protect threatened ecosystems, including the Bugoma forest.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Catherine Hobaiter
- Wild Minds Lab, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, UK
- Bugoma Primate Conservation Project, Bugoma Central Forest Reserve, Hoima, Uganda
| | - Harmonie Klein
- Wild Minds Lab, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, UK
| | - Thibaud Gruber
- Bugoma Primate Conservation Project, Bugoma Central Forest Reserve, Hoima, Uganda
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences and Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Lima T, Fam B, Tavares GM, Falótico T, Cantele C, Fanti L, Landau L, Viscardi LH, Vargas-Pinilla P, Barrientos-Diaz O, Pissinatti A, Sortica VA, Ottoni EB, Segatto ALA, Turchetto-Zolet AC, Bortolini MC. Insights into the evolutionary history of the most skilled tool-handling platyrrhini monkey: Sapajus libidinosus from the Serra da Capivara National Park. Genet Mol Biol 2023; 46:e20230165. [PMID: 37948505 PMCID: PMC10637428 DOI: 10.1590/1678-4685-gmb-2023-0165] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2023] [Accepted: 10/08/2023] [Indexed: 11/12/2023] Open
Abstract
Sapajus libidinosus members of the Pedra Furada group, living in the Serra da Capivara National Park, use stone tools in a wider variety of behaviors than any other living animal, except humans. To rescue the evolutionary history of the Caatinga S. libidinosus and identify factors that may have contributed to the emergence and maintenance of their tool-use culture, we conducted fieldwork seasons to obtain biological samples of these capuchin monkeys. UsingCYTBsequences, we show a discrete but constant population growth from the beginning of the Holocene to the present, overlapping the emergence of the Caatinga biome. Our habitat suitability reconstruction reports the presence of plants whose hard fruits, seeds, or roots are processed by capuchins using tools. TheS. libidinosusindividuals in the Caatinga were capable of dynamically developing and maintaining their autochthonous culture thanks to: a) cognitive capacity to generate and execute innovation under selective pressure; b) tolerance favoring learning and cultural inheritance; c) an unknown genetic repertoire that underpins the adaptive traits; d) a high degree of terrestriality; e) presence and abundance of natural resources, which makes some places "hot spots" for innovation, and cultural diversification within a relatively short time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thaynara Lima
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Departamento de Genética,
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Genética e Biologia Molecular (PPGBM), Instituto de
Biociências, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
| | - Bibiana Fam
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Departamento de Genética,
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Genética e Biologia Molecular (PPGBM), Instituto de
Biociências, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
| | - Gustavo Medina Tavares
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Departamento de Genética,
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Genética e Biologia Molecular (PPGBM), Instituto de
Biociências, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
| | - Tiago Falótico
- Universidade de São Paulo, Escola de Artes, Ciências e Humanidades,
São Paulo, SP, Brazil
| | - Camila Cantele
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Departamento de Genética,
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Genética e Biologia Molecular (PPGBM), Instituto de
Biociências, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
| | - Lucca Fanti
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Departamento de Genética,
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Genética e Biologia Molecular (PPGBM), Instituto de
Biociências, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
| | - Luane Landau
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Departamento de Genética,
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Genética e Biologia Molecular (PPGBM), Instituto de
Biociências, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
| | - Lucas Henriques Viscardi
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Departamento de Genética,
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Genética e Biologia Molecular (PPGBM), Instituto de
Biociências, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
- Universidade Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul,
Escola de Medicina, Programa de Pós-Graduação em Medicina e Ciências da Saúde, Porto
Alegre, RS, Brazil
| | - Pedro Vargas-Pinilla
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Departamento de Genética,
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Genética e Biologia Molecular (PPGBM), Instituto de
Biociências, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
- Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade de Medicina de Ribeirão Preto,
Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brazil
| | - Ossman Barrientos-Diaz
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Departamento de Genética,
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Genética e Biologia Molecular (PPGBM), Instituto de
Biociências, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
| | | | - Vinicius A. Sortica
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Departamento de Genética,
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Genética e Biologia Molecular (PPGBM), Instituto de
Biociências, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
- Universidade de São Paulo, Medicina Preventiva da Faculdade de
Medicina, São Paulo, SP, Brazil
| | - Eduardo B. Ottoni
- Universidade de São Paulo, Instituto de Psicologia, Departamento de
Psicologia Experimental, São Paulo, SP, Brazil
| | - Ana Lúcia A. Segatto
- Universidade Federal de Santa Maria, Centro de Ciências Naturais e
Exatas, Departamento de Bioquímica e Biologia Molecular, Santa Maria, RS,
Brazil
| | - Andreia Carina Turchetto-Zolet
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Departamento de Genética,
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Genética e Biologia Molecular (PPGBM), Instituto de
Biociências, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
| | - Maria Cátira Bortolini
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Departamento de Genética,
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Genética e Biologia Molecular (PPGBM), Instituto de
Biociências, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Smolla M, Akçay E. Pathways to cultural adaptation: the coevolution of cumulative culture and social networks. EVOLUTIONARY HUMAN SCIENCES 2023; 5:e26. [PMID: 37829290 PMCID: PMC10565192 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2023.21] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2023] [Revised: 06/22/2023] [Accepted: 06/29/2023] [Indexed: 10/14/2023] Open
Abstract
Humans have adapted to an immense array of environments by accumulating culturally transmitted knowledge and skills. Adaptive culture can accumulate either via more distinct cultural traits or via improvements of existing cultural traits. The kind of culture that accumulates depends on, and coevolves with, the social structure of societies. Here, we show that the coevolution of learning networks and cumulative culture results in two distinct pathways to cultural adaptation: highly connected populations with high proficiency but low trait diversity vs. sparsely connected populations with low proficiency but higher trait diversity. Importantly, we show there is a conflict between group-level payoffs, which are maximised in highly connected groups that attain high proficiency, and individual level selection, which favours disconnection. This conflict emerges from the interaction of social learning with population structure and causes populations to cycle between the two cultural and network states. The same conflict creates a paradox where increasing innovation rate lowers group payoffs. Finally, we explore how populations navigate these two pathways in environments where payoffs differ among traits and can change over time, showing that high proficiency is favoured when payoffs are stable and vary strongly between traits, while frequent changes in trait payoffs favour more trait diversity. Our results illustrate the complex interplay between networks, learning and the environment, and so inform our understanding of human social evolution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marco Smolla
- Department of Biology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
- Department of Human Behaviour, Ecology and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Erol Akçay
- Department of Biology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Robbins MM, Akantorana M, Arinaitwe J, Breuer T, Manguette M, McFarlin S, Meder A, Parnell R, Richardson JL, Stephan C, Stokes EJ, Stoinski TS, Vecellio V, Robbins AM. Comparative life history patterns of female gorillas. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF BIOLOGICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 2023; 181:564-574. [PMID: 37345324 DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.24792] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2022] [Revised: 03/16/2023] [Accepted: 05/24/2023] [Indexed: 06/23/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Several theories have been proposed to explain the impact of ecological conditions on differences in life history variables within and between species. Here we compare female life history parameters of one western lowland gorilla population (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and two mountain gorilla populations (Gorilla beringei beringei). MATERIALS AND METHODS We compared the age of natal dispersal, age of first birth, interbirth interval, and birth rates using long-term demographic datasets from Mbeli Bai (western gorillas), Bwindi Impenetrable National Park and the Virunga Massif (mountain gorillas). RESULTS The Mbeli western gorillas had the latest age at first birth, longest interbirth interval, and slowest surviving birth rate compared to the Virunga mountain gorillas. Bwindi mountain gorillas were intermediate in their life history patterns. DISCUSSION These patterns are consistent with differences in feeding ecology across sites. However, it is not possible to determine the evolutionary mechanisms responsible for these differences, whether a consequence of genetic adaptation to fluctuating food supplies ("ecological risk aversion hypothesis") or phenotypic plasticity in response to the abundance of food ("energy balance hypothesis"). Our results do not seem consistent with the extrinsic mortality risks at each site, but current conditions for mountain gorillas are unlikely to match their evolutionary history. Not all traits fell along the expected fast-slow continuum, which illustrates that they can vary independently from each other ("modularity model"). Thus, the life history traits of each gorilla population may reflect a complex interplay of multiple ecological influences that are operating through both genetic adaptations and phenotypic plasticity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martha M Robbins
- Department of Primate Behavior and Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Moses Akantorana
- Department of Primate Behavior and Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | - Thomas Breuer
- Mbeli Bai Study, Wildlife Conservation Society Congo Program, Brazzaville, Democratic Republic of the Congo
- World Wide Fund for Nature, Berlin, Germany
| | - Marie Manguette
- Mbeli Bai Study, Wildlife Conservation Society Congo Program, Brazzaville, Democratic Republic of the Congo
| | - Shannon McFarlin
- Department of Anthropology, Center for the Advanced Study of Human Paleobiology, The George Washington University, Washington, DC, USA
| | - Angela Meder
- Berggorilla Regenwald Direkthilfe, Hoevelhof, Germany
| | - Richard Parnell
- Mbeli Bai Study, Wildlife Conservation Society Congo Program, Brazzaville, Democratic Republic of the Congo
| | - Jack L Richardson
- Department of Anthropology, Center for the Advanced Study of Human Paleobiology, The George Washington University, Washington, DC, USA
| | - Claudia Stephan
- Mbeli Bai Study, Wildlife Conservation Society Congo Program, Brazzaville, Democratic Republic of the Congo
- Nouabalé-Ndoki Foundation, Brazzaville, Democratic Republic of the Congo
| | - Emma J Stokes
- Mbeli Bai Study, Wildlife Conservation Society Congo Program, Brazzaville, Democratic Republic of the Congo
| | - Tara S Stoinski
- Dian Fossey Gorilla Fund International, Atlanta, Georgia, USA
| | | | - Andrew M Robbins
- Department of Primate Behavior and Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Harmand S, Arroyo A. Linking primatology and archaeology: The transversality of stone percussive behaviors. J Hum Evol 2023; 181:103398. [PMID: 37329870 DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2023.103398] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2022] [Revised: 05/09/2023] [Accepted: 05/09/2023] [Indexed: 06/19/2023]
Abstract
Since the launch of the Journal of Human Evolution fifty years ago, the archaeology of human origins and the evolution of culture have witnessed major breakthroughs with the identification of several new archaeological sites whose chronology has been slowly pushed back until the discovery of the earliest evidence of stone tool making at Lomekwi 3 (West Turkana, Kenya), at 3.3 Ma. Parallel to these discoveries, the study of wild primates, especially chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), allowed the development of models to understand key aspects of the behavior of extinct hominin species. Indeed, chimpanzees possess an impressive diversity of tool-aided foraging behaviors, demonstrating that technology (and culture) is not exclusive to humans. Additionally, current research has also shown that wild capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) and long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) also rely on stone percussive foraging behaviors. The investigation of these primates is boosting new interpretative models to understand the origins of stone flaking and the archaeological signature left by these primates. This review aims to present an examination of the state-of-the-art and the current advances made in the study of the earliest hominin technology and primate percussive behaviors. Overall, we argue that while it has been shown that extant primates can generate unintentional flakes, early hominins exhibited skills in the production and use of flakes not identified in primates. Nonetheless, we stand up to continue developing interdisciplinary approaches (i.e., primate archaeology) to study extant primates, as these endeavors are essential to move forward toward a detailed understanding of the technological foraging behaviors beyond the genus Homo. Finally, we discuss future challenges for the study of the emergence of stone technology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sonia Harmand
- Turkana Basin Institute, Department of Anthropology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, 11794-4364, USA; TRACES (Travaux et Recherches Archéologiques sur les Cultures, les Espaces et les Sociétés), UMR 5608 of the CNRS, Jean Jaurès University, Toulouse, 31058, France; IFRA Nairobi, Institut Français de Recherche en Afrique, UMIFRE, USR, 3336, CNRS, Kenya.
| | - Adrián Arroyo
- Institut Català de Paleoecologia Humana i Evolució Social (IPHES-CERCA), Zona Educacional 4, Campus Sescelades URV (Edifici W3), 43007, Tarragona, Spain; Universitat Rovira i Virgili (URV), Àrea de Prehistòria, Avinguda de Catalunya 35, 43002, Tarragona, Spain.
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Klein H. Book Review: The Chimpanzees of Rubondo Island: Apes Set Free. By Josephine Nadezda Msindai and Volker Sommer. Routledge, 2023, 237 pp. INT J PRIMATOL 2023. [DOI: 10.1007/s10764-023-00358-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/31/2023]
|
27
|
Fotang C, Bröring U, Roos C, Dutton P, Tédonzong LRD, Willie J, Angwafo TE, Yuh YG, Schierack P, Birkhofer K. Mapping suitable habitat for Nigeria-Cameroon chimpanzees in Kom-Wum Forest Reserve, North-Western Cameroon. Primates 2023; 64:339-350. [PMID: 36808317 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-023-01054-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2021] [Accepted: 01/29/2023] [Indexed: 02/21/2023]
Abstract
Great apes lose suitable habitats required for their reproduction and survival due to human activities across their distribution range in Africa. Little is known about habitat suitability of the Nigeria-Cameroon chimpanzee [Pan troglodytes ellioti (Matschie, 1914)], particularly for populations inhabiting forest reserves in North-West Cameroon. To address this knowledge gap, we employed a common species distribution model (MaxEnt) to map and predict suitable habitats for the Nigeria-Cameroon chimpanzee in Kom-Wum Forest Reserve, North-West Cameroon, based on environmental factors that potentially affect habitat suitability. We related these environmental factors to a dataset of chimpanzee occurrence points recorded during line transect and reconnaissance (recce) surveys in the forest reserve and surrounding forests. Up to 91% of the study area is unsuitable for chimpanzees. Suitable habitats only represented 9% of the study area, with a high proportion of highly suitable habitats located outside the forest reserve. Elevation, secondary forests density, distance to villages and primary forests density were the most important predictors of habitat suitability for the Nigeria-Cameroon chimpanzee. The probability of chimpanzee occurrence increased with elevation, secondary forest density and distance from villages and roads. Our study provides evidence that suitable chimpanzee habitat in the reserve is degraded, suggesting that efforts to maintain protected areas are insufficient. The reserve management plan needs to be improved to conserve the remaining suitable habitat and to avoid local extinction of this endangered subspecies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chefor Fotang
- Department of Ecology, Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg, Konrad-Wachsmann-Allee 6, 03046, Cottbus, Germany.
| | - Udo Bröring
- Department of Ecology, Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg, Konrad-Wachsmann-Allee 6, 03046, Cottbus, Germany
| | - Christian Roos
- German Primate Center, Gene Bank of Primates and Primate Genetics Laboratory, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Göttingen, Germany
| | | | | | - Jacob Willie
- Centre for Research and Conservation (CRC), Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp (RZSA), Antwerp, Belgium
- Terrestrial Ecology Unit (TEREC), Department of Biology, Ghent University (UGent), Ghent, Belgium
| | - Tsi Evaristus Angwafo
- Faculty of Agronomy and Agricultural Sciences (FASA), University of Dschang, Dschang, Cameroon
| | - Yisa Ginath Yuh
- Hochschule fur nachhaltige Entwicklung Eberswalde, Forestry and Environment, Schicklerstraße 5, 16225, Eberswalde, Germany
- Szkola Glowna Gospodarstwa Wiejskiego, Nowoursynowska 166, 02-787, Warsaw, Poland
- Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Peter Schierack
- Faculty of Environment and Natural Sciences, Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg, Senftenberg, Germany
| | - Klaus Birkhofer
- Department of Ecology, Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg, Konrad-Wachsmann-Allee 6, 03046, Cottbus, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Goldsborough Z, Schel AM, van Leeuwen EJC. Chimpanzees communicate to coordinate a cultural practice. Proc Biol Sci 2023; 290:20221754. [PMID: 36651045 PMCID: PMC9845976 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2022.1754] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/19/2023] Open
Abstract
Human culture thrives by virtue of communication, yet whether communication plays an influential role in the cultural lives of other animals remains understudied. Here, we investigated whether chimpanzees use communication to engage in a cultural practice by analysing grooming handclasp (GHC) interactions-a socio-cultural behaviour requiring interindividual coordination for successful execution. Previous accounts attributed GHC initiations to behavioural shaping, whereby the initiator physically moulds the partner's arm into the desired GHC posture. Using frame-by-frame analysis and matched-control methodology, we find that chimpanzees do not only shape their partner's posture (22%), but also use gestural communication to initiate GHC (44%), which requires an active and synchronized response from the partner. Moreover, in a third (34%) of the GHC initiations, the requisite coordination was achieved by seemingly effortless synchrony. Lastly, using a longitudinal approach, we find that for GHC initiations, communication occurs more frequently than shaping in experienced dyads and less in mother-offspring dyads. These findings are consistent with ontogenetic ritualization, thereby reflecting first documentation of chimpanzees communicating to coordinate a cultural practice. We conclude that chimpanzees show interactional flexibility in the socio-cultural domain, opening the possibility that the interplay between communication and culture is rooted in our deep evolutionary history.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zoë Goldsborough
- Department for the Ecology of Animal Societies, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Bücklestraße 5a, Konstanz, 78467, Germany,Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Department of Biology, Utrecht University, Padualaan 8, Utrecht, CA 3584, The Netherlands,Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, Universitätsstraße 10, Konstanz 78464, Germany
| | - Anne Marijke Schel
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Department of Biology, Utrecht University, Padualaan 8, Utrecht, CA 3584, The Netherlands
| | - Edwin J. C. van Leeuwen
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Department of Biology, Utrecht University, Padualaan 8, Utrecht, CA 3584, The Netherlands,Behavioral Ecology and Ecophysiology Group, Department of Biology, University of Antwerp, Universiteitsplein 1, 2610 Wilrijk, Belgium,Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, K. Astridplein 26, B 2018 Antwerp, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Wessling EG, Surbeck M. Failure to account for behavioral variability significantly compromises accuracy in indirect population monitoring. Anim Conserv 2022. [DOI: 10.1111/acv.12844] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- E. G. Wessling
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology Harvard University Cambridge MA USA
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience University of St Andrews St Andrews UK
| | - M. Surbeck
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology Harvard University Cambridge MA USA
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Leipzig Germany
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
De Meester G, Van Linden L, Torfs J, Pafilis P, Šunje E, Steenssens D, Zulčić T, Sassalos A, Van Damme R. Learning with lacertids: Studying the link between ecology and cognition within a comparative framework. Evolution 2022; 76:2531-2552. [PMID: 36111365 DOI: 10.1111/evo.14618] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/03/2022] [Revised: 08/10/2022] [Accepted: 08/21/2022] [Indexed: 01/22/2023]
Abstract
Cognition is an essential tool for animals to deal with environmental challenges. Nonetheless, the ecological forces driving the evolution of cognition throughout the animal kingdom remain enigmatic. Large-scale comparative studies on multiple species and cognitive traits have been advanced as the best way to facilitate our understanding of cognitive evolution, but such studies are rare. Here, we tested 13 species of lacertid lizards (Reptilia: Lacertidae) using a battery of cognitive tests measuring inhibitory control, problem-solving, and spatial and reversal learning. Next, we tested the relationship between species' performance and (a) resource availability (temperature and precipitation), habitat complexity (Normalized Difference Vegetation Index), and habitat variability (seasonality) in their natural habitat and (b) their life history (size at hatching and maturity, clutch size, and frequency). Although species differed markedly in their cognitive abilities, such variation was mostly unrelated to their ecology and life history. Yet, species living in more variable environments exhibited lower behavioral flexibility, likely due to energetic constrains in such habitats. Our standardized protocols provide opportunities for collaborative research, allowing increased sample sizes and replication, essential for moving forward in the field of comparative cognition. Follow-up studies could include more detailed measures of habitat structure and look at other potential selective drivers such as predation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gilles De Meester
- Functional Morphology Lab, Department of Biology, University of Antwerp, Wilrijk, 2610, Belgium.,Section of Zoology and Marine Biology, Department of Biology, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, 157 84, Greece
| | - Lisa Van Linden
- Functional Morphology Lab, Department of Biology, University of Antwerp, Wilrijk, 2610, Belgium
| | - Jonas Torfs
- Functional Morphology Lab, Department of Biology, University of Antwerp, Wilrijk, 2610, Belgium
| | - Panayiotis Pafilis
- Section of Zoology and Marine Biology, Department of Biology, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, 157 84, Greece
| | - Emina Šunje
- Functional Morphology Lab, Department of Biology, University of Antwerp, Wilrijk, 2610, Belgium.,Department of Biology, Faculty of Natural Sciences, University of Sarajevo, Sarajevo, 71000, Bosnia and Herzegovina.,Herpetological Association in Bosnia and Herzegovina: BHHU: ATRA, Sarajevo, 71000, Bosnia and Herzegovina
| | - Dries Steenssens
- Functional Morphology Lab, Department of Biology, University of Antwerp, Wilrijk, 2610, Belgium
| | - Tea Zulčić
- Herpetological Association in Bosnia and Herzegovina: BHHU: ATRA, Sarajevo, 71000, Bosnia and Herzegovina
| | - Athanasios Sassalos
- Section of Zoology and Marine Biology, Department of Biology, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, 157 84, Greece
| | - Raoul Van Damme
- Functional Morphology Lab, Department of Biology, University of Antwerp, Wilrijk, 2610, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Brereton JE, Fernandez EJ. Investigating Unused Tools for the Animal Behavioral Diversity Toolkit. Animals (Basel) 2022; 12:2984. [PMID: 36359107 PMCID: PMC9654576 DOI: 10.3390/ani12212984] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/03/2022] [Revised: 10/25/2022] [Accepted: 10/28/2022] [Indexed: 10/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Behavioral diversity is a commonly used tool used to quantify the richness and evenness of animal behaviors and assess the effect of variables that may impact an animal's quality of life. The indices used in behavioral diversity research, and the study subjects, have not been formally reviewed. This paper aims to identify which indices are being used in behavioral diversity research, and under which scenarios, and uncover novel indices from other disciplines that could be applied to behavioral diversity. To investigate the techniques and species investigated in behavioral diversity literature, a Web of Science literature search was conducted. Two methods: behavioral richness and the Shannon-Wiener index, were the most frequently used indices, whereas the Behavioral Variability index featured rarely. While a range of species appeared in the behavioral literature, mammals were the most frequently studied Class, whereas amphibians did not feature in any papers. There are several diversity indices which did not feature in behavioral diversity including Simpson's index, and Chao. Such indices could be used to better understand animal behavioral study outputs or be used to estimate the number of 'unobserved' behaviors that an animal may express. Future studies could therefore extend beyond the Shannon-Wiener and richness indices.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James Edward Brereton
- Animal and Zoo Science, University Centre Sparsholt, Westley Lane, Sparsholt, Winchester SO21 2NF, UK
| | - Eduardo J. Fernandez
- School of Animal and Veterinary Sciences, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide, SA 5005, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Sarkar R, Bhadra A. How do animals navigate the urban jungle? A review of cognition in urban-adapted animals. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101177] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
33
|
Almeida-Warren K, Camara HD, Matsuzawa T, Carvalho S. Landscaping the Behavioural Ecology of Primate Stone Tool Use. INT J PRIMATOL 2022. [DOI: 10.1007/s10764-022-00305-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
AbstractEcology is fundamental in the development, transmission, and perpetuity of primate technology. Previous studies on tool site selection have addressed the relevance of targeted resources and raw materials for tools, but few have considered the broader foraging landscape. In this landscape-scale study of the ecological contexts of wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) tool use, we investigated the conditions required for nut-cracking to occur and persist in discrete locations at the long-term field site of Bossou, Guinea. We examined this at three levels: selection, frequency of use, and inactivity. We collected data on plant foods, nut trees, and raw materials using transect and quadrat methods, and conducted forest-wide surveys to map the location of nests and watercourses. We analysed data at the quadrat level (n = 82) using generalised linear models and descriptive statistics. We found that, further to the presence of a nut tree and availability of raw materials, abundance of food-providing trees as well as proximity to nest sites were significant predictors of nut-cracking occurrence. This suggests that the spatial distribution of nut-cracking sites is mediated by the broader behavioural landscape and is influenced by non-extractive foraging of perennial resources and non-foraging activities. Additionally, the number of functional tools was greater at sites with higher nut-cracking frequency, and was negatively correlated with site inactivity. Our research indicates that the technological landscape of Bossou chimpanzees shares affinities with the ‘favoured places’ model of hominin site formation, providing a comparative framework for reconstructing landscape-scale patterns of ancient human behaviour. A French translation of this abstract is provided in theelectronic supplementary information: EMS 2.
Collapse
|
34
|
Péter H, Zuberbühler K, Hobaiter C. Well-digging in a community of forest-living wild East African chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Primates 2022; 63:355-364. [PMID: 35662388 PMCID: PMC9273564 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-022-00992-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/16/2021] [Accepted: 05/07/2022] [Indexed: 10/31/2022]
Abstract
Access to resources shapes species' physiology and behaviour. Water is not typically considered a limiting resource for rainforest-living chimpanzees; however, several savannah and savannah-woodland communities show behavioural adaptations to limited water. Here, we provide a first report of habitual well-digging in a rainforest-living group of East African chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) and suggest that it may have been imported into the community's behavioural repertoire by an immigrant female. We describe the presence and frequency of well-digging and related behaviour, and suggest that its subsequent spread in the group may have involved some degree of social learning. We highlight that subsurface water is a concealed resource, and that the limited spread of well-digging in the group may highlight the cognitive, rather than physical, challenges it presents in a rainforest environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hella Péter
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, UK.,School of Anthropology and Conservation, University of Kent, Canterbury, UK
| | - Klaus Zuberbühler
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, UK.,Department of Comparative Cognition, University of Neuchâtel, Neuchâtel, Switzerland.,Budongo Conservation Field Station, PO Box 362, Masindi, Uganda
| | - Catherine Hobaiter
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, UK. .,Budongo Conservation Field Station, PO Box 362, Masindi, Uganda.
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Population dynamics and genetic connectivity in recent chimpanzee history. CELL GENOMICS 2022; 2:None. [PMID: 35711737 PMCID: PMC9188271 DOI: 10.1016/j.xgen.2022.100133] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/02/2021] [Revised: 12/29/2021] [Accepted: 04/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Knowledge on the population history of endangered species is critical for conservation, but whole-genome data on chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) is geographically sparse. Here, we produced the first non-invasive geolocalized catalog of genomic diversity by capturing chromosome 21 from 828 non-invasive samples collected at 48 sampling sites across Africa. The four recognized subspecies show clear genetic differentiation correlating with known barriers, while previously undescribed genetic exchange suggests that these have been permeable on a local scale. We obtained a detailed reconstruction of population stratification and fine-scale patterns of isolation, migration, and connectivity, including a comprehensive picture of admixture with bonobos (Pan paniscus). Unlike humans, chimpanzees did not experience extended episodes of long-distance migrations, which might have limited cultural transmission. Finally, based on local rare variation, we implement a fine-grained geolocalization approach demonstrating improved precision in determining the origin of confiscated chimpanzees.
Collapse
|
36
|
Pokharel SS, Sharma N, Sukumar R. Viewing the rare through public lenses: insights into dead calf carrying and other thanatological responses in Asian elephants using YouTube videos. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2022; 9:211740. [PMID: 35620003 PMCID: PMC9114935 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.211740] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/12/2021] [Accepted: 04/19/2022] [Indexed: 05/03/2023]
Abstract
Documenting the behavioural repertoire of an animal species is important for understanding that species' natural history. Many behaviours such as mating, parturition and death may be observed only rarely in the wild due to the low frequency of occurrence, short duration and the species' elusiveness. Opportunistic documentation of rare behaviours is therefore valuable for deciphering the behavioural complexity in a species. In this context, digital platforms may serve as useful data sources for studying rare behaviours in animals. Using videos uploaded on YouTube, we document and construct a tentative repertoire of thanatological responses (death-related behaviours) in Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). The most frequently observed thanatological responses included postural changes, guarding/keeping vigil, touching, investigating the carcass, epimeletic behaviours and vocalizations. We also describe some infrequently observed behaviours, including carrying dead calves by adult females, re-assurance-like behaviours and attempts to support dying or dead conspecifics, some of which were only known anecdotally in Asian elephants. Our observations indicate the significance of open-source video data on digital platforms for gaining insights into rarely observed behaviours and support the accumulating evidence for higher cognitive abilities of Asian elephants in the context of comparative thanatology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Nachiketha Sharma
- Centre for Ecological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science, Bengaluru, India
| | - Raman Sukumar
- Centre for Ecological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science, Bengaluru, India
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Giuliano C, Stewart FA, Piel AK. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) grouping patterns in an open and dry savanna landscape, Issa Valley, western Tanzania. J Hum Evol 2022; 163:103137. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2021.103137] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/17/2020] [Revised: 12/09/2021] [Accepted: 12/14/2021] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
38
|
Bessa J, Biro D, Hockings K. Inter-community behavioural variation confirmed through indirect methods in four neighbouring chimpanzee communities in Cantanhez NP, Guinea-Bissau. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2022; 9:211518. [PMID: 35223060 PMCID: PMC8864369 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.211518] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2021] [Accepted: 01/31/2022] [Indexed: 05/03/2023]
Abstract
Culture, while long viewed as exclusively human, has now been demonstrated across diverse taxa and contexts. However, most animal culture data are constrained to well-studied, habituated groups. This is the case for chimpanzees, arguably the most 'cultural' non-human species. While much progress has been made charting wild chimpanzees' cultural repertoire, large gaps remain in our knowledge of the majority of the continent's chimpanzees. Furthermore, few studies have compared neighbouring communities, despite such comparisons providing the strongest evidence for culture, and few have studied communities living in anthropogenic habitats although their culture is in imminent danger of disappearing. Here we combine direct, indirect and remote methods, including camera traps, to study, over 2 years, four unhabituated neighbouring chimpanzee communities inhabiting human-impacted habitats in Cantanhez NP, Guinea-Bissau. From traces collected during 1089 km of reconnaissance walks and 4197 videos from 56 camera trap locations, we identified 18 putative cultural traits. These included some noteworthy novel behaviours for these communities, and behaviours possibly new to the species. We created preliminary behavioural profiles for each community, and found inter-community differences spanning tool use, communication, and social behaviour, demonstrating the importance of comparing neighbouring communities and of studying previously neglected communities including those inhabiting anthropogenic landscapes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joana Bessa
- Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
- Centre for Research in Anthropology (CRIA NOVA FCSH), Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Dora Biro
- Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
| | - Kimberley Hockings
- Centre for Research in Anthropology (CRIA NOVA FCSH), Lisbon, Portugal
- Centre for Ecology and Conservation, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Penryn, UK
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Graham KE, Badihi G, Safryghin A, Grund C, Hobaiter C. A socio-ecological perspective on the gestural communication of great ape species, individuals, and social units. ETHOL ECOL EVOL 2022; 34:235-259. [PMID: 35529671 PMCID: PMC9067943 DOI: 10.1080/03949370.2021.1988722] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/05/2023]
Abstract
Over the last 30 years, most research on non-human primate gestural communication has been produced by psychologists, which has shaped the questions asked and the methods used. These researchers have drawn on concepts from philosophy, linguistics, anthropology, and ethology, but despite these broad influences the field has neglected to situate gestures into the socio-ecological context in which the diverse species, individuals, and social-units exist. In this review, we present current knowledge about great ape gestural communication in terms of repertoires, meanings, and development. We fold this into a conversation about variation in other types of ape social behaviour to identify areas for future research on variation in gestural communication. Given the large variation in socio-ecological factors across species and social-units (and the individuals within these groups), we may expect to find different preferences for specific gesture types; different needs for communicating specific meanings; and different rates of encountering specific contexts. New tools, such as machine-learning based automated movement tracking, may allow us to uncover potential variation in the speed and form of gesture actions or parts of gesture actions. New multi-group multi-generational datasets provide the opportunity to apply analyses, such as Bayesian modelling, which allows us to examine these rich behavioural landscapes. Together, by expanding our questions and our methods, researchers may finally be able to study great ape gestures from the perspective of the apes themselves and explore what this gestural communication system reveals about apes’ thinking and experience of their world.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kirsty E. Graham
- School of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Mary’s Quad, South St, St Andrews KY16 9JP, Scotland, UK
| | - Gal Badihi
- School of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Mary’s Quad, South St, St Andrews KY16 9JP, Scotland, UK
| | - Alexandra Safryghin
- School of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Mary’s Quad, South St, St Andrews KY16 9JP, Scotland, UK
| | - Charlotte Grund
- School of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Mary’s Quad, South St, St Andrews KY16 9JP, Scotland, UK
| | - Catherine Hobaiter
- School of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Mary’s Quad, South St, St Andrews KY16 9JP, Scotland, UK
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Carvalho S, Wessling EG, Abwe EE, Almeida‐Warren K, Arandjelovic M, Boesch C, Danquah E, Diallo MS, Hobaiter C, Hockings K, Humle T, Ikemeh RA, Kalan AK, Luncz L, Ohashi G, Pascual‐Garrido A, Piel A, Samuni L, Soiret S, Sanz C, Koops K. Using nonhuman culture in conservation requires careful and concerted action. Conserv Lett 2022. [DOI: 10.1111/conl.12860] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Susana Carvalho
- Primate Models for Behavioural Evolution Lab, Institute of Human Sciences University of Oxford Oxford UK
- Gorongosa National Park Sofala Mozambique
- Interdisciplinary Center for Archaeology and Evolution of Human Behaviour Universidade do Algarve Faro Portugal
| | - Erin G. Wessling
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology Harvard University Cambridge Massachusetts
| | - Ekwoge E. Abwe
- San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance San Diego California
- Ebo Forest Research Project Douala Cameroon
| | - Katarina Almeida‐Warren
- Primate Models for Behavioural Evolution Lab, Institute of Human Sciences University of Oxford Oxford UK
- Interdisciplinary Center for Archaeology and Evolution of Human Behaviour Universidade do Algarve Faro Portugal
| | | | - Christophe Boesch
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Leipzig Germany
- Wild Chimpanzee Foundation Cologny Switzerland
| | - Emmanuel Danquah
- Faculty of Renewable Natural Resources Department of Wildlife and Range Management Kwame Nkrumah University of Science and Technology Kumasi Ghana
| | | | - Catherine Hobaiter
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience University of St Andrews St Andrews UK
| | | | - Tatyana Humle
- Division of Human and Social Science, School of Anthropology and Conservation Durrell Institute of Conservation and Ecology, University of Kent Canterbury UK
| | | | - Ammie K. Kalan
- Department of Anthropology University of Victoria Victoria Canada
| | - Lydia Luncz
- Technological Primates Research Group, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Leipzig Germany
| | | | - Alejandra Pascual‐Garrido
- Primate Models for Behavioural Evolution Lab, Institute of Human Sciences University of Oxford Oxford UK
- Department of Anthropology Durham University Durham UK
| | - Alex Piel
- Department of Anthropology University College London London UK
| | - Liran Samuni
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology Harvard University Cambridge Massachusetts
| | - Serge Soiret
- Laboratoire de Zoologie et de Biologie Animale Université Félix Houphouët‐Boigny Abidjan Côte d'Ivoire
- Centre Suisse de Recherches Scientifiques en Côte d'Ivoire (CSRS) Abidjan Côte d'Ivoire
| | - Crickette Sanz
- Department of Anthropology Washington University in Saint Louis Saint Louis Missouri
- Wildlife Conservation Society, Congo Program Brazzaville Republic of Congo
| | - Kathelijne Koops
- Ape Behaviour & Ecology Group, Department of Anthropology University of Zurich Zürich Switzerland
- Department of Archaeology University of Cambridge Cambridge UK
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Lindshield S, Hernandez-Aguilar RA, Korstjens AH, Marchant LF, Narat V, Ndiaye PI, Ogawa H, Piel AK, Pruetz JD, Stewart FA, van Leeuwen KL, Wessling EG, Yoshikawa M. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in savanna landscapes. Evol Anthropol 2021; 30:399-420. [PMID: 34542218 DOI: 10.1002/evan.21924] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2020] [Revised: 12/17/2020] [Accepted: 07/29/2021] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are the only great apes that inhabit hot, dry, and open savannas. We review the environmental pressures of savannas on chimpanzees, such as food and water scarcity, and the evidence for chimpanzees' behavioral responses to these landscapes. In our analysis, savannas were generally associated with low chimpanzee population densities and large home ranges. In addition, thermoregulatory behaviors that likely reduce hyperthermia risk, such as cave use, were frequently observed in the hottest and driest savanna landscapes. We hypothesize that such responses are evidence of a "savanna landscape effect" in chimpanzees and offer pathways for future research to understand its evolutionary processes and mechanisms. We conclude by discussing the significance of research on savanna chimpanzees to modeling the evolution of early hominin traits and informing conservation programs for these endangered apes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stacy Lindshield
- Department of Anthropology, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, USA
| | - R Adriana Hernandez-Aguilar
- Department of Social Psychology and Quantitative Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain.,Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary Synthesis, Department of Biosciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| | - Amanda H Korstjens
- Life and Environmental Sciences Department, Bournemouth University, Talbot Campus, Poole, UK
| | | | - Victor Narat
- CNRS/MNHN/Paris Diderot, UMR 7206 Eco-anthropology, Paris, France
| | - Papa Ibnou Ndiaye
- Département de Biologie Animale, Faculté des Sciences et Techniques, Université Cheikh Anta Diop de Dakar, Dakar, Senegal
| | - Hideshi Ogawa
- School of International Liberal Studies, Chukyo University, Toyota, Aichi, Japan
| | - Alex K Piel
- Department of Anthropology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Jill D Pruetz
- Department of Anthropology, Texas State University, San Marcos, Texas, USA
| | - Fiona A Stewart
- Department of Anthropology, University College London, London, UK.,School of Biological and Environmental Sciences, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
| | - Kelly L van Leeuwen
- Department of Life and Environmental Sciences, Bournemouth University, Talbot Campus, Poole, UK
| | - Erin G Wessling
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Midori Yoshikawa
- Department of Zoology, National Museum of Nature and Science, Ibaraki, Tokyo, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Bessone M, Booto L, Santos AR, Kühl HS, Fruth B. No time to rest: How the effects of climate change on nest decay threaten the conservation of apes in the wild. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0252527. [PMID: 34191810 PMCID: PMC8244864 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0252527] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2020] [Accepted: 05/17/2021] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Since 1994, IUCN Red List assessments apply globally acknowledged standards to assess species distribution, abundance and trends. The extinction risk of a species has a major impact on conservation science and international funding mechanisms. Great ape species are listed as Endangered or Critically Endangered. Their populations are often assessed using their unique habit of constructing sleeping platforms, called nests. As nests rather than apes are counted, it is necessary to know the time it takes for nests to disappear to convert nest counts into ape numbers. However, nest decomposition is highly variable across sites and time and the factors involved are poorly understood. Here, we used 1,511 bonobo (Pan paniscus) nests and 15 years of climatic data (2003-2018) from the research site LuiKotale, Democratic Republic of the Congo, to investigate the effects of climate change and behavioural factors on nest decay time, using a Bayesian gamma survival model. We also tested the logistic regression method, a recommended time-efficient option for estimating nest decay time. Our climatic data showed a decreasing trend in precipitation across the 15 years of study. We found bonobo nests to have longer decay times in recent years. While the number of storms was the main factor driving nest decay time, nest construction type and tree species used were also important. We also found evidence for bonobo nesting behaviour being adapted to climatic conditions, namely strengthening the nest structure in response to unpredictable, harsh precipitation. By highlighting methodological caveats, we show that logistic regression is effective in estimating nest decay time under certain conditions. Our study reveals the impact of climate change on nest decay time in a tropical remote area. Failure to account for these changes would invalidate biomonitoring estimates of global significance, and subsequently jeopardize the conservation of great apes in the wild.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mattia Bessone
- School of Biological and Environmental Sciences, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, United Kingdom
| | - Lambert Booto
- LuiKotale Bonobo Project, Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Antonio R. Santos
- School of Biological and Environmental Sciences, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, United Kingdom
| | - Hjalmar S. Kühl
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv) Halle-Jena-Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Barbara Fruth
- School of Biological and Environmental Sciences, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, United Kingdom
- LuiKotale Bonobo Project, Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
- Faculty of Biology/Department of Neurobiology, Ludwig Maximilians University of Munich, Planegg-Martinsried, Germany
- Department for the Ecology of Animal Societies, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Konstanz, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Abstract
Understanding variation in host-associated microbial communities is important given the relevance of microbiomes to host physiology and health. Using 560 fecal samples collected from wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) across their range, we assessed how geography, genetics, climate, vegetation, and diet relate to gut microbial community structure (prokaryotes, eukaryotic parasites) at multiple spatial scales. We observed a high degree of regional specificity in the microbiome composition, which was associated with host genetics, available plant foods, and potentially with cultural differences in tool use, which affect diet. Genetic differences drove community composition at large scales, while vegetation and potentially tool use drove within-region differences, likely due to their influence on diet. Unlike industrialized human populations in the United States, where regional differences in the gut microbiome are undetectable, chimpanzee gut microbiomes are far more variable across space, suggesting that technological developments have decoupled humans from their local environments, obscuring regional differences that could have been important during human evolution. IMPORTANCE Gut microbial communities are drivers of primate physiology and health, but the factors that influence the gut microbiome in wild primate populations remain largely undetermined. We report data from a continent-wide survey of wild chimpanzee gut microbiota and highlight the effects of genetics, vegetation, and potentially even tool use at different spatial scales on the chimpanzee gut microbiome, including bacteria, archaea, and eukaryotic parasites. Microbial community dissimilarity was strongly correlated with chimpanzee population genetic dissimilarity, and vegetation composition and consumption of algae, honey, nuts, and termites were potentially associated with additional divergence in microbial communities between sampling sites. Our results suggest that host genetics, geography, and climate play a far stronger role in structuring the gut microbiome in chimpanzees than in humans.
Collapse
|
44
|
Delfour F, Vaicekauskaite R, García-Párraga D, Pilenga C, Serres A, Brasseur I, Pascaud A, Perlado-Campos E, Sánchez-Contreras GJ, Baumgartner K, Monreal-Pawlowsky T. Behavioural Diversity Study in Bottlenose Dolphin ( Tursiops truncatus) Groups and Its Implications for Welfare Assessments. Animals (Basel) 2021; 11:ani11061715. [PMID: 34201227 PMCID: PMC8230076 DOI: 10.3390/ani11061715] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2021] [Revised: 06/04/2021] [Accepted: 06/04/2021] [Indexed: 01/04/2023] Open
Abstract
Simple Summary For the first time, a behavioural diversity study was conducted on several bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) groups within European Association of Aquatic Mammals (EAAM) accredited facilities. This study was carried out by professional animal staff on 54 dolphins, and the goal was to analyse behavioural diversity in bottlenose dolphins at the group level to investigate how particular factors might impact the diversity of behaviours within the group and to discuss its implications for dolphin welfare assessments. This study showed its feasibility and revealed impacting factors that would need to be considered in future dolphin welfare assessments. We strongly believe that behavioural evaluations and measurements could be applied routinely on cetaceans under professional care to assess their welfare. Abstract In the recent past, animal welfare studies have tried to determine the best animal welfare measures and indicators. Expression of behavioural diversity is considered a potential positive welfare indicator, and to the authors’ knowledge, it has not been validated nor studied in cetaceans. For the first time, a behavioural diversity study on bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) groups was conducted at six European facilities. The study was carried out by the animal care staff, biologists and veterinarians and included 54 dolphins housed in several group compositions at the different participating facilities. The goal of our study was to analyse behavioural diversity in bottlenose dolphins at the group level to investigate how particular factors might impact the diversity of behaviours within the group and to discuss its implications for dolphin welfare assessments. Eight factors (i.e., “observer location”, “number of individuals”, “age class”, “sex”, “social grouping”, “presence/absence of leading male”, “presence/absence of visitors” and “enrichment provision”) impacted the behavioural diversity of the observed groups, while no significant impact of the factors “time of day” and “activity before/after observation” could be found. Our study showed the feasibility of this kind of approach for cetaceans under professional care and the relevance to considering this parameter in dolphin welfare studies, despite certain limitations that warrant further research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ruta Vaicekauskaite
- Marine Research Institute, Klaipeda University, 92294 Klaipeda, Lithuania;
- Lithuania & Fox Consulting, 67500 Haguenau, France
| | | | | | - Agathe Serres
- Institute of Deep Sea Science and Engineering, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Sanya 572000, China;
| | | | - Ana Pascaud
- ONIRIS—Ecole Nationale Vétérinaire de Nantes, 101 Route de Gachet, 44307 Nantes, France;
- Planète Sauvage, 44710 Nantes, France
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
Palmer A, Sommer V, Msindai JN. Hybrid apes in the Anthropocene: Burden or asset for conservation? PEOPLE AND NATURE 2021; 3:573-586. [PMID: 34805779 PMCID: PMC8581989 DOI: 10.1002/pan3.10214] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/09/2020] [Accepted: 04/01/2021] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Conservationists often view hybrid animals as problematic, at least if anthropogenic influence caused the intermixing to occur. However, critics propose that humans should respect non-human autonomy, reject and accept the creatures they have helped to create.Based on two case studies of our own ethological, genetic and ethnographic research about chimpanzee and orangutan subspecies hybrids, we assess what, if anything, should be done about such animals. We consider problems posed by cross-bred apes relating to: (a) Breeding-Do hybrids really experience reduced reproductive success? How are population-level concerns and welfare of individual animals balanced in conservation breeding? (b) Essentialism-Are anti-hybrid arguments based on essentialist or purist thinking? Does essentialism vary by conservation context? (c) Pragmatism-How do socio-economic circumstances influence whether hybrids are embraced or ignored? Does the erosion of 'untouched nature' render hybrids more important?We show that answers to these questions are complex and context-specific, and that therefore decisions should be made on a case-by-case basis. For example, we find that anti-hybrid arguments are essentialist in some cases (e.g. ape management in zoos) but not in others (e.g. ape reintroduction). Thus, rather than present recommendations, we conclude by posing nine questions that conservationists should ask themselves when making decisions about taxonomic hybrids. A free Plain Language Summary can be found within the Supporting Information of this article.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandra Palmer
- School of Geography and the EnvironmentUniversity of OxfordOxfordUK
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
46
|
Williams KA, Slater HD, Gillingham P, Korstjens AH. Environmental Factors Are Stronger Predictors of Primate Species’ Distributions Than Basic Biological Traits. INT J PRIMATOL 2021. [DOI: 10.1007/s10764-021-00208-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
AbstractUnderstanding the neutral, biological, and environmental processes driving species distributions is valuable in informing conservation efforts because it will help us predict how species will respond to changes in environmental conditions. Environmental processes affect species differently according to their biological traits, which determine how they interact with their environment. Therefore, functional, trait-based modeling approaches are considered important for predicting distributions and species responses to change but even for data-rich primate communities our understanding of the relationships between traits and environmental conditions is limited. Here we use a large-scale, high-resolution data set of African diurnal primate distributions, biological traits, and environmental conditions to investigate the role of biological traits and environmental trait filtering in primate distributions. We collected data from published sources for 354 sites and 14 genera with 57 species across sub-Saharan Africa. We then combined a three-table ordination method, RLQ, with the fourth-corner approach to test relationships between environmental variables and biological traits and used a mapping approach to visually assess patterning in primate genus and species’ distributions. We found no significant relationships between any groups of environmental variables and biological traits, despite a clear role of environmental filtering in driving genus and species’ distributions. The most important environmental driver of species distributions was temperature seasonality, followed by rainfall. We conclude that the relative flexibility of many primate genera means that not any one particular set of traits drives their species–environment associations, despite the clear role of such associations in their distribution patterns.
Collapse
|
47
|
Lester JD, Vigilant L, Gratton P, McCarthy MS, Barratt CD, Dieguez P, Agbor A, Álvarez-Varona P, Angedakin S, Ayimisin EA, Bailey E, Bessone M, Brazzola G, Chancellor R, Cohen H, Danquah E, Deschner T, Egbe VE, Eno-Nku M, Goedmakers A, Granjon AC, Head J, Hedwig D, Hernandez-Aguilar RA, Jeffery KJ, Jones S, Junker J, Kadam P, Kaiser M, Kalan AK, Kehoe L, Kienast I, Langergraber KE, Lapuente J, Laudisoit A, Lee K, Marrocoli S, Mihindou V, Morgan D, Muhanguzi G, Neil E, Nicholl S, Orbell C, Ormsby LJ, Pacheco L, Piel A, Robbins MM, Rundus A, Sanz C, Sciaky L, Siaka AM, Städele V, Stewart F, Tagg N, Ton E, van Schijndel J, Vyalengerera MK, Wessling EG, Willie J, Wittig RM, Yuh YG, Yurkiw K, Zuberbuehler K, Boesch C, Kühl HS, Arandjelovic M. Recent genetic connectivity and clinal variation in chimpanzees. Commun Biol 2021; 4:283. [PMID: 33674780 PMCID: PMC7935964 DOI: 10.1038/s42003-021-01806-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2020] [Accepted: 02/04/2021] [Indexed: 01/31/2023] Open
Abstract
Much like humans, chimpanzees occupy diverse habitats and exhibit extensive behavioural variability. However, chimpanzees are recognized as a discontinuous species, with four subspecies separated by historical geographic barriers. Nevertheless, their range-wide degree of genetic connectivity remains poorly resolved, mainly due to sampling limitations. By analyzing a geographically comprehensive sample set amplified at microsatellite markers that inform recent population history, we found that isolation by distance explains most of the range-wide genetic structure of chimpanzees. Furthermore, we did not identify spatial discontinuities corresponding with the recognized subspecies, suggesting that some of the subspecies-delineating geographic barriers were recently permeable to gene flow. Substantial range-wide genetic connectivity is consistent with the hypothesis that behavioural flexibility is a salient driver of chimpanzee responses to changing environmental conditions. Finally, our observation of strong local differentiation associated with recent anthropogenic pressures portends future loss of critical genetic diversity if habitat fragmentation and population isolation continue unabated.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jack D Lester
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany.
| | - Linda Vigilant
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Paolo Gratton
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Maureen S McCarthy
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Christopher D Barratt
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv) Halle-Jena-Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Paula Dieguez
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Anthony Agbor
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Paula Álvarez-Varona
- Jane Goodall Institute Spain and Senegal, Dindefelo Biological Station, Dindefelo, Kedougou, Senegal
| | - Samuel Angedakin
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | | | - Emma Bailey
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Mattia Bessone
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Gregory Brazzola
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Rebecca Chancellor
- West Chester University, Depts of Anthropology & Sociology and Psychology, West Chester, PA, USA
| | - Heather Cohen
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Emmanuel Danquah
- Department of Wildlife and Range Management, Faculty of Renewable Natural Resources, Kwame Nkrumah University of Science and Technology, Kumasi, Ghana
| | - Tobias Deschner
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Villard Ebot Egbe
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | | | | | - Anne-Céline Granjon
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Josephine Head
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Daniela Hedwig
- Elephant Listening Project, Center for Conservation Bioacoustics, Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA
| | - R Adriana Hernandez-Aguilar
- Jane Goodall Institute Spain and Senegal, Dindefelo Biological Station, Dindefelo, Kedougou, Senegal
- Department of Social Psychology and Quantitative Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Kathryn J Jeffery
- Biological and Environmental Sciences, Faculty of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling, UK
| | - Sorrel Jones
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Jessica Junker
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | | | - Michael Kaiser
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Ammie K Kalan
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Laura Kehoe
- Wild Chimpanzee Foundation (WCF), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Ivonne Kienast
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Kevin E Langergraber
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, 900 Cady Mall, Tempe, AZ 85287 Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
| | - Juan Lapuente
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
- Comoé Chimpanzee Conservation Project, Comoé National Park, Kakpin, Côte d'Ivoire
| | - Anne Laudisoit
- Ecohealth Alliance, New York, NY, USA
- University of Antwerp, Campus Drie Eiken, lokaal D.133, Universiteitsplein 1 - 2610, Antwerpen, Belgium
| | - Kevin Lee
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Sergio Marrocoli
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Vianet Mihindou
- Agence National des Parcs Nationaux (ANPN) Batterie 4, Libreville, Gabon
- Ministère des Eaux, des Forêts, de la Mer, de l'Environnement, Chargé du Plan Climat, des Objectifs de Développement Durable et du Plan d'Affectation des Terres, Libreville, Gabon
| | - David Morgan
- Lester E. Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes, Lincoln Park Zoo, Chicago, IL, USA
| | | | - Emily Neil
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Sonia Nicholl
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | | | - Lucy Jayne Ormsby
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Liliana Pacheco
- Jane Goodall Institute Spain and Senegal, Dindefelo Biological Station, Dindefelo, Kedougou, Senegal
| | - Alex Piel
- Department of Anthropology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Martha M Robbins
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Aaron Rundus
- West Chester University, Department of Psychology, West Chester, PA, USA
| | - Crickette Sanz
- Washington University in Saint Louis, Department of Anthropology, One Brookings Drive, St. Louis, MO, USA
- Wildlife Conservation Society, Congo Program, Brazzaville, Republic of Congo
| | - Lilah Sciaky
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Alhaji M Siaka
- National Protected Area Authority, Freetown, Sierra Leone
| | - Veronika Städele
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Fiona Stewart
- School of Biological & Environmental Sciences, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
| | - Nikki Tagg
- KMDA, Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Els Ton
- Chimbo Foundation, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | | | | | - Erin G Wessling
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Jacob Willie
- KMDA, Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Roman M Wittig
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
- Taï Chimpanzee Project, Centre Suisse de Recherches Scientifiques, Abidjan, Côte d'Ivoire
| | - Yisa Ginath Yuh
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Kyle Yurkiw
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
- Pan Verus Project Outamba-Kilimi National Park, Freetown, Sierra Leone
| | - Klaus Zuberbuehler
- Budongo Conservation Field Station, Masindi, Uganda
- Université de Neuchâtel, Institut de Biologie, Neuchâtel, Switzerland
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, UK
| | - Christophe Boesch
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
| | - Hjalmar S Kühl
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany
- German Centre for Integrative Biodiversity Research (iDiv) Halle-Jena-Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Mimi Arandjelovic
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI EVAN), Leipzig, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Chimpanzees balance resources and risk in an anthropogenic landscape of fear. Sci Rep 2021; 11:4569. [PMID: 33633129 PMCID: PMC7907193 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-83852-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2020] [Accepted: 02/09/2021] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Human-wildlife coexistence is possible when animals can meet their ecological requirements while managing human-induced risks. Understanding how wildlife balance these trade-offs in anthropogenic environments is crucial to develop effective strategies to reduce risks of negative interactions, including bi-directional aggression and disease transmission. For the first time, we use a landscape of fear framework with Bayesian spatiotemporal modelling to investigate anthropogenic risk-mitigation and optimal foraging trade-offs in Critically Endangered western chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus). Using 12 months of camera trap data (21 camera traps, 6722 camera trap days) and phenology on wild and cultivated plant species collected at Caiquene–Cadique, Cantanhez National Park (Guinea-Bissau), we show that humans and chimpanzees broadly overlapped in their use of forest and anthropogenic parts of the habitat including villages and cultivated areas. The spatiotemporal model showed that chimpanzee use of space was predicted by the availability of naturalised oil-palm fruit. Chimpanzees used areas away from villages and agriculture more intensively, but optimised their foraging strategies by increasing their use of village areas with cultivated fruits when wild fruits were scarce. Our modelling approach generates fine-resolution space–time output maps, which can be scaled-up to identify human-wildlife interaction hotspots at the landscape level, informing coexistence strategy.
Collapse
|
49
|
YAMAMOTO S. “UNWILLING” VERSUS “UNABLE”: UNDERSTANDING CHIMPANZEES’ RESTRICTIONS IN COGNITION AND MOTIVATION. PSYCHOLOGIA 2021. [DOI: 10.2117/psysoc.2021-b020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
|
50
|
Marchant LF, Wessling EG, Lindshield SM. Introduction to the Special Issue on Savanna Chimpanzees. INT J PRIMATOL 2020. [DOI: 10.1007/s10764-020-00188-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
|