1
|
Sant AM, Black M. Elucidating managerial, staff and client perspectives on the Speech and Language Therapy service in Malta. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2024; 59:2412-2422. [PMID: 39024022 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.13093] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2023] [Accepted: 07/03/2024] [Indexed: 07/20/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Speech and language therapy (SLT) services are fundamental for communication, academic and social development. Evidence shows that demand for services is increasing, and this can adversely affect the quality of care. There are limited published studies in relation to the demand and quality of speech and language services in Malta. AIMS To elucidate the perspectives of speech-language pathologists (SLPs), service managers and parents of clients about the impact of factors pertaining to human resource development (HRD), human resource management (HRM) and the environment on the quality of the children's SLT service in western Malta. Also, to outline the beneficial strategies or recommendations to improve the quality of the service provided. METHODS & PROCEDURES A mixed-method study was used. Quantitative data provided by the Speech and Language Center (SLC) were analysed to determine demand over time using descriptive statistics. Qualitative data from one-to-one interviews using a bespoke questionnaire with managers, SLPs and parents were transcribed and analysed. Common and distinct themes in relation to the quality of services were then formulated and evaluated. OUTCOME & RESULTS Quantitative results highlighted that clients were not receiving the required number of the therapeutic sessions. In total, nine themes in relation to the quality of the SLT service were identified by parents, SLPs and managers. The factors affecting the service quality were outlined and subdivided under three branches: HRD, HRM and the environment. All factors within these subgroups are expected to enhance the quality of the service provided if they are enhanced and improved upon. Additionally, the results showed that a discrepancy between the perceived and offered SLT services was felt from the managers' and SLPs' perspectives; however, this was not felt by parents. For parents, the main concern was the availability and frequency of the therapeutic sessions provided. CONCLUSION & IMPLICATIONS Insights from SLPs, managers and parents of clients highlighted the barriers and enablers of quality of service in SLT services for children. These findings can be used to improve services in Malta and other countries with similar contexts, mainly by altering time management, reducing demands and improving availability. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS What is already known on the subject SLT is proven to enhance a child's communication skills. Increasing demands for therapy can affect the quality of the service provided, ultimately affecting the child's progress. There is a gap in the evidence base regarding the quality of speech and language services for children in Malta and the factors which affect the quality of the service. What this study adds to the existing knowledge This is the first study of its kind in Malta. It seeks to identify the quality of SLT services for children from three different perspectives: service managers, SLPs and parents. In addition, this study investigated what affects the quality of SLT services positively or negatively. Through this study, the impeding factors were divided into three domains; HRD factors, HRM factors and environmental factors. These domains were mentioned by all three groups of participants because they affected the service negatively or positively. The main negative aspects included bad time management, high demands, and reduced availabilities and accessibilities, whilst positive aspects included improved support, relationships and the environment. What are the practical and clinical implications of this work? This study suggests reducing the managerial and administrational demands on SLPs and improving session frequency would enhance the quality of service. In relation to the three domains, the HRD factors that would enhance the quality of service are: SLPs' and parental competencies, enhancing SLPs' positive characteristics, positive attitudes and cooperation from all personas; for HRM resources the factors important for quality are strategic and risk management, workforce and recruitment; and for the environmental factors the physical environment and resources are important as they affect the service depending on their condition, that is, improved resources result in improved service. Such alterations would reduce the SLPs' demotivation and burnout due to reduced responsibilities, whilst improving accessibility and availability, ultimately enhancing the quality of the service provided.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ann-Marie Sant
- The University of Sheffield School of Health and Related Research, Sheffield, UK
| | - Michelle Black
- University of Liverpool, Institute of Population Health-Public Health, Policy and Systems, Liverpool, UK
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Frizelle P, O'Shea A, Murphy A, Dahly D, McKean C. Evaluating a targeted selective speech, language, and communication intervention at scale - Protocol for the Happy Talk cluster randomised controlled trial. HRB Open Res 2024; 7:65. [PMID: 39931387 PMCID: PMC11808849 DOI: 10.12688/hrbopenres.13973.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/01/2024] [Indexed: 02/13/2025] Open
Abstract
Background In areas of social disadvantage up to 40-50% of children enter preschool with speech and language skills significantly poorer than would be expected for their age. The Happy Talk trial tests if a community embedded, targeted selective speech and language programme that simultaneously engages with parents and early childhood educators, (1) improves language outcomes in children aged between 2 years 10 months and 6 years and (2) is cost effective for the health care system. Method The Happy Talk trial is a large scale cluster randomised trial of a 12-week manualised intervention delivered in pre/school settings serving socially disadvantaged communities, in Ireland. Seventy-two clusters will receive the intervention (12 participants per cluster). Parents and pre/school staff engage in group training and coaching in the form of 12 1-hour sessions for parents and four staff workshops, over the course of the pre/school year. Training/coaching includes core interaction skills (modelling, expanding, balancing questions and comments), early literacy and phonological awareness. Blinded assessments pre- and immediately post-intervention and at 6 months follow up, will measure the primary outcomes of children's receptive and expressive language and functional impact, and secondary outcomes of quality of life. Parental responsiveness and educator-child interactions will also be evaluated. Discussion This robust study evaluates a public health approach to the delivery of speech language and communication intervention in the 'real world' in the community, which focuses on prevention and equity of access. Pilot work indicates that the programme is feasible, acceptable to parents and staff, cost effective, and suitable for implementation at scale. The trial includes a process evaluation, a well-developed economic evaluation and the outcomes are directly relevant to children, families and educators. This work has the potential to improve the long-term outcomes and life chances of people living in social disadvantage. Trial registration clinicaltrials.gov NCT06460090. Trial Management There is a formal governance structure to oversee the conduct and running of the trial, consisting of a trial management group and a steering committee. More details on the composition, roles and responsibilities of each committee can be found in the supplemental material.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pauline Frizelle
- Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, School of Clinical Therapies, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland
| | - Aoife O'Shea
- Speech and Language Therapy Department, Health Services Executive, Cork, Ireland
| | - Aileen Murphy
- Department of Economics, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland
| | - Darren Dahly
- School of Public Health, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Plonczak AM, Hennedige A, Kearney A, Blumenow W, Piggott K, Garvie L, Milligan C, Duncan C, Parks CJ. Multimodal Outcomes of Early Open Extended Midline Strip Craniectomy With Bilateral Microbarrel Staving for Correction of Isolated Nonsyndromic Sagittal Synostosis. J Craniofac Surg 2024:00001665-990000000-01995. [PMID: 39356245 DOI: 10.1097/scs.0000000000010642] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/09/2023] [Accepted: 08/01/2024] [Indexed: 10/03/2024] Open
Abstract
AIM Sagittal synostosis is the most common craniosynostosis. The aim of surgery is to preserve cognitive function and to correct the morphologic changes. In our unit, the authors offer strip craniectomy and microbarrel staving (SMB) performed at 16 to 22 weeks. This study aims to ascertain the 7-year outcome of scaphocephaly correction by SMB. The authors investigate whether the improvement in cranial index (CI) is sustained over time, and report speech and language and neurodevelopmental outcomes. METHODS All nonsyndromic patients with sagittal synostosis who underwent SMB between 2009 and 2012 were identified from a prospectively created database that recorded anthropometric measurements, quality metrics, and neurodevelopmental outcomes. RESULTS A total of 27 patients were analyzed. The mean CI preoperatively was 67.5% (±3.5), and at 7 years 78% (±4.8). Regarding neurodevelopmental outcomes at 7 years, SDQ responses did not highlight any concerns for social, emotional, or behavioral outcomes. The vast majority of those who completed a WASI-II assessment performed within the average ranges in terms of cognitive ability. In terms of speech and language 70% of subjects demonstrated receptive language within normal level (WNL). Seventy-seven percent of subjects demonstrated expressive language WNL. CONCLUSIONS In conclusion, after 7 years of follow-up, early open extended SMB is a safe and effective method for correction of isolated nonsyndromic isolated sagittal synostosis. Across the neurodevelopmental measures and assessments, the children in the current study appeared to perform within expected ranges. Speech and language problems are more prevalent than in the general population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Agata M Plonczak
- Department of Craniofacial Surgery, Alder Hey Children's Hospital NHS Trust, Eaton
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Krygsman A, Vaillancourt T, Janson H, Idsoe T, Nærde A. Depression symptoms, communication and cooperation skills, and friendship: longitudinal associations in young Norwegian children. FRONTIERS IN CHILD AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHIATRY 2024; 3:1328527. [PMID: 39816582 PMCID: PMC11731607 DOI: 10.3389/frcha.2024.1328527] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2023] [Accepted: 07/22/2024] [Indexed: 01/18/2025]
Abstract
Introduction Symptoms of depression in early childhood have been linked to interpersonal difficulties, whereas friendships serve a protective function. Methods In the present study, we examined depression symptoms in preschool age (4 years) in relation to social skills (communication and cooperation), and friendships into early school age (Grades 1 and 2) in a large subsample (n = 943) of Norwegian children. Results The results indicated that preschool depression symptoms negatively predicted Grade 1 communication skills, which in turn predicted Grade 2 depression symptoms. This pathway suggests that communication skills may be a maintenance factor for depression symptoms in young children. In addition, preschool depression symptoms predicted lower Grade 1 cooperation skills, which in turn predicted lower Grade 2 communication skills, suggesting that preschool depression symptoms may begin a cascade of social skill problems that affect cooperation and communication skills into early school years. Best friendships were negatively related to depression symptoms in preschool and Grade 2. Discussion Given that preschool depression symptoms impact the development of social skills and friendships, it is important to attend to depression symptoms in early childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amanda Krygsman
- Counselling Psychology, Faculty of Education, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON, Canada
| | - Tracy Vaillancourt
- Counselling Psychology, Faculty of Education, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON, Canada
- School of Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON, Canada
| | - Harald Janson
- Norwegian Center for Child Behavioral Development, Oslo, Norway
| | - Thormod Idsoe
- Department of Special Needs Education, Faculty of Educational Sciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| | - Ane Nærde
- Norwegian Center for Child Behavioral Development, Oslo, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Elm L, Lundeborg Hammarström I, Samuelsson C, Plejert C. Swedish speech and language pathologists reflect on how their clinical practises align to everyday language and communication skills of children with developmental language disorder. LOGOP PHONIATR VOCO 2024:1-9. [PMID: 38949090 DOI: 10.1080/14015439.2024.2371284] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/28/2023] [Accepted: 06/18/2024] [Indexed: 07/02/2024]
Abstract
This study aims to extend current knowledge about the possibilities and challenges encountered by Swedish speech and language pathologists (SLPs) in targeting everyday language and communication in children with developmental language disorder (DLD). To explore this matter, unstructured focus groups were conducted where 15 SLPs, working with children with DLD, shared their views on the alignment between their clinical practices and children's everyday lives. Thematic analysis was used to analyse the data, which resulted in five themes: It's everyday life that matters; As an SLP, you're not a part of the child's everyday life; How do we merge the different worlds?; Resources at home vary, and The employer sets the framework for clinical practices. The SLPs stressed the importance of targeting everyday skills and needs, but they experienced themselves as being detached from the children's daily context. Collaboration with caregivers and (pre)school staff was emphasised; however, the resources and capacity of the caregivers and staff varied, and this was experienced as a challenge for providing the most appropriate care. Some children and their families were situated in a multifaceted context and needed more extensive care, and this group was described as increasing. However, the services that the SLPs were able to offer varied and were largely regulated by organisational constraints. Individualised services are crucial for ensuring a positive development for children with DLD and for empowering caregivers to be effective collaborative partners in intervention. Therefore, it is essential for SLPs to have the time and resources to ensure high-quality care.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lovisa Elm
- Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
| | | | - Christina Samuelsson
- Department of Clinical Science, Intervention and Technology (CLINTEC), Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Charlotta Plejert
- Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Williams P, Slonims V, Weinman J. 'Turning up and tuning in'. Factors associated with parental non-attendance and non-adherence in intervention for young children with speech, language communication needs. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2024; 59:762-778. [PMID: 37824327 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12961] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2023] [Accepted: 09/18/2023] [Indexed: 10/14/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND When parents bring their child to appointments and then adhere to agreed speech and language therapy (SLT) recommendations, there is the potential to increase the intensity of the intervention, support generalization and improve outcomes. In SLT, however, little is known about factors that may promote attendance or adherence. Studies in other clinical areas such in medicine, psychology and physiotherapy have identified risk factors for non-attendance or non-adherence that are multifactorial and variable dependent on, for example, population and intervention. AIMS To identify rates of non-attendance and non-adherence, and to identify parent or child factors associated with parent involvement in intervention for children under 5 years of age receiving SLT. METHODS Parents completed questionnaires at two time points assessing the domains of parents' beliefs (problem perceptions, self-efficacy), personal circumstances (socio-demographics, family functioning), treatment experience and child factors. Predictors of parent attendance and adherence were identified through multiple regression analyses. Non-attendance rates were identified via local health records and non-adherence ascertained using a specific parent-reported measure within the treatment experience domain. RESULTS Participants (N = 199) were predominantly mothers, and were ethnically and socio-economically diverse, speaking a wide range of languages. Their children presented with a range of speech, language communication needs (SLCN). The rate of non-attendance was 25% and the main predictors of non-attendance were maternal age, education level and two factors within the parent beliefs domain. This model explained 40% of the variance in attendance. The rate of non-adherence in this cohort was 26% with parental rating of the importance of a recommendation and self-efficacy beliefs predicting adherence; this explained 56% of the variance in adherence to SLT recommendations at home. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS Our research has provided preliminary evidence of the influence of parents' beliefs, personal circumstances and treatment experiences on their involvement in their child's therapy. Speech and language therapists should consider factors impacting attendance and adherence to treatment and explore parental perceptions of their child's SLCN before embarking on an intervention, a foundation for collaborative practice. A possible limitation of this study is that the levels of attrition in our sample led to generally high measured rates of participation, which should be considered in future studies. Future research should explore adherence in treatments with varying doses, with different types of SLCN or interventions and in different settings. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS What is already known on the subject It is acknowledged that parent involvement in their child's therapy, such as attending and adhering to recommendations, is important but little is known about the rates of involvement and what factors may be associated with attendance and adherence in SLT. Qualitative research has explored parental involvement suggesting that beliefs about an intervention may be pertinent. Extensive research in other clinical areas suggest multiple and varied factors are influential and further exploration of particular populations and interventions is necessary. What this paper adds to the existing knowledge This study identified rates of parental non-attendance and non-adherence in a cohort of predominantly mothers of children under the age of 5 years. It is the first study to measure parent adherence in SLT and identify factors that are associated parental adherence to SLT recommendations. It adds to the small body of SLT specific research in understanding risk factors for non-attendance. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? This study highlights the need for a speech and language therapist to consider and explore parents' perspectives of their child's SLCN as a part of achieving collaboration with a parent in order to achieve the best outcomes. It provides a foundation for further systematic research into parent involvement with the ultimate aim of enhancing outcomes for children with SLCN.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Penny Williams
- Children's neurosciences, Evelina London Children's Hospital, London, UK
- Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
| | - Vicky Slonims
- Children's neurosciences, Evelina London Children's Hospital, London, UK
- Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
| | - John Weinman
- School of Cancer & Pharmaceutical Sciences, King's College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Dahlén J, Drevenhorn E, Kalnak N. Assessment fidelity of a language screening instrument for 4-year-olds. LOGOP PHONIATR VOCO 2023; 48:189-196. [PMID: 35649166 DOI: 10.1080/14015439.2022.2081874] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/18/2021] [Accepted: 05/20/2022] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
AIM The aim of the study was to explore the assessment fidelity of Språkfyran, a language screening instrument for four-year-old children. Språkfyran is a mandatory part of the healthcare program within the Swedish Child Health Service (CHS) and is offered to all four-year-olds in the region Scania in Sweden. METHODS The study was based on structured observations of twenty-four specialist CHS nurses' adherence to the Språkfyran protocol during screening. RESULTS All the observed nurses deviated from the test protocol. There was a large variation in the number of deviations from the test protocol per nurse, with the highest number of deviations occurring for three specific testing items. Significantly more deviations were made with four-year-old bilingual children as opposed to four-year-old monolingual children. Half of the nurses did not use the test protocol. CONCLUSIONS There is a clear need to improve the assessment fidelity of Språkfyran. Both the training that the nurses are offered, and the development of the test, are essential in securing the aim of high-quality work within the CHS. Support from experts in child speech-language development and disorders is suggested to be available at the CHS in Sweden.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Johanna Dahlén
- Public Health Nurse, Department of Health Sciences, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | - Eva Drevenhorn
- Public Health Nurse, Department of Health Sciences, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | - Nelli Kalnak
- Department of Women and Children's Health, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Pouscoulous N, Perovic A. Linguistic Skills and Socioeconomic Status: Two Oft Forgotten Factors in Child Metaphor Comprehension. CHILDREN (BASEL, SWITZERLAND) 2023; 10:1847. [PMID: 38136049 PMCID: PMC10741720 DOI: 10.3390/children10121847] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2023] [Revised: 11/10/2023] [Accepted: 11/13/2023] [Indexed: 12/24/2023]
Abstract
Metaphor understanding can be tricky for children until mid-childhood, yet some research suggests that pre-schoolers are already competent. Many factors have been proposed to play a role in the development of metaphor comprehension. In this study we focus on two obvious contenders that have been overlooked in recent years: general language skills and socioeconomic status (SES). Two-hundred and seventy-two children, aged from 2;11 to 11;04 (146 girls) were recruited from 21 British schools and nurseries. Their SES was established using a composite measure linked to school location, while general language skills were assessed using a standardised measure of vocabulary comprehension. Novel metaphor comprehension was tested with a simple reference assignment task. Our study confirms that children interpret novel metaphors confidently from the age of 4. Our findings indicate that novel metaphor understanding is associated with age and, importantly, that it is linked to vocabulary skills, as well as SES, but not gender. These two factors should therefore be considered in future research on metaphor development, as well as intervention and education.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Alexandra Perovic
- Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, Chandler House, 2 Wakefield Street, London WC1N 1PF, UK;
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Levickis P, Eadie P, Mensah F, McKean C, Bavin EL, Reilly S. Associations between responsive parental behaviours in infancy and toddlerhood, and language outcomes at age 7 years in a population-based sample. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2023; 58:1098-1112. [PMID: 36726027 PMCID: PMC10946965 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12846] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/24/2022] [Accepted: 12/06/2022] [Indexed: 06/18/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND A wealth of evidence supports the important role high-quality parent-child interactions play in children's early language acquisition. However, the impact on later language outcomes remains unclear. AIMS To examine the associations between responsive parental behaviours across the early years and child language outcomes at age 7 years with families from an Australian longitudinal cohort study (N = 1148, 50% female). METHODS & PROCEDURES At child ages 12, 24 and 36 months, parents completed a self-report measure of responsive parental behaviours. Child language was directly assessed at age 7 using the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, 4th edition (CELF-4), Australian Standardisation. Linear regression was used to examine associations between responsive parental behaviours from 12 to 36 months (consistently high, inconsistent and consistently low responsive parental behaviours at the three time points) and language scores at age 7 years. Adjusted models were run, including the following potential confounders: child sex; birth weight; birth order; maternal education; socio-economic disadvantage; non-English-speaking background; family history of speech-language problems; mother's vocabulary score; maternal mental health score; and mother's age at birth of child. A final adjusted model was run, including the potential confounder variables as well as adjusting for children's earlier language skills. OUTCOMES & RESULTS Linear regression results showed children with parents who rated high on responsive parental behaviours at all three time points had higher mean language scores at age 7 than children whose parents reported low responsive parental behaviours across early childhood. This association attenuated after adjusting for earlier child language skills. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS Findings support the consistent use of responsive parental behaviours across the very early years of childhood to support long-term language outcomes. Findings also suggest that models of surveillance and support which monitor and assist families at multiple time-points over the early years are likely to be most effective for preventing ongoing language difficulties. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS What is already known on this subject There is extensive evidence consistently demonstrating the important contribution of aspects of parent-child interaction, specifically responsive parental behaviours, to children's language development. What this paper adds to the existing knowledge Understanding the cumulative benefit of responsive parent-child interactions across the very early years may help to inform preventive interventions and service delivery models for supporting young children's language development. This study demonstrates in a large, population-based cohort the contribution of consistency of responsive parental behaviours during infancy and toddlerhood to school-age language outcomes, accounting for other child, family and environmental factors. Capturing regular parent behaviours via self-report during the early years may be a more efficient and less costly method than parent-child interaction observations to monitor the home language-learning environment during routine developmental checks. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? Findings support the need for surveillance of children and families in the early years, ensuring that intervention occurs when families need it most, that is, support is responsive to changing needs and that nuanced advice and support strategies are provided to activate positive developmental cascades. Capturing both parent behaviours and child language may assist clinicians to identify those families who may benefit from parent-child interaction intervention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Penny Levickis
- Melbourne Graduate School of EducationThe University of MelbourneMelbourne, VICAustralia
- GeneticsMurdoch Children's Research InstituteMelbourne, VICAustralia
| | - Patricia Eadie
- Melbourne Graduate School of EducationThe University of MelbourneMelbourne, VICAustralia
| | - Fiona Mensah
- Intergenerational HealthMurdoch Children's Research InstituteParkvilleVICAustralia
- Department of PaediatricsUniversity of MelbourneParkvilleVICAustralia
| | - Cristina McKean
- School of Education, Communication & Language SciencesNewcastle UniversityNewcastle upon TyneUK
| | - Edith L. Bavin
- Intergenerational HealthMurdoch Children's Research InstituteParkvilleVICAustralia
- School of Psychology and Public HealthLa Trobe UniversityMelbourneVICAustralia
| | - Sheena Reilly
- GeneticsMurdoch Children's Research InstituteMelbourne, VICAustralia
- Health GroupGriffith UniversityGold CoastQLDAustralia
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Auza B. A, Murata C, Peñaloza C. Predictive validity of a parental questionnaire for identifying children with developmental language disorders. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1110449. [PMID: 37404588 PMCID: PMC10316708 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1110449] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2022] [Accepted: 05/30/2023] [Indexed: 07/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Background The underdiagnosis of developmental language disorder (DLD) in children is a serious problem in developing countries with limited resources. It has long been noted that the concerns parents have about their children's health and development are richly informative, and if this information can be used for diagnosis, it may provide a means to address the problem of underdiagnosis of DLD. This study aimed to quantify the utility of parental linguistic concern questions (PLCQ) on the identification of language disorders in monolingual Spanish-speaking children in Mexico. It also explored whether a combination of biological and environmental conditions questions (BECQ) might improve the performance of a screening test to identify DLD. Methods A total of 680 monolingual Mexican Spanish-speaking children and their parents from urban areas in Mexico participated in the study. The distribution of responses to questions about DLD concerns was compared between 185 children diagnosed with DLD and 495 control subjects, and multiple logistic regression analysis was performed to select questions with high predictivity, based on the Akaike information criterion. The diagnostic utility of the questions was assessed by receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves, stratum-specific likelihood ratios (SSLRs), and changes in pretest and post-test probabilities of DLD. A similar procedure was used to explore whether adding BECQ would improve the diagnostic utility of questions about DLD concerns using data of 128 children. Results Four questions regarding parental linguistic concerns were found to be useful in identifying children with DLD. When all four concerns were present, the SSLR was 8.79, while it was only 0.27 when there were no concerns at all. The estimates of DLD probability increased from 0.12 to 0.55 at pretest and post-test. On the other hand, the BECQ did not perform as well as the PLCQ in identifying DLD, and the improvement in diagnostic performance it provided was limited to one question. Conclusion The parental questionnaire can be used as a screening tool to help in identifying children with DLD. The data presented in this study underscore the importance of considering linguistic parental concerns as part of the screening process. This is a realistic option to provide a solution to the current problem of underdiagnosis of DLD in Mexico.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alejandra Auza B.
- Language and Cognition Laboratory, Hospital General Dr. Manuel Gea González, Mexico City, Mexico
| | - Chiharu Murata
- Departamento de Metodología de la Investigación, Instituto Nacional de Pediatría, Mexico City, Mexico
| | - Christian Peñaloza
- Departamento de Fonoaudiología, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
McKean C, Watson R, Charlton J, Roulstone S, Holme C, Gilroy V, Law J. ‘Making the most of together time’: development of a Health Visitor–led intervention to support children’s early language and communication development at the 2–2½-year-old review. Pilot Feasibility Stud 2022; 8:35. [PMID: 35135632 PMCID: PMC8822642 DOI: 10.1186/s40814-022-00978-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2021] [Accepted: 01/11/2022] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Early interventions to support young children’s language development through responsive parent–child interaction have proven efficacy but are not currently delivered universally. A potential universal delivery platform is the Health Visitor (HV)–led 2–2½-year-old review in England’s Healthy Child Programme. It is unclear if it is feasible to offer such interventions through this platform. We report an intervention development process, including extensive stakeholder consultation and co-design which aimed to develop an acceptable, feasible and equitable early language intervention for delivery in this context. Methods The study involved five phases including 13 stakeholder co-design workshops with 7 parents and 39 practitioners (HVs, early years practitioners and speech and language therapists): (1) Identification of existing intervention evidence, (2) qualitative review of intervention studies extracting candidate target behaviours for intervention and intervention techniques, (3) co-design workshops with parents and practitioners examining acceptability, barriers and enablers to those behaviours and techniques (particular attention was paid to diverse family circumstances and the range of barriers which might exist), (4) findings were analysed using COM-B and theoretical domains frameworks and a prototype intervention model designed, and (5) co-design workshops iteratively refined the proposed model. Results Practitioners were committed to offering language intervention at the 2–2½-year-old review but were not sure precisely how to do so. Parents/caregivers wanted to be proactive and to have agency in supporting their own children and to do this as soon as possible. For equitable intervention, it must be proportionate, with higher ‘intensity’ for higher levels of disadvantage, and tailored, offering differing approaches considering the specific barriers and enablers, assets and challenges in each family. The importance and potential fragility of alliances between parent/caregiver and practitioner were identified as key, and so, strategies to engender successful collaborative partnership are also embedded in intervention design. Conclusion It is possible to develop a universal intervention which parents and practitioners judge would be acceptable, feasible and equitable for use at the 2–2½-year review to promote children’s language development. The result is one of the most explicitly developed universal interventions to promote children’s language development. Further development and piloting is required to develop materials to support successful widespread implementation. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s40814-022-00978-5.
Collapse
|
12
|
Eadie P, Levickis P, McKean C, Westrupp E, Bavin EL, Ware RS, Gerner B, Reilly S. Developing Preschool Language Surveillance Models - Cumulative and Clustering Patterns of Early Life Factors in the Early Language in Victoria Study Cohort. Front Pediatr 2022; 10:826817. [PMID: 35186809 PMCID: PMC8854765 DOI: 10.3389/fped.2022.826817] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2021] [Accepted: 01/10/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Screening and surveillance of development are integral to ensuring effective early identification and intervention strategies for children with vulnerabilities. However, not all developmental skills have reliable screening processes, such as early language ability. METHOD We describe how a set of early life factors used in a large, prospective community cohort from Australia are associated with language abilities across the preschool years, and determine if either an accumulation of risk factors or a clustering of risk factors provide a feasible approach to surveillance of language development in preschool children. RESULTS There were 1,208 children with a 7-year language outcome. The accumulation of early life factors increased the likelihood of children having low language skills at 7-years. Over a third of children with typical language skills (36.6%) had ≤ two risks and half of the children with low language (50%) had six or more risks. As the number of factors increases the risk of having low language at 7-years increases, for example, children with six or more risks had 17 times greater risk, compared to those with ≤ two risks. Data collected from 1,910 children at 8- to 12-months were used in the latent class modeling. Four profile classes (or groups) were identified. The largest group was developmentally enabled with a supportive home learning environment (56.2%, n = 1,073). The second group was vulnerable, both developmentally and in their home learning environment (31.2%, n = 596); the third group was socially disadvantaged with a vulnerable home learning environment (7.4%, n = 142); the final group featured maternal mental health problems and vulnerable child socio-emotional adjustment (5.2%, n = 99). Compared to developmentally enabled children, the risk of low language at 7-years was greater for children in the three other groups. CONCLUSION The cumulative and cluster risk analyses demonstrate the potential to use developmental surveillance to identify children within the first years of life who are at risk of language difficulties. Importantly, parent-child interaction and the home learning environment emerged as a consistent cluster. We recommend they be adopted as the common focus for early intervention and universal language promotion programs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patricia Eadie
- Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
| | - Penny Levickis
- Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia.,Genetics, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
| | - Cristina McKean
- School of Education, Communication and Language Sciences, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Elizabeth Westrupp
- Deakin University, Centre for Social and Early Emotional Development, School of Psychology, Geelong, VIC, Australia.,Judith Lumley Centre, La Trobe University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
| | - Edith L Bavin
- Genetics, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, VIC, Australia.,School of Psychology and Public Health La Trobe University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
| | - Robert S Ware
- Menzies Health Institute Queensland, Griffith University, Gold Coast, QLD, Australia
| | - Bibi Gerner
- Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia.,Genetics, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
| | - Sheena Reilly
- Menzies Health Institute Queensland, Griffith University, Gold Coast, QLD, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Sullivan A, Moulton V, Fitzsimons E. The intergenerational transmission of language skill. THE BRITISH JOURNAL OF SOCIOLOGY 2021; 72:207-232. [PMID: 33595850 PMCID: PMC8653888 DOI: 10.1111/1468-4446.12780] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2019] [Revised: 07/14/2020] [Accepted: 07/27/2020] [Indexed: 05/21/2023]
Abstract
This paper examines the relationship between parents' and children's language skills for a nationally representative birth cohort born in the United Kingdom-the Millennium Cohort Study (MCS). We investigate both socioeconomic and ethnic differentials in children's vocabulary scores and the role of differences in parents' vocabulary scores in accounting for these. We find large vocabulary gaps between highly educated and less educated parents, and between ethnic groups. Nevertheless, socioeconomic and ethnic gaps in vocabulary scores are far wider among the parents than among their children. Parental vocabulary is a powerful mediator of inequalities in offspring's vocabulary scores at age 14, and also a powerful driver of change in language skills between the ages of five and 14. Once we account for parental vocabulary, no ethnic minority group of young people has a negative "vocabulary gap" compared to whites.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alice Sullivan
- Department of Social ScienceCentre for Longitudinal Studies (CLS)UCL Institute of EducationLondonUK
| | - Vanessa Moulton
- Department of Social ScienceCentre for Longitudinal Studies (CLS)UCL Institute of EducationLondonUK
| | - Emla Fitzsimons
- Department of Social ScienceCentre for Longitudinal Studies (CLS)UCL Institute of EducationLondonUK
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Smith J, Levickis P, Neilson R, Mensah F, Goldfeld S, Bryson H. Prevalence of language and pre-literacy difficulties in an Australian cohort of 5-year-old children experiencing adversity. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2021; 56:389-401. [PMID: 33704873 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12611] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2020] [Revised: 01/14/2021] [Accepted: 01/28/2021] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Early reading success is predicated on language and pre-literacy skills. Children who are behind their peers in language and pre-literacy development before formal schooling are less likely to be proficient beginner readers, and difficulties may persist throughout primary school and beyond. We know children experiencing adversity are at greater risk of early language and pre-literacy difficulties; we do not know the prevalence of these difficulties in an Australian adversity context. AIMS To investigate the prevalence and co-occurrence of language and pre-literacy difficulties in a cohort of Australian 5-year-old children experiencing social adversity. METHODS & PROCEDURES Data were drawn from a large Australian community-based trial of nurse home visiting (right@home), which aimed to support women experiencing social adversity from pregnancy until their child turned 2 years of age. Social adversity was determined by two of more risk factors: young pregnancy, not living with another adult, no support, poorer health, current smoker, long-term illness, anxious mood, not finishing high school, no household income and no previous employment. Children whose mothers were enrolled in the control group (receiving usual maternal and child healthcare) were included in the current study (n = 359). Language and pre-literacy skills were measured at age 5 using the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamental Preschool-Second Edition (CELF-P2) and the School Entry Alphabetic and Phonological Awareness Readiness Test (SEAPART). Language difficulties were defined as ≥ 1.25 standard deviations (SD) below the Australian normative mean on CELF-P2 Core Language scores. Pre-literacy difficulties were defined as children scoring in the Beginner (versus Developing or Competent) criterion-referenced level on the SEAPART First Sound Identification and/or Alphabet Letter Identification subtests. Co-occurrence of language and pre-literacy difficulties was also determined. OUTCOMES & RESULTS At the 5-year follow-up, 201/359 (56%) children were assessed (mean age = 5.1 years, SD = 0.1). Mean Core Language score for this cohort (91.8, SD = 15.9) was 0.54 SD below the normative mean (100, SD = 15). The proportion of children presenting with language difficulties was 24.9%. Regarding pre-literacy skills, 43.8% of children were 'Beginner' for identifying first sounds and 58.6% for identifying alphabet letters/sounds. There was also considerable overlap whereby 76.7% of children with language difficulties also exhibited pre-literacy difficulties. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS This is the first empirical Australian-based study highlighting the high prevalence and co-occurrence of language and pre-literacy difficulties in preschool children experiencing social adversity. Clinicians should be aware of co-morbid language and pre-literacy difficulties in disadvantaged populations and consider both areas during assessment and intervention planning. What this paper adds What is already known on the subject The prevalence of language and literacy difficulties is substantially higher in cohorts experiencing social adversity when compared with more advantaged families. There is some evidence that adversity also contributes to pre-literacy difficulties, but less is known here. What this paper adds to existing knowledge This study presents new prevalence data showing high rates of language and pre-literacy difficulties for 5-year-old children experiencing adversity within an Australian context. It is the first to explore these skills in a large cohort of pre-schoolers recruited from community settings in Australia. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? In this cohort experiencing adversity, most children who presented with language difficulties likewise exhibited pre-literacy difficulties. This concordance reflects how early oral language and pre-literacy skills develop together. Clinicians should assess both skills in preschool populations-especially those working with children experiencing adversity-to ensure all children have strong foundations to become proficient beginner readers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jodie Smith
- School of Psychology and Public Health, La Trobe University, Bundoora, VIC, Australia
| | - Penny Levickis
- Melbourne Graduate School of Education, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia
- Population Health, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Parkville, VIC, Australia
| | - Roslyn Neilson
- Language, Speech and Literacy Services, Jamberoo, NSW, Australia
| | - Fiona Mensah
- Population Health, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Parkville, VIC, Australia
- Department of Paediatrics, University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia
| | - Sharon Goldfeld
- Population Health, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Parkville, VIC, Australia
- Department of Paediatrics, University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia
- Centre for Community Child Health, The Royal Children's Hospital, Parkville, VIC, Australia
| | - Hannah Bryson
- Population Health, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Parkville, VIC, Australia
- Centre for Community Child Health, The Royal Children's Hospital, Parkville, VIC, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Rowe A, Titterington J, Holmes J, Henry L, Taggart L. A classroom intervention targeting working memory, attention and language skills: a cluster randomised feasibility trial. Pilot Feasibility Stud 2021; 7:45. [PMID: 33549138 PMCID: PMC7866677 DOI: 10.1186/s40814-021-00771-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2020] [Accepted: 01/07/2021] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Background International debate around the best models of speech and language therapy provision for children with language disorders has highlighted the need for research into classroom-based approaches and intervention dosage. Working memory (WM) is a cognitive skill linked to attention and language. ‘Recall to Enhance Children’s Attention, Language and Learning’ (RECALL) is a novel, 6-week, classroom-based intervention delivered by health professionals (HPs) and teachers. It is designed to target WM and enhance attention and language skills in 4–5 year olds. Methods A cluster randomised feasibility trial was conducted to investigate aspects of the feasibility of a definitive trial to evaluate RECALL: (i) recruitment and sampling procedures; (ii) compliance and fidelity; (iii) the acceptability of RECALL to HPs and teachers; (iv) the appropriateness of the outcome measures. Six classes of 4–5 year olds participated: two received RECALL, two received an existing intervention targeting attention skills (not underpinned by WM theory), and two received education as usual (no intervention). Ten children in each class (n = 60) were sampled to assess the appropriateness of the outcome measures. Classroom observations were conducted to measure fidelity and semi-structured interviews with HPs, and teachers explored the acceptability of RECALL. Results The recruitment targets were met, and all six schools completed the trial, but the sampling procedures require modification. Compliance was good (95% of RECALL sessions were delivered), but fidelity to the intervention protocol varied between 76% and 45% across the two schools. This was influenced by large class sizes, child factors, and facilitator factors, e.g., their understanding of the theory underpinning the intervention. The lack of fidelity reduced the dose (number of practice items) accessed by individual children, particularly those most at risk. There were mixed findings regarding the acceptability of RECALL and the appropriateness of the outcome measures. Conclusions The trial protocol could be easily scaled-up in a future definitive trial, with an amended sampling procedure. RECALL should be repackaged as a small group intervention to enhance the fidelity of its delivery and its acceptability to HPs and teachers. This study highlights the need for thorough training for professionals who deliver classroom-based interventions for children with language disorders. Trial registration ISRCTN13633886. Registered on 7 September 2018 Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s40814-021-00771-w.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anita Rowe
- Institute of Nursing and Health Research, Ulster University, Shore Road, Newtownabbey, Co Antrim, BT37 0QB, Northern Ireland.
| | - Jill Titterington
- Institute of Nursing and Health Research, Ulster University, Shore Road, Newtownabbey, Co Antrim, BT37 0QB, Northern Ireland
| | - Joni Holmes
- MRC Cognition & Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge, CB2 7EF, England
| | - Lucy Henry
- Division of Language and Communication Science, City University of London, 10 Northampton Square, London, EC1V 0HB, England
| | - Laurence Taggart
- Institute of Nursing and Health Research, Ulster University, Shore Road, Newtownabbey, Co Antrim, BT37 0QB, Northern Ireland
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Levickis P, McKean C, Wiles A, Law J. Expectations and experiences of parents taking part in parent-child interaction programmes to promote child language: a qualitative interview study. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2020; 55:603-617. [PMID: 32525603 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12543] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2019] [Revised: 05/11/2020] [Accepted: 05/12/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Parent-child interaction therapies are commonly used by speech and language therapists (SLTs) when providing services to young children with language learning difficulties. However, the way parents react to the demands of such interventions is clearly important, especially for those from socially disadvantaged backgrounds. Parents play a central role in the therapy process so to ensure parent engagement, and to maximize intervention effectiveness, parents' views must be considered. AIMS To explore the expectations and experiences of parents from socially disadvantaged backgrounds who had taken part in a parent-child interaction programme aimed at promoting language development in 2-3 year olds with language difficulties. METHODS & PROCEDURES The sample included parents who had a child aged 2-3 years and had attended a parent-child interaction programme to promote their child's language development. Parents were eligible to take part if they were living in the 30% most deprived areas in a city in the North of England that constituted the study site. Ten parents participated in a qualitative semi-structured face-to-face interview in the home. Framework analysis was used to analyse the interview transcripts. OUTCOMES & RESULTS Parents' expectations before taking part in parent-child interaction interventions contribute to how they may engage throughout the intervention process. Barriers include parents' uncertainty about the nature of the intervention and differing attitudes regarding intervention approaches and strategies. Facilitators during the intervention process include gaining support from other parents, reassurance from the SLT regarding their child's language development, and their own ability to support their child's language learning, as well as increased confidence in how they support their child's development. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS Parents respond very differently to parent-child interaction intervention for children with language difficulties, depending on their expectations and attitudes towards intervention. Thus, it is critical that these different perspectives are understood by practitioners before intervention commences to ensure successful engagement. What this paper adds What is already known on this subject Parent-child interaction interventions are widely used to promote child language development. Parents play a central role in the therapy process of such interventions, so to maximize effectiveness, parents must be appropriately 'engaged' in that intervention. This involves attending, fully participating and having appropriate attitudinal and/or emotional involvement. The reciprocal nature of engagement means that parents are more likely to become engaged in intervention over time when they are supported by their SLT. What this paper adds to existing knowledge Parental expectations about the intervention process vary considerably and often need to be negotiated before the start of intervention. Reassurance and supporting positive attitudes to co-working with their SLT may be particularly important for families living with social disadvantage. Supporting parent engagement in parent-child interaction programmes can contribute to the parents' capability to continue implementing language-promoting strategies outside the intervention context and beyond the end of therapy. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? Parents have different expectations regarding programme involvement. Therefore, having a two-way, open dialogue between parents and SLTs from the beginning is clearly important, not only as a way of sharing information but also to build on parents' understanding of what the intervention will involve and trust that the SLT will be able to deliver the intervention in collaboration with the parent. SLTs can enhance parent engagement by supporting parents to feel confident and providing reassurance in terms of their child's development and how they can support their child's language learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Penny Levickis
- Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
- Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Parkville, VIC, Australia
- Melbourne Graduate School of Education, The University of Melbourne, VIC, Australia
| | - Cristina McKean
- Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
- Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Parkville, VIC, Australia
| | - Alex Wiles
- Newcastle upon Tyne Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
| | - James Law
- Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Noble C, Cameron-Faulkner T, Jessop A, Coates A, Sawyer H, Taylor-Ims R, Rowland CF. The Impact of Interactive Shared Book Reading on Children's Language Skills: A Randomized Controlled Trial. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2020; 63:1878-1897. [PMID: 32539588 DOI: 10.1044/2020_jslhr-19-00288] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Purpose Research has indicated that interactive shared book reading can support a wide range of early language skills and that children who are read to regularly in the early years learn language faster, enter school with a larger vocabulary, and become more successful readers at school. Despite the large volume of research suggesting interactive shared reading is beneficial for language development, two fundamental issues remain outstanding: whether shared book reading interventions are equally effective (a) for children from all socioeconomic backgrounds and (b) for a range of language skills. Method To address these issues, we conducted a randomized controlled trial to investigate the effects of two 6-week interactive shared reading interventions on a range of language skills in children across the socioeconomic spectrum. One hundred and fifty children aged between 2;6 and 3;0 (years;months) were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: a pause reading, a dialogic reading, or an active shared reading control condition. Results The findings indicated that the interventions were effective at changing caregiver reading behaviors. However, the interventions did not boost children's language skills over and above the effect of an active reading control condition. There were also no effects of socioeconomic status. Conclusion This randomized controlled trial showed that caregivers from all socioeconomic backgrounds successfully adopted an interactive shared reading style. However, while the interventions were effective at increasing caregivers' use of interactive shared book reading behaviors, this did not have a significant impact on the children's language skills. The findings are discussed in terms of practical implications and future research. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.12420539.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claire Noble
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Liverpool, United Kingdom
| | - Thea Cameron-Faulkner
- School of Arts, Languages and Cultures, The University of Manchester, United Kingdom
| | - Andrew Jessop
- Language Development Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | - Anna Coates
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Liverpool, United Kingdom
| | - Hannah Sawyer
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Liverpool, United Kingdom
| | - Rachel Taylor-Ims
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Liverpool, United Kingdom
| | - Caroline F Rowland
- Language Development Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
James K, Munro N, Togher L, Cordier R. The Spoken Language and Social Communication Characteristics of Adolescents in Behavioral Schools: A Controlled Comparison Study. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 2020; 51:115-127. [PMID: 31603367 DOI: 10.1044/2019_lshss-18-0090] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose Working with students with emotional behavioral disorders is a challenging area of speech-language pathology practice. In this study, we compare and profile the narrative discourse, structural language, and social communication characteristics of adolescents attending behavioral support and mainstream schools. We also examine relationships between narratives, structural language, social communication, and behavior. Method Fifty-four students aged between 12 and 16 years participated. Twenty-seven students were from 3 Australian government Schools for Specific Purposes for students with behavioral difficulties, and 27 typically developing students were from a mainstream, government school. Students were matched for age and closely matched for sex and socioeconomic status. All students completed 3 communication assessments: oral narrative, structural language, and social communication skills. Teachers were asked to complete 2 behavioral questionnaires. Results Students in behavioral schools had significant difficulties generating narratives. Their structural language and overall social communication skills were also significantly poorer than their mainstream peers. One third of the behavioral group experienced significant difficulties across all 3 of these communication areas. Externalizing behavior was significantly related to narrative, structural language, and social communication only when the data were pooled across both groups. Conclusions Language and social communication difficulties were evident in adolescents who attend alternative school settings-behavioral schools. These findings confirm the need for speech-language pathologists to be engaged in specialist behavioral schools and provide impetus to service providers, speech-language pathologists, and educators to address relationships between behavior and communication ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karen James
- Discipline of Speech Pathology, Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
| | - Natalie Munro
- Discipline of Speech Pathology, Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney, New South Wales, Australia.,School of Occupational Therapy, Social Work and Speech Pathology, Curtin University, Perth, Western Australia, Australia
| | - Leanne Togher
- Discipline of Speech Pathology, Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
| | - Reinie Cordier
- School of Occupational Therapy, Social Work and Speech Pathology, Curtin University, Perth, Western Australia, Australia.,Faculty of Educational Sciences, Department of Special Needs Education, The University of Oslo, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Law J, Clegg J, Rush R, Roulstone S, Peters TJ. Association of proximal elements of social disadvantage with children's language development at 2 years: an analysis of data from the Children in Focus (CiF) sample from the ALSPAC birth cohort. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2019; 54:362-376. [PMID: 30479068 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12442] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/19/2017] [Revised: 10/01/2018] [Accepted: 10/23/2018] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND An association between social disadvantage and early language development is commonly reported in the literature, but less attention has been paid to the way that different aspects of social disadvantage affect both expressive and receptive language in the first 2 years of life. AIMS To examine the contributions of gender, parental report of early language skills and proximal social variables (the amount of stimulation in the home, the resources available to the child and the attitudes/emotional status of the primary carer and the support available to him/her) controlling for distal social variables (family income and maternal education) to children's expressive and receptive language development at 2 years in a community ascertained population cohort. METHODS & PROCEDURES Data from 1314 children in the Children in Focus (CiF) sample from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) were analyzed. Multivariable regression models identified the contribution of proximal (what parents do with their children) measures of social disadvantage adjusting for more distal (e.g., family income and material wealth) measures as well as early language development at 15 months to the development of verbal comprehension, expressive vocabulary and expressive grammar (word combinations) at 2 years of age. OUTCOME & RESULTS In the final multivariable models gender, earlier language and proximal social factors, co-varying for distal factors predicted 36% of the variance for expressive vocabulary, 22% for receptive language and 27% for word combinations at 2 years. Language development at 15 months remained a significant predictor of outcomes at 24 months. Environmental factors were associated with both expressive scales but the picture was rather more mixed for receptive language suggesting that there may be different mechanisms underlying the different processes. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS This study supports the argument that social advantage makes a strong contribution to children's language development in the early years. The results suggest that what parents/carers do with their children is critical even when structural aspects of social disadvantage such as family income and housing have been taken into consideration although this relationship varies for different aspects of language. This has the potential to inform the targeting of public health interventions focusing on early language and pre-literacy skills on the one hand and home learning environments on the other and, potentially, the two in combination.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James Law
- Institute of Health and Society, School of Education, Communication and Language Sciences, Newcastle University, UK
| | - Judy Clegg
- Department of Human Communication Sciences, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK
| | | | - Sue Roulstone
- Faculty of Health and Applied Sciences, University of the West of England, Bristol, UK
| | - Tim J Peters
- School of Clinical Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Sedgwick A, Stothard J. An exploration of educational psychologists’ perceptions, knowledge and practices regarding speech, language and communication needs. EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY IN PRACTICE 2019. [DOI: 10.1080/02667363.2019.1598939] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Adrienne Sedgwick
- School of Environment, Education and Development, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
| | - Jan Stothard
- School of Environment, Education and Development, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Affiliation(s)
- James Law
- Professor of Speech and Language Sciences
| | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Smith J, Levickis P, Eadie T, Bretherton L, Conway L, Goldfeld S. Concurrent associations between maternal behaviours and infant communication within a cohort of women and their infants experiencing adversity. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2018; 20:516-527. [PMID: 28682122 DOI: 10.1080/17549507.2017.1329458] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/18/2016] [Revised: 04/06/2017] [Accepted: 05/08/2017] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Evidence suggests that children living in adversity are at greater risk of poorer language than their peers with the quality of parental interactions potentially mediating this association. Studies typically measure the mediatory impact of generic interaction styles making it difficult to discern which particular aspects of the interaction are facilitating language. This study aims to bridge this gap by identifying specific maternal behaviours associated with concurrent infant communication, in a cohort of 12-month old infants and their mothers experiencing adversity. METHOD A total of 249 mother-infant free-play videos were collected from women experiencing adversity in Victoria and Tasmania, Australia. From those videos, specific maternal behaviours, infant communication acts and the interaction quality were coded. RESULT Maternal verbal imitations uniquely predicted concurrent use of infant vocalisations, total words and unique words. Furthermore, the more fluent and connected the mother-infant dyad, the stronger the association between imitations and all three infant measures. CONCLUSION Frequent use of maternal imitations, within highly connected mother-infant dyads, may help mediate the impact of adversity on early communication. This information is important for early years professionals working with at-risk populations in augmenting current knowledge of risk and protective factors related to early language.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jodie Smith
- a Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, The Royal Children's Hospital , Melbourne , Australia
- b Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of Melbourne , Melbourne , Australia , and
| | - Penny Levickis
- a Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, The Royal Children's Hospital , Melbourne , Australia
| | - Tricia Eadie
- b Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of Melbourne , Melbourne , Australia , and
| | - Lesley Bretherton
- a Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, The Royal Children's Hospital , Melbourne , Australia
- b Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of Melbourne , Melbourne , Australia , and
- c Royal Children's Hospital , Melbourne , Australia
| | - Laura Conway
- a Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, The Royal Children's Hospital , Melbourne , Australia
| | - Sharon Goldfeld
- a Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, The Royal Children's Hospital , Melbourne , Australia
- b Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of Melbourne , Melbourne , Australia , and
- c Royal Children's Hospital , Melbourne , Australia
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Dockrell JE, Hurry J. The identification of speech and language problems in elementary school: Diagnosis and co-occurring needs. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2018; 81:52-64. [PMID: 29724642 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.04.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/20/2017] [Revised: 04/08/2018] [Accepted: 04/09/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Oral language skills are the foundation for success at school and in employment. A significant minority of children experience difficulties in the acquisition of oral language resulting in speech and language needs (SLN). There are disjunctures between clinical studies using standardised assessment and educational studies. The current study examines teacher reported SLN alongside assessments of language and cognitive skills to explore children's profiles of needs, developmental trajectories and risk factors. PROCEDURE Data from the UK Millennium Cohort Study were used to examine teacher identification of SLN at seven (n = 8658) and 11 years (n = 7275). RESULTS There were high levels of co-occurrence between SLN and other special educational needs at seven and 11 years, with SLN being less common at 11. Vocabulary levels and parental concerns at three and five and educational attainment at seven were highly predictive of SLN at seven, slightly less so at 11. However, a significant proportion of parents of children who scored in the bottom 2nd centile on vocabulary measures did not report their child as experiencing a language problem. Gender and disadvantage were also predictive of SLN but were mediated by the cognitive and behavioural variables. IMPLICATIONS These results raise questions about whether children's language needs at age 11 are recognised in schools. The extent of co-occurrence challenges the way diagnostic categories should be used and supports the value of profiling of dimensions of need.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julie E Dockrell
- Psychology and Human Development, UCL Institute of Education, 20 Bedford Way, London WC1H 0AL, United Kingdom.
| | - Jane Hurry
- Psychology and Human Development, UCL Institute of Education, 20 Bedford Way, London WC1H 0AL, United Kingdom.
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Goldfeld S, Snow P, Eadie P, Munro J, Gold L, Le HND, Orsini F, Shingles B, Lee K, Connell J, Watts A. Classroom Promotion of Oral Language (CPOL): protocol for a cluster randomised controlled trial of a school-based intervention to improve children's literacy outcomes at grade 3, oral language and mental health. BMJ Open 2017; 7:e016574. [PMID: 29162571 PMCID: PMC5719328 DOI: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-016574] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Oral language and literacy competence are major influences on children's developmental pathways and life success. Children who do not develop the necessary language and literacy skills in the early years of school then go on to face long-term difficulties. Improving teacher effectiveness may be a critical step in lifting oral language and literacy outcomes. The Classroom Promotion of Oral Language trial aims to determine whether a specifically designed teacher professional learning programme focusing on promoting oral language can lead to improved teacher knowledge and practice, and advance outcomes in oral language and literacy for early years school children, compared with usual practice. METHODS AND ANALYSIS This is a two-arm cluster multisite randomised controlled trial conducted within Catholic and Government primary schools across Victoria, Australia. The intervention comprises 4 days of face-to-face professional learning for teachers and ongoing implementation support via a specific worker. The primary outcome is reading ability of the students at grade 3, and the secondary outcomes are teacher knowledge and practice, student mental health, reading comprehension and language ability at grade 1; and literacy, writing and numeracy at grade 3. Economic evaluation will compare the incremental costs of the intervention to the measured primary and secondary outcomes. ETHICS AND DISSEMINATION This trial was approved by the Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee #CF13/2634-2013001403 and later transferred to the University of Melbourne #1545540. The investigators (including Government and Catholic partners) will communicate trial results to stakeholders, collaborators and participating schools and teachers via appropriate presentations and publications. TRIAL REGISTRATION NUMBER ISRCTN77681972; Pre-results.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sharon Goldfeld
- Centre for Community Child Health, The Royal Children’s Hospital, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
- Department of Paediatrics, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
| | - Pamela Snow
- La Trobe Rural Health School, La Trobe University, Bendigo, Victoria, Australia
| | - Patricia Eadie
- Melbourne Graduate School of Education, The University of Melbourne Parkville, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
| | - John Munro
- Melbourne Graduate School of Education, The University of Melbourne Parkville, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
| | - Lisa Gold
- Deakin University, Geelong, Victoria, Australia
| | - Ha N D Le
- Deakin University, Geelong, Victoria, Australia
| | - Francesca Orsini
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
| | - Beth Shingles
- Department of Paediatrics, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
| | - Katherine Lee
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
- Department of Paediatrics, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
| | - Judy Connell
- Catholic Education Melbourne, East Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| | - Amy Watts
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Spencer S, Clegg J, Lowe H, Stackhouse J. Increasing adolescents' depth of understanding of cross-curriculum words: an intervention study. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2017; 52:652-668. [PMID: 28421646 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12309] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2016] [Accepted: 11/12/2016] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND There is some evidence that vocabulary intervention is effective for children, although further research is needed to confirm the impact of intervention within contexts of social disadvantage. Very little is known about the effectiveness of interventions to increase adolescent knowledge of cross-curriculum words. AIMS To evaluate the effectiveness of an intervention programme designed to develop adolescents' knowledge of cross-curriculum words. METHODS & PROCEDURES Participants were 35 adolescents aged between 12 and 14 years who were at risk of educational underachievement with low scores on a range of assessments. Participants received a 10-week intervention programme in small groups, targeting 10 cross-curriculum words (e.g., 'summarize'). This was evaluated using a bespoke outcome measure (the Word Knowledge Profile). The study involved an AABA design, with a repeated baseline, delayed intervention cohort and blind assessment. Intervention included both semantic and phonological information about the target words and involved the adolescents using the words in multiple contexts. OUTCOMES & RESULTS Results were promising and participants' knowledge of the targeted words significantly increased following intervention. Progress was demonstrated on the Word Knowledge Profile on the item requiring participants to define the word (for the summer intervention group only). This increase in depth of knowledge was seen on taught words but not on matched non-taught words. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS Cross-curriculum words are not consistently understood by adolescents at risk of low educational attainment within a low socio-economic context. A 10-week intervention programme resulted in some increases to the depth of knowledge of targeted cross-curriculum words.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah Spencer
- Department of Human Communication Sciences, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK
| | - Judy Clegg
- Department of Human Communication Sciences, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK
| | | | - Joy Stackhouse
- Department of Human Communication Sciences, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Smith J, Levickis P, Eadie T, Bretherton L, Conway L, Goldfeld S. Associations between Maternal Behaviors at 1 Year and Child Language at 2 Years in a Cohort of Women Experiencing Adversity. INFANCY 2017. [DOI: 10.1111/infa.12200] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Jodie Smith
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute
- University of Melbourne
| | | | - Tricia Eadie
- Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of Melbourne
| | - Lesley Bretherton
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute
- University of Melbourne
- The Royal Children's Hospital
| | - Laura Conway
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute
- University of Melbourne
| | - Sharon Goldfeld
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute
- The Royal Children's Hospital
- University of Melbourne
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Spencer S, Clegg J, Stackhouse J, Rush R. Contribution of spoken language and socio-economic background to adolescents' educational achievement at age 16 years. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2017; 52:184-196. [PMID: 27432281 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12264] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2014] [Revised: 03/31/2016] [Accepted: 04/11/2016] [Indexed: 05/15/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Well-documented associations exist between socio-economic background and language ability in early childhood, and between educational attainment and language ability in children with clinically referred language impairment. However, very little research has looked at the associations between language ability, educational attainment and socio-economic background during adolescence, particularly in populations without language impairment. AIMS To investigate: (1) whether adolescents with higher educational outcomes overall had higher language abilities; and (2) associations between adolescent language ability, socio-economic background and educational outcomes, specifically in relation to Mathematics, English Language and English Literature GCSE grade. METHOD & PROCEDURES A total of 151 participants completed five standardized language assessments measuring vocabulary, comprehension of sentences and spoken paragraphs, and narrative skills and one nonverbal assessment when between 13 and 14 years old. These data were compared with the participants' educational achievement obtained upon leaving secondary education (16 years old). Univariate logistic regressions were employed to identify those language assessments and demographic factors that were associated with achieving a targeted A* -C grade in English Language, English Literature and Mathematics General Certificate of Secondary Education (GCSE) at 16 years. Further logistic regressions were then conducted to examine further the contribution of socio-economic background and spoken language skills in the multivariate models. RESULTS & OUTCOMES Vocabulary, comprehension of sentences and spoken paragraphs, and mean length utterance in a narrative task along with socio-economic background contributed to whether participants achieved an A* -C grade in GCSE Mathematics and English Language and English Literature. Nonverbal ability contributed to English Language and Mathematics. The results of multivariate logistic regressions then found that vocabulary skills were particularly relevant to all three GCSE outcomes. Socio-economic background only remained important for English Language, once language assessment scores and demographic information were considered. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS Language ability, and in particular vocabulary, plays an important role for educational achievement. Results confirm a need for ongoing support for spoken language ability throughout secondary education and a potential role for speech and language therapy provision in the continuing drive to reduce the gap in educational attainment between groups from differing socio-economic backgrounds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah Spencer
- University of Sheffield, Department of Human Communication Sciences, Sheffield, UK
| | - Judy Clegg
- University of Sheffield, Department of Human Communication Sciences, Sheffield, UK
| | - Joy Stackhouse
- University of Sheffield, Department of Human Communication Sciences, Sheffield, UK
| | - Robert Rush
- Queen Margaret University, School of Health Sciences, Edinburgh, UK
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Malas K, Trudeau N, Giroux MC, Gauthier L, Poulin S, McFarland DH. Prior History of Feeding-Swallowing Difficulties in Children With Language Impairment. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2017; 26:138-145. [PMID: 28166549 DOI: 10.1044/2016_ajslp-15-0171] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/04/2015] [Accepted: 07/13/2016] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE This study updated and extended our previous investigation (Malas et al., 2015) of feeding-swallowing difficulties and concerns (FSCs) in children with language impairments (LI) by using more stringent inclusion criteria and targeting children earlier in the care delivery pathway. METHOD Retrospective analyses were performed on the clinical files of 29 children (average age: 60 months, SD = 9.0) diagnosed as having LI using standardized testing, nonstandardized testing and final speech-language pathologist judgment. The files of children born prematurely or with a history of anatomical, structural, neurodevelopmental, cognitive, sensory, motor, or speech disorders were excluded. Literature-based indicators were used to determine the prevalence of difficulties in sucking, food transition, food selectivity, and salivary control. Values were compared with the general population estimate of Lindberg et al. (1992). RESULTS A significantly higher percentage of histories of FSCs (48%) were found in the files of children with LI when compared with the population estimate (χ2 = 13.741, df = 1, p < .001). Difficulties in food transition (31%) and food selectivity (14%) were the most frequent. Data confirm and extend our previous findings and suggest that a previous history of FSCs may characterize children with LI early in their care delivery pathway.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathy Malas
- Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Sainte-Justine, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| | - Natacha Trudeau
- Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Sainte-Justine, Montréal, Québec, CanadaCentre de Recherche Interdisciplinaire en Réadaptation-Institut Raymond-Dewar, Montréal, Québec, CanadaUniversité de Montréal, Québec, Canada
| | | | | | | | - David H McFarland
- Université de Montréal, Québec, CanadaMcGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Reilly S, Harper M, Goldfeld S. The demand for speech pathology services for children: Do we need more or just different? J Paediatr Child Health 2016; 52:1057-1061. [PMID: 27662112 DOI: 10.1111/jpc.13318] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/26/2016] [Revised: 04/18/2016] [Accepted: 06/19/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
An inability or difficulty communicating can have a profound impact on a child's future ability to participate in society as a productive adult. Over the past few years the number of interventions for children with speech and language problems has almost doubled; the majority are targeted interventions delivered by speech pathologists. In this paper we examine the distribution of speech pathology services in metropolitan Melbourne and how these are aligned with need as defined by vulnerability in language and social disadvantage. We identified three times as many private sector services compared to public services for the 0-5 year age group. Overall there was poorer availability of services in some of the most vulnerable areas. The profound and long-term impact of impoverished childhood language, coupled with the considerable limitations on public spending, provide a strong impetus to deliver more equitably distributed speech pathology services.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sheena Reilly
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia.,Department of Paediatrics, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia.,Menzies Health Institute Queensland, Griffith University, Gold Coast, Queensland, Australia
| | - Megan Harper
- Victoria Department of Education and Training, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| | - Sharon Goldfeld
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia.,Department of Paediatrics, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia.,Centre for Community Child Health, Royal Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Chiat S, Polišenská K. A Framework for Crosslinguistic Nonword Repetition Tests: Effects of Bilingualism and Socioeconomic Status on Children's Performance. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2016; 59:1179-1189. [PMID: 27750281 DOI: 10.1044/2016_jslhr-l-15-0293] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/22/2015] [Accepted: 04/05/2016] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE As a recognized indicator of language impairment, nonword repetition has unique potential for distinguishing language impairment from difficulties due to limited experience and knowledge of a language. This study focused on a new Crosslinguistic Nonword Repetition framework, comprising 3 tests that vary the phonological characteristics of nonwords, in the quest for an assessment that minimizes effects of language experience and knowledge and thereby maximizes potential for assessing children with diverse linguistic experience. METHOD The English version of the new framework was administered, with a test of receptive vocabulary, to 4- to 7-year-old monolingual and bilingual children with typical development (n = 21 per group) from neighborhoods with midhigh and low socioeconomic status (SES). RESULTS Receptive vocabulary was affected by both bilingualism and neighborhood SES. In contrast, no effects of bilingualism or neighborhood SES were found on 2 of our nonword repetition tests, whereas the most language-specific test yielded a borderline effect of neighborhood SES but no effect of bilingualism. CONCLUSIONS The findings support the potential of the new tests for assessing children regardless of lingual or socioeconomic background. They also highlight the importance of considering the characteristics of nonword targets and investigating the compound influence of bilingualism and SES on different language assessments.
Collapse
|
31
|
Pring T. Inequalities in the provision of paediatric speech and language therapy services across London boroughs. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2016; 51:442-446. [PMID: 27018277 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12221] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/19/2015] [Accepted: 10/12/2015] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The inverse-care law suggests that fewer healthcare resources are available in deprived areas where health needs are greatest. AIMS To examine the provision of paediatric speech and language services across London boroughs and to relate provision to the level of deprivation of the boroughs. METHODS & PROCEDURES Information on the employment of paediatric speech and language therapists was obtained from London boroughs by freedom-of-information requests. The relationship between the number of therapists and the index of multiple deprivation for the borough was examined. OUTCOMES & RESULTS Twenty-nine of 32 boroughs responded. A positive relationship between provision and need was obtained, suggesting that the inverse-care law does not apply. However, large inequalities of provision were found particularly among the more socially deprived boroughs. In some instances boroughs had five times as many therapists per child as other boroughs. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS The data reveal that large differences in speech and language therapy provision exist across boroughs. The reasons for these inequalities are unclear, but the lack of comparative information across boroughs is likely to be unhelpful in planning equitable services. The use of freedom of information in assessing health inequalities is stressed and its future availability is desirable.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tim Pring
- Division of Language and Communication Science, City University London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Kornilov SA, Lebedeva TV, Zhukova MA, Prikhoda NA, Korotaeva IV, Koposov RA, Hart L, Reich J, Grigorenko EL. Language development in rural and urban Russian-speaking children with and without developmental language disorder. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2016; 46:45-53. [PMID: 27346924 DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.07.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Abstract
Using a newly developed Assessment of the Development of Russian Language (ORRIA), we investigated differences in language development between rural vs. urban Russian-speaking children (n = 100 with a mean age of 6.75) subdivided into groups with and without developmental language disorders. Using classical test theory and item response theory approaches, we found that while ORRIA displayed overall satisfactory psychometric properties, several of its items showed differential item functioning favoring rural children, and several others favoring urban children. After the removal of these items, rural children significantly underperformed on ORRIA compared to urban children. The urbanization factor did not significantly interact with language group. We discuss the latter finding in the context of the multiple additive risk factors for language development and emphasize the need for future studies of the mechanisms that underlie these influences and the implications of these findings for our understanding of the etiological architecture of children's language development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sergey A Kornilov
- Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA; Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT, USA; Saint-Petersburg State University, Saint-Petersburg, Russia; Moscow State University, Moscow, Russia
| | - Tatiana V Lebedeva
- City Center for Psychological, Medical, and Social Services, Moscow, Russia; Moscow City University for Psychology and Education, Moscow, Russia
| | | | | | | | | | | | - Jodi Reich
- Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA; Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Elena L Grigorenko
- Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA; Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT, USA; Saint-Petersburg State University, Saint-Petersburg, Russia; Moscow City University for Psychology and Education, Moscow, Russia
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Ryan A, Gibbon FE, O'shea A. Expressive and receptive language skills in preschool children from a socially disadvantaged area. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2016; 18:41-52. [PMID: 28425366 DOI: 10.3109/17549507.2015.1089935] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Evidence suggests that children present with receptive language skills that are equivalent to or more advanced than expressive language skills. This profile holds true for typical and delayed language development. This study aimed to determine if such a profile existed for preschool children from an area of social deprivation and to investigate if particular language skills influence any differences found between expressive and receptive skills. METHOD Data from 187 CELF P2 UK assessments conducted on preschool children from two socially disadvantaged areas in a city in southern Ireland. RESULT A significant difference was found between Receptive Language Index (RLI) and Expressive Language Index (ELI) scores with Receptive scores found to be lower than Expressive scores. The majority (78.6%) of participants had a lower Receptive Language than Expressive score (RLI < ELI), 18.2% of participants had a higher Receptive score than score (RLI > ELI), with very few (3.2%) having the same Receptive and Expressive scores (RLI = ELI). Scores for the Concepts and Following Directions (receptive) sub-test were significantly lower than for the other receptive sub tests, while scores for the Expressive Vocabulary sub-test were significantly higher than for the other expressive sub tests. CONCLUSION The finding of more advanced expressive than receptive language skills in socially deprived preschool children is previously unreported and clinically relevant for speech-language pathologists in identifying the needs of this population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ashling Ryan
- a University College Cork, Speech and Hearing Sciences, Brookfield Health Sciences Complex , Cork , Ireland , and
| | - Fiona E Gibbon
- a University College Cork, Speech and Hearing Sciences, Brookfield Health Sciences Complex , Cork , Ireland , and
| | - Aoife O'shea
- b Happy Talk Project, Cork City Partnership Ltd , Cork , Ireland
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Sciberras E, Westrupp EM, Wake M, Nicholson JM, Lucas N, Mensah F, Gold L, Reilly S. Healthcare costs associated with language difficulties up to 9 years of age: Australian population-based study. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2015; 17:41-52. [PMID: 24801409 DOI: 10.3109/17549507.2014.898095] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE This study aimed to quantify the non-hospital healthcare costs associated with language difficulties within two nationally representative samples of children. METHOD Data were from three biennial waves (2004-2008) of the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (B cohort: 0-5 years; K cohort: 4-9 years). Language difficulties were defined as scores ≤ 1.25 SD below the mean on measures of parent-reported communication (0-3 years) and directly assessed vocabulary (4-9 years). Participant data were linked to administrative data on non-hospital healthcare attendances and prescription medications from the universal Australian Medicare subsidized healthcare scheme. RESULT It was found that healthcare costs over each 2-year age band were higher for children with than without language difficulties at 0-1, 2-3, and 4-5 years, notably 36% higher (mean difference = $AU206, 95% CI = $90, $321) at 4-5 years (B cohort). The slightly higher 2-year healthcare costs for children with language difficulties at 6-7 and 8-9 years were not statistically different from those without language difficulties. Modelled to the corresponding Australian child population, 2-year government costs ranged from $AU1.2-$AU12.1 million (depending on age examined). Six-year healthcare costs increased with the persistence of language difficulties in the K cohort, with total Medicare costs increasing by $192 (95% CI = $74, $311; p = .002) for each additional wave of language difficulties. CONCLUSION Language difficulties (whether transient or persistent) were associated with substantial excess population healthcare costs in childhood, which are in addition to the known broader costs incurred through the education system. It is unclear whether healthcare costs were specifically due to the assessment and/or treatment of language difficulties, as opposed to conditions that may be co-morbid with or may cause language difficulties.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emma Sciberras
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute , Melbourne , Australia
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
McCartney E, Boyle J, Ellis S. Developing a universal reading comprehension intervention for mainstream primary schools within areas of social deprivation for children with and without language-learning impairment: a feasibility study. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2015; 50:129-135. [PMID: 25181284 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12124] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/02/2013] [Accepted: 07/07/2014] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Some children in areas of social deprivation in Scotland have lower reading attainment than neighbouring children in less deprived areas, and some of these also have lower spoken language comprehension skills than expected by assessment norms. There is a need to develop effective reading comprehension interventions that fit easily into the school curriculum and can benefit all pupils. A feasibility study of reading comprehension strategies with existing evidence of efficacy was undertaken in three mainstream primary schools within an area of social deprivation in west central Scotland, to decide whether further investigation of this intervention was warranted. AIMS Aims were to measure comprehension of spoken language and reading via standardised assessments towards the beginning of the school year (T1) in mainstream primary school classrooms within an area of social deprivation; to have teachers introduce previously-validated text comprehension strategies, and to measure change in reading comprehension outcome measures towards the end of the year (T2). METHODS & PROCEDURES A pre- and post-intervention cohort design was used. Reading comprehension strategies were introduced to staff in participating schools and used throughout the school year as part of on-going reading instruction. Spoken language comprehension was measured by TROG-2 at T1, and reading progress by score changes from T1 to T2 on the WIAT-II(UK) -T reading comprehension scale. OUTCOMES & RESULTS Forty-seven pupils in five classes in three primary schools took part: 38% had TROG-2 scores below the 10(th) centile. As a group, children made good reading comprehension progress, with a medium effect size of 0.46. Children with TROG-2 scores below the 10(th) centile had lower mean reading scores than others at T1 and T2, although with considerable overlap. However, TROG-2 did not make a unique contribution to reading progress: children below the 10(th) centile made as much progress as other children. The intervention was welcomed by schools, and the measure of reading comprehension proved responsive to change. CONCLUSIONS The outcomes suggest the reading intervention may be effective for children with and without spoken language comprehension difficulties, and warrants further investigation in larger, controlled, studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elspeth McCartney
- School of Psychological Sciences and Health, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, UK
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Bishop DVM. Ten questions about terminology for children with unexplained language problems. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2014; 49:381-415. [PMID: 25142090 PMCID: PMC4314704 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12101] [Citation(s) in RCA: 163] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/01/2013] [Accepted: 03/01/2013] [Indexed: 05/06/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND In domains other than language, there is fairly consistent diagnostic terminology to refer to children's developmental difficulties. For instance, the terms 'dyslexia', 'attention deficit hyperactivity disorder' and 'autistic spectrum disorder' are used for difficulties with reading, attention or social cognition, respectively. There is no agreed label, however, for children with unexplained language problems. AIMS To consider whether we need labels for unexplained language problems in children, and if so, what terminology is appropriate. MAIN CONTRIBUTION There are both advantages and disadvantages to labels, but they are important to ensure children receive services, and to increase our knowledge of the nature and causes of such problems. A survey of labels in current use found 132 different terms, 33 of which had 600 or more returns on Google Scholar between 1994 and 2013. Many of these labels were too general to be useful. Of the remainder, the term 'specific language impairment' was the most commonly used. CONCLUSIONS The current mayhem in diagnostic labels is unsustainable; it causes confusion and impedes research progress and access to appropriate services. We need to achieve consensus on diagnostic criteria and terminology. The DSM-5 term 'language disorder' is problematic because it identifies too wide a range of conditions on an internet search. One solution is to retain specific language impairment, with the understanding that 'specific' means idiopathic (i.e., of unknown origin) rather than implying there are no other problems beyond language. Other options are the terms 'primary language impairment', 'developmental language disorder' or 'language learning impairment'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D V M Bishop
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of OxfordOxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Early stimulation and language development of economically disadvantaged young children. Indian J Pediatr 2014; 81:333-8. [PMID: 23904067 DOI: 10.1007/s12098-013-1154-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2013] [Accepted: 06/19/2013] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To examine the effect of home stimulation on the language functioning of young children from low income families. METHODS The language functioning of 102 children (Mean age = 3.3 y, SD = 1.3) was assessed by the communication sub-scale of the Indian Developmental Inventory (IDI). Home visits were made to assess the quality of stimulation provided by parents to children. Seven items measuring stimulation of the child were selected from the Mohite Home Environment Inventory, a scale measuring the quality of home environment. RESULTS Nearly 16 % of children from economically disadvantaged homes had language delay. Children with language delay as compared to children with adequate language skills had significantly lower stimulation at home (t = 2.59, P = 0.01), specifically parents were significantly less likely to praise their child (25 % vs. 52 %, χ (2) = 4.03, P = 0.045) or provide verbal stimulation (44 % vs. 72 %, χ (2) = 4.95, P = 0.026). Multivariate stepwise regression analysis revealed that 18 % of the variance in the communication developmental quotient (DQs) of children was accounted by stimulation and age of the child (F = 10.47, P = 0.000). CONCLUSIONS Programs that seek to increase early stimulation for disadvantaged children by providing cognitive-linguistic enriched learning experiences would go a long way in paving the way for improved language, cognition and school performance in young children.
Collapse
|
38
|
Law J, Rush R, McBean K. The relative roles played by structural and pragmatic language skills in relation to behaviour in a population of primary school children from socially disadvantaged backgrounds. EMOTIONAL AND BEHAVIOURAL DIFFICULTIES 2013. [DOI: 10.1080/13632752.2013.854960] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
39
|
Camilleri B, Botting N. Beyond static assessment of children's receptive vocabulary: the dynamic assessment of word learning (DAWL). INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2013; 48:565-581. [PMID: 24033654 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Children's low scores on vocabulary tests are often erroneously interpreted as reflecting poor cognitive and/or language skills. It may be necessary to incorporate the measurement of word-learning ability in estimating children's lexical abilities. AIMS To explore the reliability and validity of the Dynamic Assessment of Word Learning (DAWL), a new dynamic assessment of receptive vocabulary. METHODS & PROCEDURES A dynamic assessment (DA) of word learning ability was developed and adopted within a nursery school setting with 15 children aged between 3;07 and 4;03, ten of whom had been referred to speech and language therapy. OUTCOMES & RESULTS A number of quantitative measures were derived from the DA procedure, including measures of children's ability to identify the targeted items and to generalize to a second exemplar, as well as measures of children's ability to retain the targeted items. Internal, inter-rater and test-retest reliability of the DAWL was established as well as correlational measures of concurrent and predictive validity. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS The DAWL was found to provide both quantitative and qualitative information which could be used to improve the accuracy of differential diagnosis and the understanding of processes underlying the child's performance. The latter can be used for the purpose of designing more individualized interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bernard Camilleri
- Division of Language and Communication Science, City University, London, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
40
|
Letts C, Edwards S, Sinka I, Schaefer B, Gibbons W. Socio-economic status and language acquisition: children's performance on the new Reynell Developmental Language Scales. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2013; 48:131-143. [PMID: 23472954 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Several studies in recent years have indicated a link between socio-economic status (SES) of families and children's language development, including studies that have measured children's language through formal standardized test procedures. High numbers of children with low performance have been found in lower socio-economic groups in some studies. This has proved a cause for concern for both clinicians and educationalists. AIMS To investigate the relationship between maternal education and postcode-related indicators of SES, and children's performance on the New Reynell Developmental Scales (NRDLS). METHODS & PROCEDURES Participants were 1266 children aged between 2;00 and 7;06 years who were recruited for the standardization of a new assessment procedure (NRDLS). Children were divided into four groups reflecting years of maternal education, and five groups reflecting SES Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) quintiles for the location of participating schools and nurseries. Groups were compared using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), with age as a covariate, in order to identify which might be affected by the two SES variables. Where relationships were found between SES and performance on the scales, individual children's standard scores were looked at to determine numbers potentially at risk for language delay. OUTCOMES & RESULTS An effect of years of maternal education on performance was found such that children whose mothers had minimum years performed less well than other children in the study, this effect being stronger for younger children. Children attending schools or nurseries in IMD quintile 1 areas performed less well in language production. Higher than expected numbers with language delay were found for younger children whose mothers had minimum years of education, and for children in quintile 1 schools and nurseries; however, numbers were not as high as noted in some other studies. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS Characteristics of the participant sample and measures used for language and SES may explain these results and are important considerations when interpreting results of studies or developing policies for intervention. The usefulness of commonly used categories of language delay is questioned.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carolyn Letts
- School of ECLS, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Joffe VL, Black E. Social, emotional, and behavioral functioning of secondary school students with low academic and language performance: perspectives from students, teachers, and parents. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 2012; 43:461-73. [PMID: 22826365 DOI: 10.1044/0161-1461(2012/11-0088)] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
PURPOSE Adolescence is a time of transition when young people with language difficulties are at increased risk of experiencing social, emotional, and behavioral difficulties (SEBD). Most studies of social, emotional, and behavioral functioning (SEBF) in individuals with language difficulties focus on children with a clinical diagnosis of language impairment. This study explores SEBF in a nonclinical group of 12-year-old students with low educational and language performance from their own perspectives and those of their parents and teachers. METHOD The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire ( Goodman, 1997) was given to 352 mainstream secondary school students who were underperforming academically and had poor language performance. Two hundred and twenty-five of their parents and 230 of their teachers also completed the questionnaire. RESULTS Students with low educational attainment and poor language showed significantly greater SEBD than a normative sample as reported by themselves, their parents, and their teachers. Significant differences were found across informants, with students identifying more overall difficulties than parents or teachers. CONCLUSION Secondary school students with low academic and language performance are more vulnerable to experiencing SEBD compared to typically developing peers. The extent of their difficulties varied depending on the informant, emphasizing the importance of gaining views from multiple perspectives.
Collapse
|